Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:57:10.615Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion understanding, parent mental state language, and behavior problems in internationally adopted children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 November 2015

Amanda R. Tarullo*
Affiliation:
Boston University
Adriana Youssef
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Kristin A. Frenn
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Kristen Wiik
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Melissa C. Garvin
Affiliation:
Sonoma State University
Megan R. Gunnar
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Amanda R. Tarullo, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, 64 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA 02215; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Internationally adopted postinstitutionalized (PI) children are at risk for lower levels of emotion understanding. This study examined how postadoption parenting influences emotion understanding and whether lower levels of emotion understanding are associated with behavior problems. Emotion understanding and parent mental state language were assessed in 3-year-old internationally adopted PI children (N = 25), and comparison groups of children internationally adopted from foster care (N = 25) and nonadopted (NA) children (N = 36). At 5.5-year follow-up, PI children had lower levels of emotion understanding than NA children, a group difference not explained by language. In the total sample, parent mental state language at age 3 years predicted 5.5-year emotion understanding after controlling for child language ability. The association of parent mental state language and 5.5-year emotion understanding was moderated by adoption status, such that parent mental state language predicted 5.5-year emotion understanding for the internationally adopted children, but not for the NA children. While postadoption experience does not erase negative effects of early deprivation on emotion understanding, results suggest that parents can promote emotion understanding development through mental state talk. At 5.5 years, PI children had more internalizing and externalizing problems than NA children, and these behavioral problems related to lower levels of emotion understanding.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bosacki, S. L., & Moore, C. (2004). Preschoolers’ understanding of simple and complex emotions: Links with gender and language. Sex Roles, 50, 659675. doi:10.1023/B:Sers.0000027568.26966.27 Google Scholar
Bruce, J., Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Disinhibited social behavior among internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 157171. doi:S0954579409000108[pii]10.1017/S0954579409000108 Google Scholar
Camras, L. A., Perlman, S. B., Fries, A. B., & Pollak, S. (2006). Post-institutionalized Chinese and Eastern European children: Heterogeneity in the development of emotion understanding. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 193199.Google Scholar
Croft, C., Beckett, C., Rutter, M., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., et al. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes of institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees: II. Language as a protective factor and a vulnerable outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 3144. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01689.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., et al. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence? Child Development, 74, 238256.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E. A., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development, 61, 11451152.Google Scholar
Desmarais, C., Roeber, B. J., Smith, M. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2012). Sentence comprehension in postinstitutionalized school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 4554. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0246) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2010). Maternal discussions of mental states and behaviors: Relations to emotion situation knowledge in European American and immigrant Chinese children. Child Development, 81, 14901503. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01487.x Google Scholar
Ensor, R., Devine, R. T., Marks, A., & Hughes, C. (2014). Mothers’ cognitive references to 2-year-olds predict theory of mind at ages 6 and 10. Child Development, 85, 12221235. doi:10.1111/cdev.12186 Google Scholar
Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Goldstein, L. H., Armstrong, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social, and academic difficulties in middle childhood: II. Developing the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 588603. doi:10.1097/00004583-200205000-00017 Google Scholar
Feldman, H. M., Dale, P. S., Campbell, T. F., Colborn, D. K., Kurs-Lasky, M., Rockette, H. E., et al. (2005). Concurrent and predictive validity of parent reports of child language at ages 2 and 3 years. Child Development, 76, 856868. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00882.x Google Scholar
Fries, A. B., & Pollak, S. D. (2004). Emotion understanding in postinstitutionalized Eastern European children. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 355369.Google Scholar
Garvin, M. C., Tarullo, A. R., Van Ryzin, M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2012). Postadoption parenting and socioemotional development in postinstitutionalized children. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 3548. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000642 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavazzi, I. G., & Ornaghi, V. (2011). Emotional state talk and emotion understanding: A training study with preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 38, 11241139. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000772 Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Bruce, J., & Grotevant, H. D. (2000). International adoption of institutionally reared children: Research and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 677693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., & van Dulmen, M. H. (2007). Behavior problems in postinstitutionalized internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 129148. doi:10.1017/S0954579407070071 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L., Donnelly, K., Guz, G. R., & Pitt-Watson, R. (1986). Children's understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Development, 57, 895909.Google Scholar
Hawk, B., & McCall, R. B. (2010). CBCL behavior problems of post-institutionalized international adoptees. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13, 199211. doi:10.1007/s10567-010-0068-x Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hoksbergen, R., Rijk, K., Van Dijkum, C., & Ter Laak, J. (2004). Adoption of Romanian children in The Netherlands: Behavior problems and parenting burden of upbringing for adoptive parents. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25, 175180. doi:10.1097/00004703-200406000-00006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12, 1823.Google Scholar
Jeon, H., Moulson, M. C., Fox, N., Zeanah, C., & Nelson, C. (2010). The effects of early institutionalization on the discrimination of facial expressions of emotion in young children. Infancy, 15, 209221. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00007.x Google Scholar
Johnson, D. E. (2000). Medical and developmental sequelae of early childhood institutionalization in Eastern European adoptees. In Nelson, C. A. (Ed.), The Minnesota symposia on child psychology: Vol. 31. The effects of early adversity on neurobehavioral development (pp. 113162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Judge, S. (2004). The impact of early institutionalization on child and family outcomes. Adoption Quarterly, 7, 3148. doi:10.1300/J145v07n03_02 Google Scholar
Juffer, F., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Behavior problems and mental health referrals of international adoptees: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 25012515. doi:10.1001/jama.293.20.2501 Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report A-8). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Lee, K. (2000). Crying patterns of Korean infants in institutions. Child Care, Health, and Development, 26, 217228.Google Scholar
Lemery-Chalfant, K., Schreiber, J. E., Schmidt, N. L., Van Hulle, C. A., Essex, M. J., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2007). Assessing internalizing, externalizing, and attention problems in young children: Validation of the MacArthur HBQ. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 13151323.Google Scholar
Loman, M. M., Wiik, K. L., Frenn, K. A., Pollak, S. D., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Postinstitutionalized children's development: Growth, cognitive, and language outcomes. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, 426434. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181b1fd08 Google Scholar
Martin, R. M., & Green, J. A. (2005). The use of emotion explanations by mothers: Relation to preschoolers’ gender and understanding of emotions. Social Development, 14, 229249. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00300.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mcquaid, N., Bigelow, A. E., McLaughlin, J., & MacLean, K. (2008). Maternal mental state language and preschool children's attachment security: Relation to children's mental state language and expressions of emotional understanding. Social Development, 17, 6183. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00415.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 17151726. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00501 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, A. L., Fine, S. E., Gouley, K. K., Seifer, R., Dickstein, S., & Shields, A. (2006). Showing and telling about emotions: Interrelations between facets of emotional competence and associations with classroom adjustment in Head Start preschoolers. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 11701192.Google Scholar
Moulson, M. C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). Early adverse experiences and the neurobiology of facial emotion processing. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1730. doi:10.1037/a0014035 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2012). The science of neglect: The persistent absence of responsive care disrupts the developing brain (Working Paper 12). Retrieved from http://www.developingchild.harvard.edu Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., Westerlund, A., McDermott, J. M., Zeanah, C. H., & Fox, N. A. (2013). Emotion recognition following early psychosocial deprivation. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 517525. doi:10.1017/S0954579412001216 Google Scholar
Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127152. doi:10.1080/17405620344000022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pons, F., Lawson, J., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2003). Individual differences in children's emotion understanding: Effects of age and language. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44, 347353. doi:10.1111/1467-9450.00354 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Racine, T. P., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Turnbull, W. (2007). Parent-child talk and children's understanding of beliefs and emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 480494. doi:10.1080/02699930600717599 Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734751.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Devitt, K., & Crowe, E. (2006). What mothers say and what they do: The relation between parenting, theory of mind, language and conflict/cooperation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 105124. doi:10.1348/026151005x82848 Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Taumoepeau, M., & Perkins, C. (2012). Statistical learning as a basis for social understanding in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 87104. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02045.x Google Scholar
Salmon, K., Dadds, M. R., Allen, J., & Hawes, D. J. (2009). Can emotional language skills be taught during parent training for conduct problem children? Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 40, 485498. doi:10.1007/s10578-009-0139-8 Google Scholar
Salmon, K., Evans, I. M., Moskowitz, S., Grouden, M., Parkes, F., & Miller, E. (2013). The components of young children's emotion knowledge: Which are enhanced by adult emotion talk? Social Development, 22, 94110. doi:10.1111/Sode.12004 Google Scholar
Schultz, D., Izard, C., & Ackerman, B. (2000). Children's anger attributional bias: Relations to family environment and social adjustment. Social Development, 9, 284301.Google Scholar
Schultz, D., Izard, C. E., Ackerman, B. P., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2001). Emotion knowledge in economically disadvantaged children: Self-regulatory antecedents and relations to social difficulties and withdrawal. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 5367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloutsky, V. M. (1997). Institutional care and developmental outcomes of 6- and 7-year-old children: A contextualist perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 131151.Google Scholar
Smyke, A. T., Koga, S. F., Johnson, D. E., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., Nelson, C. A., et al. (2007). The caregiving context in institution-reared and family-reared infants and toddlers in Romania. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 210218. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01694.x Google Scholar
Spackman, M. P., Fujiki, M., & Brinton, B. (2006). Understanding emotions in context: The effects of language impairment on children's ability to infer emotional reactions. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 173188. doi:10.1080/13682820500224091 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarullo, A. R., Bruce, J., & Gunnar, M. R. (2007). False belief and emotion understanding in post-institutionalized children. Social Development, 16, 5778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarullo, A. R., Garvin, M. C., & Gunnar, M. R. (2011). Atypical EEG power correlates with indiscriminately friendly behavior in internationally adopted children. Developmental Psychology, 47, 417431. doi:10.1037/a0021363 Google Scholar
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77, 465481. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00882.x Google Scholar
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others’ minds: Maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development, 79, 284302. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01126.x Google Scholar
Trentacosta, C. J., & Fine, S. E. (2010). Emotion knowledge, social competence, and behavior problems in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Social Development, 19, 129. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00543.x Google Scholar
Van Bergen, P., Salmon, K., Dadds, M. R., & Allen, J. (2009). The effects of mother training in emotion-rich, elaborative reminiscing on children's shared recall and emotion knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 162187. doi:10.1080/15248370903155825 Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2006). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 2006: Adoption as intervention. Meta-analytic evidence for massive catch-up and plasticity in physical, socio-emotional, and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 12281245. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01675.x Google Scholar
Vorria, P., Papaligoura, Z., Dunn, J., van, I. M. H., Steele, H., Kontopoulou, A., et al. (2003). Early experiences and attachment relationships of Greek infants raised in residential group care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 12081220.Google Scholar
Wiig, E., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (2004). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—Preschool (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wiik, K. L., Loman, M. M., Van Ryzin, M. J., Armstrong, J. M., Essex, M. J., Pollak, S. D., et al. (2011). Behavioral and emotional symptoms of post-institutionalized children in middle childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 5663. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02294.x Google Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., Egger, H. L., Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., et al. (2009). Institutional rearing and psychiatric disorders in romanian preschool children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 777785. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08091438 Google Scholar