Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:50:34.413Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Child maltreatment, attachment, and the self system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in toddlers at high social risk

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Marjorie Beeghly*
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Harvard Medical School & Child Development Unit, Children's Hospital, Boston
Dante Cicchetti
Affiliation:
Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Marjorie Beeghly, Ph.D., Child Development Unit, Children's Hospital, 300 Longwood Avenue, Boston, MA 02115.

Abstract

The ability to talk about the internal states (ISs) and feelings of self and other is an age-appropriate development of late toddlerhood hypothesized to reflect toddlers' emergent self–other understanding and to be fundamental to the regulation of social interaction. This study examined the effects of child maltreatment on the emergence of low-socioeconomic status 30-month-old toddlers' IS lexicons. Children's lexicons were derived both from maternal interviews and from observations of children's spontaneous IS utterances in four laboratory contexts. Results from both data sources indicated that maltreated toddlers produced significantly fewer IS words, fewer IS word types, and proportionately fewer IS words denoting physiological states and negative affect than nonmaltreated toddlers. In addition, maltreated toddlers were more context bound in IS language use and more restricted in their attributions of internal states to self and other. Gender differences were also observed. Individual differences in children's IS language production were significantly related to general linguistic maturity in both groups but to toddlers' conversational skills only in the comparison group. In addition, a cumulative risk model describing the effects of the child's attachment relationship with the caregiver on early IS language was tested. Toddlers most severely at risk (maltreated/insecure) had the most compromised IS language. Thus, secure attachment may serve as a protective mechanism against self-dysfunction in maltreated toddlers.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aber, J. L., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). The socio-emotional development of maltreated children: An empirical and theoretical analysis. In Fitzgerald, H., Lester, B., & Yogman, M. W. (Eds.), Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 2, pp. 147205). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Alessandri, S. M. (1991). Play and social behavior in maltreated preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 191205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, S., Gurgreund, M., Satterly, D., & Wells, G. (1983). Characteristics of adult speech which predict children's language development. Journal of Child Language, 10, 6584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, K. C., & Campos, J. J. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development II: A functionalist approach to emotions. In Osofsky, J. D. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 555578). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1990). Language about me and you. Pronominal references and the emerging concept of self. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (Eds.), The self in transition (pp. 165182). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., O'Connell, B., & Shore, C. (1987). Language and communication in infancy. In Osofsky, J. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Beckwith, R. T. (1991). The language of emotion, the emotions, and nominalist bootstrapping. In Fry, D. & Moore, C. (Eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 7796). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M. (1993). Parent-child play as a window on infant competence. In MacDonald, K. (Ed.), Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications (pp. 71112). New York: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., & Mervis, C. (1986). Mothers' internal state language to toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 247260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., & Vondra, J. (1989). Lessons from child abuse: The determinants of parenting. In Cicchetti, D. & Carlson, V. (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 153202). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blager, F., & Martin, H. (1976). Speech and language of abused children. In Martin, H. P. (Ed.), The abused child (pp. 8392). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1991). Language development: From two to three. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., & Beckwith, R. (1989). Talking with feeling: Integrating affective and linguistic expression in early language development. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 313342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bousha, D., & Twentyman, C. (1984). Mother–child interactional style in abuse, neglect, and control groups: Naturalistic observations in the home. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 106114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. I. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. (1987). New perspectives on attachment relations: Security, communication, and internal stale working models. In Osofsky, J. D. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 10591100). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. (1991). Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. In Frye, D. & Moore, C. (Eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 4975). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1989). Pretense: Acting “as if.” In Bridges, J. J. & Hazen, N. H. (Eds.), Action in social context: Perspectives on early development (pp. 239271). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986). Learning to talk about emotions: A functionalist perspective. Child Development, 55, 529548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural communication: When do infants acquire a “theory of mind”? In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition (pp. 333373). Hills-dale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bromley, D. B. (1977). Natural language and the development of self. In Keasey, C. B. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 117167). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1980a). Coding child utterances for conversational relevance. Unpublished coding manual, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1980b). The maintenance of conversation. In Olson, D. R. (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Burgess, R., & Conger, R. (1978). Family interaction in abusive, neglectful, and normal families. Child Development, 49, 11631173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 525531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, J. (1988). Child-mother attachment and the self at age six. Child Development, 57, 331337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relationship to other defensive processes. In Belsky, J. & Nezworski, T. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 300323). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cassidy, J., & Marvin, R., with the MacArthur Working Group on Attachment Beyond Infancy. (1992). Attachment organization in preschool children: Procedure and coding manual. Unpublished manuscript, University of Virginia, Charlottesville.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1991). Fractures in the crystal: Developmental psychopathology and the emergence of self. Developmental Review, 11, 271287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (1987). Symbolic development in maltreated youngsters: An organizational perspective. New Directions in Child Development, 36, 4768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Carlson, V. (Eds.). (1989). Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Carlson, V., Braunwald, K., & Aber, J. L. (1987). The Harvard child maltreatment project: A context for research on the sequelae of child maltreatment. In Gelles, R. & Lancaster, J. (Eds.), Research in child abuse: Biosocial perspectives (pp. 277298). Chicago: Aldine Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Ganiban, J., & Barnett, D. (1991). Contributions from the study of high risk populations to understanding the development of emotion regulation. In Garber, J. & Dodge, K. (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 1549). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Garmezy, N. (Eds.). (1993). Milestones in the development of resilience [Special issue]. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 497502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional model of community violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for children's development. Psychiatry, 56, 96118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Manly, J. T. (1990). A personal perspective on conducting research with maltreating families: Problems and solutions. In Brody, G. & Sigel, I. (Eds.), Methods of family research: Vol. 2. Families at risk (pp. 87133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rizley, R. (1981). Developmental perspectives on the etiology, intergenerational transmission, and sequelae of child maltreatment. In Rizley, R. & Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on child maltreatment (Vol. 11, pp. 3157). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Lynch, M., & Holt, K. D. (1993). Resilence in maltreated children: Processes leading to adaptive outcome. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 629647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coster, W., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Research on the communicative development of maltreated children: Clinical implications. Topics in Language Disorders, 13, 2538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coster, W., Gersten, M. S., Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Communicative functioning in maltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 10201029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, P. M. (1988). Relationships at risk. In Belsky, J. & Nezworski, T. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 136174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crittenden, P. M. (1990). Internal representational models of attachment relationships. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 259277.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, P. M., & Ainsworth, M. (1989). Attachment and child abuse. In Cicchetti, D. & Carlson, V. (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 432463). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, P. M., & DiLalla, D. L. (1988). Compulsive compliance: The development of an inhibitory coping strategy in infancy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 585599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denham, S., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of peer status in young preschoolers. Child Development, 61, 11451152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 132139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1991). Relationships, talk about feelings, and the development of affect regulation in early childhood. In Garber, J. & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 89108). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991a). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of others' emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27, 448455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991b). Young children's understanding of other peoples' feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 13521366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. (1981). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981). Developmental sequelae of maltreatment in infancy. New Directions for Child Development, 11, 7792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emde, R., Johnson, W., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1987). The do's and don'ts of early moral development: Psychoanalytic tradition and current research. In Kagan, J. & Lamb, S. (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 245276). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1991). Technical manual for the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. San Diego: San Diego State University.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (1989). Exploring sex differences in the emotional content of mother–child conversations about the past. Sex Roles, 20, 675691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaensbauer, T. J., & Sands, S. K. (1979). Distorted affective communications in abused/neglected infants and their potential impact on caretakers. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 236250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gersten, M., Coster, W., Schneider-Rosen, K., Carlson, V., & Cicchetti, D. (1986). The socio-emotional bases of communicative functioning: Quality of attachment, language development, and early maltreatment. In Lamb, M. E., Brown, A. L., & Rogoff, B. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 105151). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Giovannoni, J., & Becerra, R. M. (1979). Defining child abuse. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false-belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 2637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Speltz, M. (1991). Emotional regulation, self-control, and psycho-pathology: The role of relationships in early childhood. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 2. Internalizing and externalizing expressions of dysfunction (pp. 2155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Grusec, J. E., & Walters, G. E. (1991). Psychological abuse and childrearing belief systems. In Starr, R. H. & Wolfe, D. A. (Eds.). The effects of child abuse and neglect: Issues and research (pp. 186202). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. New York: Basil Blackwcll.Google Scholar
Hood, L., & Bloom, L. (1979). What, when, and how about why: A longitudinal study of early expressions in causality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44 (2, Serial No. 181).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (1987). The consequences of language delay for other aspects of development. In Yule, W. & Rutter, M. (Eds.), Language development and language disorders (pp. 271294). Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Jaffe, P. G., Wolfe, D. A., & Wilson, S. K. (1990). Children of battered women: Vol. 21. Developmental clinical psychology and psychiatry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1981). The second year: The emergence of self awareness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., & Lamb, S. (1987). The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kestenbaum, R., Farber, E. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Individual differences in empathy among preschoolers: Relation to attachment history. In Eisenberg, N. (Ed.), Empathy and related emotional responses (pp. 5156). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., Stanger, C., & Sullivan, M. W. (1989). Deception in three year olds. Developmental Psychology, 25, 439443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self-development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 59, 146156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Patterns of relatedness in maltreated and nonmaltreated children: Connections among multiple representation models. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 207226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D. B., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relations among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Developmental Psychology, 23, 223232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahler, M., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research (pp. 66104). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In Greenberg, M., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121160). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 425444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competent functioning. Child Development, 49, 547555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLloyd, V. C., & Wilson, L. (1991). The strain of living poor: Parenting, social support, and child mental health. In Huston, A. C. (Ed.), Children in poverty: Child development and public policy (pp. 105135). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. (1984). Early lexical development: The contributions of mother and child. In Sophian, C. (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 339370). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morrisset, C., Barnard, K. E., Greenberg, M. T., Booth, C. L., & Spieker, S. J. (1990). Environmental influences on early language development: The context of social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 127149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nock, S., & Rossi, P. (1979). Household types and social standing. Social Forces, 57, 13251345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oates, R. K., Peacock, A., & Forrest, D. (1984). The development of abused children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 26, 649656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Pipp, S., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Harmon, R. (1992). The relation between attachment and knowledge of self and mother in one- to three-year-old infants. Child Development, 63, 738750.Google ScholarPubMed
Prizant, B. M., & Wetherby, A. M. (1990). Toward an integrated view of early language and communication development and socioemolional development. Topics in Language Disorders, 10, 116.Google Scholar
Radke-Yarrow, M., & Sherman, T. (1990). Hard growing: Children who survive. In Rolf, J., Masten, A. S., Cicchetti, D., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Weintraub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 97119). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridgeway, D., Waters, E., & Kuczaj, S. (1985). Acquisition of emotion-descriptive language: Receptive and productive vocabulary norms for ages 18 months to 6 years. Developmental Psychology, 21, 901908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rieder, C., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Organizational perspective on cognitive control functioning and cognitive-affective balances in maltreated children. Developmental Psychology, 25, 382393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsy-chiatry, 57, 316331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent–child discourse. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Child language (Vol. 4, pp. 128). New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In Horowitz, F. (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4, pp. 187244). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J., & Seifer, R. (1990). Early contributors to developmental risk. In Rolf, J., Masten, A. S., Cicchetti, D., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Weintraub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 5266). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). The relationship between affect and cognition in maltreated infants: Quality of attachment and the development of visual self-recognition. Child Development, 55, 648658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Early self-knowledge and emotional development: Visual self-recognition and affective reactions to mirror self-images in maltreated and nonmaltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 27, 471478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the. first reference to mental state. Cognition, 14, 301321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silber, S. (1990). Conflict negotiation in child abusing and nonabusing families. Journal of Family Psychology, 3, 368384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silber, S., Bermann, E., Henderson, M., & Lehman, A. (1993). Patterns of influence and response in abusing and nonabusing families. Journal of Family Violence, 8, 2738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smiley, P., & Huttenlocher, J. (1989). Young children's acquisition of emotion concepts. In Saarni, C. & Harris, P. L. (Eds.), Children's understanding of emotions (pp. 2749). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1984). Parent–child interaction and the development of communicative ability. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Pickar, J. (Eds.), The acquisition of communicative competence. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Spieker, S. J., & Booth, C. L. (1988). Maternal antecedents of attachment quality. In Belsky, J. & Nezworski, T. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1990). An organizational perspective on the self. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 281308). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Starr, R., & Wolfe, D. (Eds.). (1991). The effects of child abuse and neglect: Issues and research. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Trickett, P., Aber, J. L., Carlson, V., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Child rearing characteristics and child development in two samples of physically abusive families. Developmental Psychology, 27, 148158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E. (1989). Emotions and emotions communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vondra, J., Barnett, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Perceived and actual competence among maltreated and comparison school children. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 237255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasserman, G., Green, A., & Allen, R. (1983). Going beyond abuse: Maladaptive patterns of interaction in abusing mother–infant pairs. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 22, 245252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, M., & Fischer, K. (1977). A developmental sequence of agent use in late infancy. Child Development, 48, 828836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1988). First steps in the child's theorizing about the mind. In Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 6492). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, D. A. (1985). Child abusive parents: An empirical review and analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 462482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar