Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:39:29.212Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Child effects and child care: Implications for risk and adjustment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 October 2015

Emily K. Snell*
Affiliation:
Temple University
Annemarie H. Hindman
Affiliation:
Temple University
Jay Belsky
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Emily Snell, College of Education, Temple University, Ritter Hall 437, 301 Cecil B. Moore Avenue, Philadelphia, PA 19122-6091; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Evocative effects of child characteristics on the quality and quantity of child care were assessed in two studies using longitudinal data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. We focus on the influence of child characteristics on two important aspects of the child care experience: language stimulation provided by caregivers and quantity of care. In Study 1, associations between the developmental status of children aged 15 to 54 months and the language stimulation provided by their caregivers were examined using path models, and longitudinal child effects were detected across the earliest time points of the study. In Study 2, the associations among child behavior, temperament, development, and time in care were examined. Little evidence was found for such child effects on time in care. The results are discussed in terms of the effects of child care on child development and implications for developmental processes, particularly for children at greatest risk for developmental delay or psychopathology.

Type
Special Section Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/2–3 and 1992 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Acock, A. C. (2005). Working with missing values. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 67, 10121028.Google Scholar
Anderson, K. E., Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1986). Mothers’ interactions with normal and conduct-disordered boys: Who affects whom? Developmental Psychology, 22, 604609.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley II Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 8195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J. (1986). Infant day care: A cause for concern? Zero to Three, 6, 19.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (1988). The “effects” of infant day care reconsidered. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 3, 235272.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (1990). Parental and non-parental child care and children's socio-emotional development: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 885903.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (1994, September 4). The effects of infant day care: 1986–1994. Invited plenary address presented to the British Psychological Association Division of Developmental Psychology, University of Portsmouth.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (2001). Emmanuel Miller Lecture: Developmental risks (still) associated with early child care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 845859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Vandell, D., Burchinal, M., Clarke-Stewart, K. A., McCartney, K., Owen, M., et al. (2007). Are there long-term effects of early child care? Child Development, 78, 681701.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J., & Bonvillian, J. (1997). Theoretical approaches to language acquisition. In Berko Gleason, J. (Ed.), The development of language (4th ed., pp. 259316). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J. N., & Marquis, A. L. (1977). Children's control of adult speech. Child Development, 48, 10021008.Google Scholar
Bokhorst, C. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Fearon, R. M. P., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Fonagy, P., & Schuengel, C. (2003). The importance of shared environment in mother-infant attachment security: A behavioral genetic study. Child Development, 74, 17691783.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Infant into conversant: Language and nonlanguage processes in developing early communication. In Budwig, N., Uzgiris, I. C., & Wertsch, J. V. (Eds.), Communication: An arena of development (pp. 109129). Stamford, CT: Ablex.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Administration manual: HOME observation for measurement of the environment. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.Google Scholar
Claessens, A., & Chen, J. H. (2013). Multiple child care arrangements and child well being: Early care experiences in Australia. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 4961.Google Scholar
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1989). Infant day care: Maligned or malignant? American Psychologist, 44, 266.Google Scholar
Clarke-Stewart, K., VanderStoep, L., & Killian, G. (1979). Analysis and replication of mother–child relations at two years of age. Child Development, 50, 777793.Google Scholar
Côté, S. M., Mongeau, C., Japel, C., Xu, Q., Séguin, J. R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2013). Child care quality and cognitive development: Trajectories leading to better preacademic skills. Child Development, 84, 752766.Google Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1978). Mothers' speech and its association with rate of linguistic development in young children. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (Eds.), The development of communication. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cross, T. G., & Morris, J. E. (1980). Linguistic feedback and maternal speech: Comparisons of mothers addressing infants, one-year-olds and two-year-olds. First Language, 1, 98121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crouter, A. C., & Booth, A. (2003). Children's influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
De Schipper, J. C., Tavecchio, L. W., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & van Zeijl, J. (2004). Goodness-of-fit in center day care: Relations of temperament, stability, and quality of care with the child's adjustment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 257272.Google Scholar
Dunham, P., & Dunham, F. (1992). Lexical development during middle infancy: A mutually driven infant-caregiver process. Developmental Psychology, 28, 414420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 728.Google Scholar
Fox, N., & Fein, G. (1990). Infant day-care: The debate. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Fox, N., Henderson, H., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance. Child Development, 72, 121.Google Scholar
Gagnon, S. G., & Nagle, R. J. (2000). Comparison of the revised and original versions of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. School Psychology International, 21, 293305.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L., Hoaken, L., Weitzman, E., & van Lieshout, R. (2000). Patterns of adult-child linguistic interaction in integrated day care groups. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 155168.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2002). Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions with toddlers and preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 268.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 13681378.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input in acquiring a lexicon. Child Development, 73, 418433.Google Scholar
Howes, C., Whitebook, M., & Phillips, D. (1992). Teacher characteristics and effective teaching in child care. Findings from the National Child Care Staffing Study: Child and Youth Care Forum, 21, 399414.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236248.Google Scholar
Johansen, A. S., Leibowitz, A., & Waite, L. J. (1996). The importance of child-care characteristics to choice of care. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 759772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knafo, A., & Jaffee, S. R. (2013). Gene–environment correlation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 16.Google Scholar
Koles, B., O'Connor, E., & McCartney, K. (2009). Teacher-child relationships in preschool: The influence of child and teacher characteristics. Journal of Early Childhood Teacher Education, 30, 321.Google Scholar
Langlois, J. H., & Liben, L. S. (2003). Child care research: An editorial perspective. Child Development, 74, 969975.Google Scholar
Lansford, J. E., Criss, M. M., Laird, R. D., Shaw, D. S., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., et al. (2011). Reciprocal relations between parents’ physical discipline and children's externalizing behavior during middle childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 225238.Google Scholar
Leyva, D., Weiland, C., Barata, M., Yoshikawa, H., Snow, C., Treviño, E., et al. (2015). Teacher-child interactions in Chile and their associations with prekindergarten outcomes. Child Development. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Liang, X., Fuller, B., & Singer, J. D. (2000). Ethnic differences in child care selection: The influence of family structure, parental practices, and home language. Early Child Research Quarterly, 15, 357384.Google Scholar
Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Loeb, S., Fuller, B., Kagan, S. L., & Carrol, B. (2005). Child care in poor communities: Early learning effects of type, quality, and stability. Child Development, 75, 4765.Google Scholar
Love, J. M., Harrison, L., Sagi-Schwartz, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Ross, C., Ungerer, J. A., et al. (2003). Child care quality matters: How conclusions may vary with context. Child Development, 74, 10211033.Google Scholar
Marshall, N. (2004). The quality of early child care and children's development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 165168.Google Scholar
Medoff-Cooper, B., Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1993). The early infancy temperament questionnaire. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 14, 230235.Google Scholar
Merton, R. K. (1968). The Matthew effect in science. Science, 159, 5663.Google Scholar
Newcombe, N. S. (2003). Some controls control too much. Child Development, 74, 10501052.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1996). Characteristics of infant care: Factors contributing to positive caregiving. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11, 269306.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1998). Early child care and self-control, compliance and problem behavior at 24 and 36 months. Child Development, 69, 11451170.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). The relation of child care to cognitive and language development. Child Development, 71, 960980.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2001). Nonmaternal care and family factors in early development: An overview of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 457492.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2006). Child-care effect sizes for the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. American Psychologist, 61, 99116.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, & Duncan, G. (2003). Modeling the impact of child care quality on children's cognitive development. Child Development, 74, 14541475.Google Scholar
Nomaguchi, K. M. (2006). Maternal employment, nonparental care, mother-child interactions, and child outcomes during the preschool years. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 68, 13411369.Google Scholar
Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., Burchinal, M., Clifford, R. M., Culkin, M., Howes, C., Kagan, S. L., et al. (2001). The relation of preschool child care quality to children's cognitive and social developmental trajectories through second grade. Child Development, 72, 15341553.Google Scholar
Pener-Tessler, R., Avinun, R., Uzefovsky, F., Edelman, S., Ebstein, R. P., & Knafo, A. (2013). Boys’ serotonin transporter genotype affects maternal behavior through self-control: A case of evocative gene–environment correlation. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 151162.Google Scholar
Phillips, D., McCartney, K., Scarr, S., & Howes, C. (1987). Selective review of infant day care research: A cause for concern. Zero to Three, 7, 1821.Google Scholar
Pungello, E. P., & Kurtz-Costes, B. (1999). Why and how working women choose child care: A review with a focus on infancy. Developmental Review, 19, 3196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.Google Scholar
Reynell, J. (1991). Reynell Developmental Language Scales (US ed.). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Service.Google Scholar
Reynell, J., & Gruber, C. P. (1990). Reynell Language Development Scales (US ed.). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Royston, P. (2004). Multiple imputation of missing values. Stata Journal, 4, 227241.Google Scholar
Rudasill, K. M., & Rimm-Kaufman, S. E. (2009). Teacher-child relationship quality: The roles of child temperament and teacher-child interactions. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 107120.Google Scholar
Rudasill, K. M., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Justice, L. M., & Pence, K. (2006). Temperament and language skills as predictors of teacher-child relationship quality in preschool. Early Education and Development, 17, 271291.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424435.Google Scholar
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2003). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147177.Google Scholar
Serbin, L. A., O'Leary, K. D., Kent, R. N., & Tonick, I. J. (1973). A comparison of teacher response to the preacademic and problem behavior of boys and girls. Child Development, 44, 796804.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J., & Phillips, D. (Eds). (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Singer, J. D., Fuller, B., Keiley, M. K., & Wolf, A. (1998). Early child-care selection: Variation by geographic location, maternal characteristics, and family structure. Developmental Psychology, 34, 11291144.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1989). Understanding social interaction and language acquisition: Sentences are not enough. In Bornstein, M. H. & Bruner, J. S. (Eds.), Interaction in human development (pp. 83103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sokolov, J. (1993). A local contingency analysis of the fine-tuning hypothesis. Developmental Psychology, 29, 10081023.Google Scholar
Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2012). Preschool quality and educational outcomes at age 11: Low quality has little benefit. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 9, 109124 Google Scholar
Sylva, K., Stein, A., Leach, P., Barnes, J., & Malmberg, L. E. (2007). Family and child factors related to the use of non-maternal infant care: An English study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 118136.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C., Bornstein, M., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748767.Google Scholar
Vallotton, C. D. (2009). Do infants influence their quality of care? Infants’ communicative gestures predict caregivers’ responsiveness. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 351365.Google Scholar
Vandell, D. L., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., Steinberg, L., & Vandergrift, N. (2010). Do effects of early child care extend to age 15 years? Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. Child Development, 81, 737756.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 21432152.Google Scholar
Widaman, K. F. (2006). III. Missing data: What to do with or without them. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 71(3), 4264.Google Scholar
Winer, B. J. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw–Hill.Google Scholar
Zachrisson, H. D., Dearing, E., Lekhal, R., & Toppelberg, C. O. (2013). Little evidence that time in child care causes externalizing problems during early childhood in Norway. Child Development, 84, 11521170. doi:10.1111/cdev.12040 Google Scholar
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1979). Preschool Language Scale (rev. ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, B., Stevenson, J., & Bailey, V. (1987). Sleep problems in early childhood: Continuities, predictive factors, and behavioral correlates. Pediatrics, 80, 664671.Google Scholar