Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:14:40.320Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attachment behavior of infants exposed prenatally to alcohol: Mediating effects of infant affect and mother-infant interaction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Mary J. O'Connor
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Institute, University of California at Los Angeles
Marian Sigman
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Institute, University of California at Los Angeles
Connie Kasari
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatric Institute, University of California at Los Angeles

Abstract

Children exposed to alcohol prenatally have been found to be at risk for developmental and behavioral problems; however, the relation between maternal alcohol consumption and other environmental factors has not been fully explored in the literature. In this study, subjects were 44 firstborn infants of women over 30 years of age. Structural equation modeling was used to test the hypothesis that maternal alcohol use during pregnancy was related to infant negative affect. Furthermore, negative affect was assumed to influence the mother-infant relationship. It was hypothesized that mothers of infants exhibiting negative affect would be less positive in interaction and that insecure infant attachment behavior would be more prevalent in these infants. Results of model testing confirmed the hypothesis. In addition, maternal alcohol consumption following pregnancy was not found to influence significantly either maternal interaction or infant attachment behavior.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1972). Individual differences in strange-situation behavior of one-year-olds. In Schaffer, H. R. (Ed.), The origins of human social relations (pp. 1752). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (Eds.). (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Algina, J. (1978). Comment on Bartko's “On various intraclass correlation reliability coefficients.” Psychological Bulletin, 85, 133138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnard, K. E., Bee, H. L., & Hammond, M. A. (1984). Developmental changes in maternal interactions with term and preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 101113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 11711190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Rovine, M., & Taylor, D. G. (1984). The Pennsylvania infant and family development project: 3. The origins of individual differences in infant-mother attachment: Maternal and infant contributions. Child Development, 55, 718728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentler, P. M. (1983a). Simultaneous equation systems as moment structure models: With an introduction to latent variable models. Journal of Econometrics, 22, 1342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1983b). Some contributions to efficient statistics in structural models: Specification and estimation of moment structures. Psychometrika, 48, 493517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1985). Theory and implementation of EQS: A structural equations program. Los Angeles: BMDP Statistical Software.Google Scholar
Bentler, P. M., & Dijkstra, T. (1985). Efficient estimation via linearization in structural models. In Krishnaiah, P. R. (Ed.), Multivariate analysis (Vol. 6, pp. 942). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Boukydis, C. F. Z., & Burgess, R. L. (1982). Adult psychological response to infant cries: Effects of temperament of infant. Child Development, 53, 12911298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyle, B. S. (1967). Shipley Institute of Living Scale. Hartford, CT: Neuropsychiatric Institute of Hartford Retreat.Google Scholar
Browne, M. W. (1982). Covariance structures. In Hawkins, D. M. (Ed.), Topics in applied multivariate analysis (pp. 72141). London: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne, M. W. (1984). Asymptotically distribution-free methods for the analysis of covariance structures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 37, 6283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. B. G. (1979). Mother-infant interaction as a function of maternal ratings of temperament. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 10, 6776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawley, S. B., & Spiker, D. (1983). Mother-child interactions involving two-year-olds with Down syndrome. Child Development, 54, 13121323.Google Scholar
Crockenberg, S. (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and social support influences on the security of infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 52, 857865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowler, J. K., & Jacobson, S. W. (1984, 03). Alternative measures of maternal smoking and caffeine consumption as predictors of neonatal outcome. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies,New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egeland, B., & Farber, E. A. (1984). Infant-mother attachment. Factors related to its development and changes over time. Child Development, 55, p753771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emhart, C. D., Morrow-Tlucak, M. E., Sokol, R. J., & Martier, S. S. (1988). Underreporting of alcohol use in pregnancy. Alcoholism Clinical and Experimental Research, 12, 506511.Google Scholar
Goldberg, S., Brachfeld, S., & Divitto, B. (1980). Feeding, fussing and play: Parent-infant interaction in the first year as a function of prematurity and perinatal medical problems In Field, T. M., Goldberg, S., & Stern, D. (Eds.), High-risk infants and children: Adult and peer interactions (pp. 133153). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heinicke, C. M. (1985, 04). Pre-birth couple functioning and the quality of the mother-infant relationship in the second half of the first year of life. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,Toronto.Google Scholar
Heinicke, C. M., Diskin, S. D., Ramsey-Klee, D. M., & Gates, D. S. (1986). Pre- and post-birth antecedents of two-year-old attention, capacity for relationships, and verbal expressiveness. Developmental Psychology, 22, 777787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Jacobson, S. W., Fein, G. G., Jacobson, J. L., Schwartz, P. M., & Dowler, J. K. (1984). Neonatal correlates of prenatal exposure to smoking, caffeine, and alcohol. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 121140.Google Scholar
Jacobson, S. W., Jacobson, J. L., Sokol, R. J., Martier, S. S., Ager, J. W., & Kaplan, M. O. (1991). Maternal recall of alcohol, cocaine, and marijuana use during pregnancy. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 13, 535540.Google Scholar
Jessor, R., Graves, R. D., Hanson, R. C., & Gessor, S. L. (1968). Society, personality and deviant behavior: A study of a triethnic community. New York: Rhinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Littman, B., & Parmelee, A. H. (1978). Medical correlates of infant development. Pediatrics, 61, 470474.Google Scholar
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Predicting a mother's rejection of her infant from representations of her own childhood experiences. International Journal of Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 202217.Google Scholar
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern: Procedures, findings and implications for the classification of behavior. In Yogman, M. & Brazelton, T. B. (Eds.), Affective development in infancy (pp. 95124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Milliones, J. (1978). Relationship between perceived child temperament and maternal behaviors. Child Development, 49, 12551257.Google Scholar
Mitchell, S. (1979). Interobserver agreement, reliability and generalizability of data collected in observational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 376390.Google Scholar
O'Connor, M. J., Brill, N., & Sigman, M. (1986). Alcohol use in elderly primips: Relation to infant development. Pediatrics, 78, 444450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, M. J., Sigman, M., & Brill, N. (1987). Disorganization of attachment in relation to maternal alcohol consumption. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 831836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pianta, R. C., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1989). Continuity and discontinuity in maternal sensitivity at 6, 24, and 42 months in a high risk sample. Child Development, 60, 481487.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In Horowitz, F. D. (Ed.), Review of child development research (pp. 187244). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shapiro, A. (1983). Asymptotic distribution theory in the analysis of covariance structures (a unified approach). South African Statistical Journal, 17, 3381.Google Scholar
Sigman, M. (1983). Individual differences in infant attention: Relations to birth status and intelligence at five years. In Field, T. & Sostek, A. (Eds.), Infants born at risk: Physiological, perceptual and cognitive processes. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., & Pederson, D. R. (1988). Maternal sensitivity and patterns of infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 59, 10971101.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 11841199.Google Scholar
Streissguth, A. P. (1978). Fetal alcohol syndrome: An epidemiologic perspective. American Journal of Epidemiology, 107, 467478.Google Scholar
Streissguth, A. P. (1986). The behavioral teratology of alcohol: Performance, behavioral, and intellectual deficits in prenatally exposed children. In West, J. R. (Ed.), Alcohol and brain development (pp. 344). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Streissguth, A. P., Barr, H. M., & Martin, D. C. (1983). Maternal alcohol use and neonatal habituation assessed with the Brazelton Scale. Child Development, 54, 11091118.Google Scholar
Streissguth, A. P., Barr, H. M., & Sampson, P. D. (1990). Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure: Effects on child IQ and learning problems at age 7½ years. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 14(5), 662669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Streissguth, A. P., Barr, H. M., Sampson, P. D., Darby, B. L., & Martin, D. C. (1989). IQ at age 4 in relation to maternal alcohol use and smoking during pregnancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Streissguth, A. P., Landesman-Dwyer, S. J., Martin, S. C., & Smith, D. W. (1980). Teratogenic effects of alcohol in humans and animals. Science, 209, 353361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. J. (1979). Effects of maternal alcohol consumption on offspring: Review, critical assessment, and future directions. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 4, 265276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar