Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:14:01.447Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mothers' depressive symptoms and children's facial emotions: Examining the depression–inhibition hypothesis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2012

Theodore Dix*
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Austin
Leah N. Meunier
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Austin
Kathryn Lusk
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Austin
Michelle M. Perfect
Affiliation:
University of Arizona
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Theodore Dix, 1University Station, Department of Human Development and Family Sciences, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Vibrant expression of emotion is the principal means infants and young children use to elicit appropriate and timely caregiving, stimulation, and support. This study examined the depression–inhibition hypothesis: that declines in mothers' support as their depressive symptoms increase inhibit children's emotional communication. Ninety-four mothers and their 14- to 27-month-olds interacted in a university playroom. Based on microanalytic coding of discrete facial displays, results supported three components of the hypothesis. (a) As mothers' depressive symptoms increased, children displayed less facial emotion (more flat affect, less joy, less sadness, less negative). (b) Mothers' low emotional and behavioral support predicted children's low facial communication and mediated relations between mothers' depressive symptoms and children's infrequent emotion. (c) Children who were passive with mothers behaviorally expressed emotion infrequently. Children's passivity mediated relations between mothers' depressive symptoms and children's infrequent emotion displays. Contrary to modeling and contagion theories, mothers' facial displays did not mediate relations between their depressive symptoms and children's facial displays. Nor were the outcomes children experienced regulating their facial displays. Rather, findings suggest that, even when depressive symptoms are modest, young children inhibit emotion as mothers' depressive symptoms increase to withdraw from unresponsive mothers, which may adversely affect children's subsequent relationships and competencies.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abe, J. A., & Izard, C. E. (1999). The developmental functions of emotions: An analysis in terms of differential emotions theory. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 523549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahnert, L., Gunner, M. R., Lamb, M. E., & Barthel, M. (2004). Transition to child care: Association with infant–mother attachment, infant negative emotion, and cortisol elevations. Child Development, 75, 639650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (1992). Development in infancy: An introduction. New York: McGraw–Hill.Google Scholar
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Garwood, M., Powers, B. P., & Wang, X. (2001). Parental sensitivity, infant affect, and affect regulation: Predictors of later attachment. Child Development, 72, 252270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brownell, C. A., & Kopp, C. B. (2007). Socioemotional development in the toddler years. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Buss, K. A., & Kiel, E. J. (2004). Comparison of sadness, anger, and fear facial expressions when toddlers look at their mothers. Child Development, 75, 17611773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, A. C., & Trivedi, P. K. (1990 ). Regression-based tests for overdispersion in the Poisson model. Journal of Econometrics, 46, 347364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences on attachment relationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3, Serial No. 240).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, J. F., Campbell, S. B., Matias, R., & Hopkins, J. (1990). Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed and nondepressed mother–infant pairs at 2-months. Developmental Psychology, 26, 1523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, J. F., Matias, R., Tronick, E. Z., Connell, D., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (1986). Face to face interactions of depressed mothers and their infants. In Tronick, E. Z. & Field, T. (Eds.), New directions for child development: Vol. 34. Maternal depression and infant disturbance (pp. 3145). San Francisco, CA: Jossey–Bass.Google Scholar
Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental Psychology, 26, 961971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, G., Klinger, L. G., Panagiotides, H., Hill, D., & Spieker, S. (1992). Frontal lobe activity and affective behavior of infants of mothers with depressive symptoms. Child Development, 63, 725737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptive processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dix, T., & Branca, S. (2003). Parenting as a goal-regulation process. In Kuczynski, L. (Ed.), Handbook of the dynamics of parenting. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Dix, T., & Buck, K. A. (2011). The emergence of social approach and avoidance motivation in early parent-child relationships. In Campbell, L. & Loving, T. J. (Eds.), Close relationships: An integrative perspective (pp. 5381). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Dix, T., Cheng, N., & Day, W. H. (2009). Connecting with parents: Mothers' depressive symptoms and responsive behaviors in the regulation of social contact by 1- and young 2-year-olds. Social Development, 18, 2450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dix, T., Gershoff, E. T., Meunier, L. N., & Miller, P. C. (2004). The affective structure of supportive parenting: Depressive symptoms, immediate emotions, and child-oriented motivation. Developmental Psychology, 40, 12121227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dix, T., & Meunier, L. N. (2009). Depressive symptoms and parenting competence: An analysis of thirteen regulatory processes. Developmental Review, 29, 4568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dix, T., Stewart, A. D., Gershoff, E. T., & Day, W. H. (2007). Autonomy and children's reactions to being controlled: Evidence that both compliance and defiance may be positive markers in early development. Child Development, 78, 12041221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (1990). Developmental psychopathology of children with depressed mothers. Developmental Psychology, 26, 36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downey, G., & Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of depressed parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 5076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1987). Development of justification in disputes with mother and sibling. Developmental Psychology, 23, 791798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2002). Emotion-related regulation and its relation to quality of social functioning. In Hartup, W. W. & Weinberg, R. A. (Eds.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 133171). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parents' reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67, 22272247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Feng, X., Shaw, D. S., Kovacs, M., Lane, T., O'Rourke, F. E., & Alarcon, J. H. (2008). Emotion regulation in preschoolers: The roles of behavioral inhibition, maternal affective behavior, and maternal depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 132141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feng, X., Shaw, D. S., Skuban, E. M., & Lane, T. (2007). Emotional exchanges in mother–child dyads: Stability, mutual influence, and associations with maternal depression and child problem behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 714725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 527532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T. (1995). Psychologically depressed parents. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Applied and practical parenting. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Bendell, D., Schanberg, S., Zimmerman, E. A., & Kuhn, C. (1988). Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” behavior even with nondepressed adults. Child Development, 59, 15691579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., Diego, M., Feijo, L., Vera, Y., Gil, K., et al. (2007). Still-face and separation effects on depressed mother–infant interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28, 314323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T., Sandberg, D., Garcia, R., Vega-Lahr, N., Goldstein, S., & Guy, L. (1985). Pregnancy problems, postpartum depression, and early mother–infant interaction. Developmental Psychology, 21, 11521156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, E. E., Shaw, D. S., Silk, J. S., Feng, X., Cohn, J. F., Fox, N. A., & Kovacs, M. (2008). Children's affect expression and frontal EEG asymmetry: Transactional associations with mothers' depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 207221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridlund, A. J. (1992). The behavioral ecology and sociality of human faces. In Clark, M. S. (Ed.), Emotion. Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 90121). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Garber, J., Braafladt, N., & Zeman, J. (1991). The regulation of sad affect: An information-processing perspective. In Garber, J. & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 208240). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gianino, A., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). The mutual regulation model: The infant's self and interactive regulation and coping and defensive capacities. In Field, T. M., McCabe, P. M., & Schneiderman, N. (Ed.), Stress and coping across development (pp. 4768). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gunner, M. R., Porter, F. L., Wolf, C. M., Rigatuso, J., & Larson, M. C. (1995). Neonatal stress reactivity: Predictions to later emotional temperament. Child Development, 66, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastings, P. D., & Grusec, J. E. (1998). Parenting goals as organizers of responses to parent–child disagreement. Developmental Psychology, 34, 465479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript. Yale University, Department of Sociology.Google Scholar
Huebner, R. R., & Izard, C. E. (1988). Mothers' responses to infants' facial expressions of sadness, anger, and physical distress. Motivation and Emotion, 3, 185196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1984). Emotion-cognition relationships and human development. In Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., & Zijonc, R. B. (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 1736). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E., Dougherty, L. M., & Hembree, E. A. (1983). A system for affect expression identification by holistic judgments (Affex). Newark, DE: University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E., Fantauzzo, C. A., Castle, J. M., Haynes, O. M., Rayias, M. F., & Putnam, P. H. (1995). The ontogeny and significance of infants' facial expressions in the first nine months of life. Developmental Psychology, 31, 9971013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E., Haynes, O. M., Chisholm, G., & Baak, K. (1991). Emotional determinants of infant–mother attachment. Child Development, 62, 906917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. S., & Hong, H. W. (2001). Onset of voluntary communication: Smiling looks to mother. Infancy, 2, 353370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. S., & Raag, T. (1989). Smile production in older infants: The importance of a social recipient for the facial signal. Child Development, 60, 811818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G. (1991). Patterns of inhibition to the unfamiliar in children of normal and affectively ill mothers. Child Development, 62, 250263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68, 94112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Tjebkes, T. L., & Forman, D. R. (1998). Children' emerging regulation of conduct: Restraint, compliance, and internalization from infancy to the second year. Child Development, 69, 13781389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczynski, L., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Developmental of children's noncompliance strategies from todderhood to age 5. Development Psychology, 26, 398408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczynski, L., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Girnius-Brown, O. (1987). A developmental interpretation of young children's noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 23, 799806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, M. E., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1981). Individual differences in parental sensitivity. In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 127153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leadbeater, B. J., Bishop, S. J., & Raver, C. C. (1996). Quality of mother–toddler interactions, maternal depressive symptoms, and behavior problems in preschoolers of adolescent mothers. Developmental Psychology, 32, 280288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., & Goldberg, S. (1969). Perceptual–cognitive development in infancy: A generalized expectancy model as a function of the mother–infant interaction. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 15, 81100.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O'Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 561592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundy, B., Field, T., & Pickens, J. (1996). Newborns of mothers with depressive symptoms are less expressive. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 419424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child interaction. In Mussen, P. (Series Ed.) & Mussen, P. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed., pp. 1101). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C. Z., Tesman, J. R., & Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54(1–2, Serial No. 219).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mertesacker, B., Bade, U., Haverkock, A., & Pauli-Pott, U. (2004). Predicting maternal reactivity/sensitivity: The role of infant emotionality, maternal depressiveness/anxiety, and social support. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 4761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wolfson, A., Mumme, D., & Guskin, K. (1995). Helplessness in children of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31, 377387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Canet, P., Livert, G., & Gerardin, P. (2005). Two-month-old infants of depressed mothers show mild, delayed and persistent change in emotional state after non-contingent interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 28, 418425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Developmental Psychology, 35, 12971310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pauli-Pott, U., Mertesacker, B., & Bechman, D. (2004). Predicting the development of infant emotionality from maternal characteristics. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 1942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickens, J., & Field, T. (1993). Facial expressivity in infants of depressed mothers. Developmental Psychology, 29, 986988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redding, R. E., Harmon, R. J., & Morgan, G. A. (1990). Relationships between maternal depression and infant's mastery behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 391395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J. L, & Acevedo, M. C. (2001). Infant reactivity and reliance on mother during emotion challenges: Prediction of cognition and language skills in a low-income sample. Child Development, 72, 402415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of effortful control. In Mayr, U. & Awh, E. (Eds.), Developing individuality in the human brain (pp. 167188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., Schonberg, M., Sherrill, J., Huffman, D., Lukon, J., Obrosky, D., & Kovacs, M. (2006). Responsivity to offspring's expression of emotion among childhood-onset depressed mothers. Journal of Child and Adolescent Clinical Psychology, 35, 490503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silk, J. S., Shaw, D. S., Skuban, E. M., Oland, A. A., & Kovacs, A. A. (2006). Emotion regulation strategies in offspring of childhood-onset depressed mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 6978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenberg, C. R., Campos, J. J., & Emde, R. N. (1983). The facial expression of anger in seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 54, 178184.Google ScholarPubMed
Teti, D. M., Gelfand, D. M., & Pompa, J. (1990). Depressed mothers' behavioral competence with their infants: Demographic and psychosocial correlates. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 259270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E. Z., & Gianino, A. (1986). The transmission of maternal disturbance to the infant. In Tronick, E. Z. & Field, T. (Eds.), Maternal depression and infant disturbance (pp. 6182). San Francisco, CA: Jossey–Bass.Google ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, B. E., Kopp, C. B., & Krakow, J. B. (1984). The emergence and consolidation of self-control from eighteen to thirty months of age: Normative trends and individual differences. Child Development, 55, 9901004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, L. (1993). The parent version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Unpublished manuscript. University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar