Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-dvmhs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-05T20:34:06.500Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Life stress influences the relationship between sex hormone fluctuation and affective symptoms in peripubertal female adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 March 2023

Elizabeth Andersen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Hannah Klusmann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA Division of Clinical Psychological Intervention, Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
Tory Eisenlohr-Moul
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
Kayla Baresich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Susan Girdler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
*
Corresponding author: Elizabeth Andersen, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Female adolescents have a greatly increased risk of depression starting at puberty, which continues throughout the reproductive lifespan. Sex hormone fluctuation has been highlighted as a key proximal precipitating factor in the development of mood disorders tied to reproductive events; however, hormone-induced affective state change is poorly understood in the pubertal transition. The present study investigated the impact of recent stressful life events on the relationship between sex hormone change and affective symptoms in peripubertal female participants. Thirty-five peripubertal participants (ages 11–14, premenarchal, or within 1 year of menarche) completed an assessment of stressful life events, and provided weekly salivary hormone collections [estrone, testosterone, dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA)] and mood assessments for 8 weeks. Linear mixed models tested whether stressful life events provided a context in which within-person changes in hormones predicted weekly affective symptoms. Results indicated that exposure to stressful life events proximal to the pubertal transition influenced the directional effects of hormone change on affective symptoms. Specifically, greater affective symptoms were associated with increases in hormones in a high stress context and decreases in hormones in a low stress context. These findings provide support for stress-related hormone sensitivity as a diathesis for precipitating affective symptoms in the presence of pronounced peripubertal hormone flux.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The pubertal transition (peripuberty) is characterized by nearly a threefold greater risk of a depressive disorder in female adolescents (SAMHSA, 2020). Among other factors, this increased risk may be a result of simultaneous increases in sex hormone fluctuations and interpersonal stressors in female adolescents (Balzer et al., Reference Balzer, Duke, Hawke and Steinbeck2015). The sex disparity first emerges at mid-puberty (Tanner stage 3 or 4) and continues throughout the female reproductive lifespan, suggesting a role of ovarian hormone fluctuations in vulnerability to psychopathology (Angold et al., Reference Angold, Costello, Erkanli and Worthman1999; Angold, Reference Angold1993; Schiller et al., Reference Schiller, Johnson, Abate, Schmidt and Rubinow2016). An abnormal neurobiological sensitivity to normal reproductive hormone flux can trigger affective symptoms in hormone-sensitive individuals. Furthermore, stress exposure interferes with the maturation of neurodevelopmental trajectories (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Sabuwalla and Huot2004) and can predict who is at risk for developing reproductive mood disorders (Bromberger et al., Reference Bromberger, Kravitz, Chang, Cyranowski, Brown and Matthews2011; Namavar Jahromi et al., Reference Namavar Jahromi, Pakmehr and Hagh-Shenas2011). However, the biological mechanisms by which life stress exposure interacts with the peripubertal reproductive endocrine environment to promote depression risk are poorly understood. The present study examines how within-person deviations in sex hormones may predict depressive symptoms in peripubertal female adolescents, particularly in the context of recent stressful life events.

Mechanisms of depression risk during the pubertal transition

Reproductive hormone environment

The pubertal transition is accompanied by extensive changes in ovarian and adrenal hormones (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Fiacco, Gordon, Kozik, Baresich, Rubinow and Girdler2022; Apter et al., Reference Apter, Cacciatore, Alfthan and Stenman1989; Apter, Reference Apter1980), heightened interpersonal stress exposure and a critical window in which neurodevelopmental trajectories are established (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2008). Adrenarche, increased activity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, initiates the developmental hormone cascade with the production of androgens, including dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA). This is followed by reactivation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal (HPG) axis (i.e., gonadarche) and stimulation of sex steroid production, for example estradiol (E2), estrone (E1), and testosterone (T). Moreover, not only absolute concentrations but also the degree of hormone fluctuation increases with the onset of the first menstrual cycle (menarche) (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Fiacco, Gordon, Kozik, Baresich, Rubinow and Girdler2022; Janfaza et al., Reference Janfaza, Sherman, Larmore, Brown-Dawson and Klein2006). The first gynecological year (first year after menarche) is characterized by frequent anovulatory (up to 66%) and irregular cycles that further increase exposure to hormone fluctuation (Carlson & Shaw, Reference Carlson and Shaw2019; Gunn et al., Reference Gunn, Tsai, McRae and Steinbeck2018). This dramatic reproductive endocrine instability may contribute to the abrupt emergence of affective illness during the pubertal transition in female adolescents (Angold, Reference Angold1993; Patton et al., Reference Patton, Olsson, Bond, Toumbourou, Carlin, Hemphill and Catalano2008).

Given the significant regulatory effect of sex hormones on mood and behavior, women are more likely to experience affective dysregulation during reproductive transition events characterized by significant hormonal flux, including premenstrual, postpartum, and perimenopausal periods (Balzer et al., Reference Balzer, Duke, Hawke and Steinbeck2015; Schiller et al., Reference Schiller, Johnson, Abate, Schmidt and Rubinow2016). Hormone manipulation studies simulating reproductive transition events indicate that a vulnerability to normal changes in sex hormones, rather than the absolute level, precipitate the emergence of depressive symptoms in women with a history of reproductive hormone-related mood impairment (Bloch et al., Reference Bloch, Schmidt, Danaceau, Murphy, Nieman and Rubinow2000; Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Girdler, Meltzer-Brody, Stika, Thurston, Clark, Prairie, Moses-Kolko, Joffe and Wisner2015, Reference Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman and Girdler2016; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Nieman, Danaceau, Adams and Rubinow1998, Reference Schmidt, Martinez, Nieman, Koziol, Thompson, Schenkel, Wakim and Rubinow2017). Accordingly, fluctuations in sex hormones such as estrogens (e.g., estradiol, estrone), progesterone and its derivatives, and testosterone have been shown to trigger the development of affective symptoms in a subset of hormone-sensitive individuals across the lifespan (premenstrual: Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Nieman, Danaceau, Adams and Rubinow1998; perinatally: Bloch et al., Reference Bloch, Schmidt, Danaceau, Murphy, Nieman and Rubinow2000; perimenopausal: Sander et al., Reference Sander, Muftah, Sykes Tottenham, Grummisch and Gordon2021; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Dor, Martinez, Guerrieri, Harsh, Thompson, Koziol, Nieman and Rubinow2015; in males: Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Berlin, Danaceau, Neeren, Haq, Roca and Rubinow2004). We recently demonstrated that a large proportion of peripubertal female adolescents showed mood sensitivity to sex hormone flux. Specifically, 57% of participants exhibited increased dysphoric mood with weekly changes (i.e., increase, decrease or large change in either direction from the previous week) in estrone and 37% of participants were mood sensitive to weekly changes in testosterone (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Fiacco, Gordon, Kozik, Baresich, Rubinow and Girdler2022). Our previous findings suggest that peripubertal female adolescents’ mood sensitivity to sex hormone flux can be categorized into four different hormone sensitivity profiles: (a) insensitive (no increase in mood symptoms with weekly change in hormone); (b) withdrawal sensitive (increased mood symptom severity with decreases in hormone change from the previous week); (c) increase sensitive (increased mood severity with increased hormone change); and (d) change sensitive (increased mood severity with large changes in hormone, regardless of direction) (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Fiacco, Gordon, Kozik, Baresich, Rubinow and Girdler2022). The identification of these different hormone sensitivity profiles highlights the importance of assessing within-person hormone variability in determining mood and hormone relationships.

Life stress exposure and HPA axis dysfunction

Along with profound physical developmental changes, the pubertal transition is characterized by elevated life stress exposure (Spear, Reference Spear2009). The impact of life stress is especially relevant during critical developmental windows characterized by dramatic neurodevelopment, including childhood and adolescence (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2008). Exposure to life stress during these early developmental periods has been shown to modify biological stress response systems (i.e., HPA axis) and is associated with sustained alterations in brain function, behavior, and cognition (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). As such, dysregulation of the HPA axis can have long-term consequences and increase the risk for psychological disorders (De Carvalho Tofoli et al., Reference De Carvalho Tofoli, Von Werne Baes, Martins and Juruena2011). The deleterious effect of stress may be even more pronounced for adolescents who experience earlier pubertal maturation relative to their peers (i.e., early pubertal timing) (Hamlat et al., Reference Hamlat, Stange, Abramson and Alloy2014). African American or Black and Latino/Latina adolescents, youth from lower socioeconomic contexts (James-Todd et al., Reference James-Todd, Tehranifar, Rich-Edwards, Titievsky and Terry2010), and those exposed to early-life adversity (Colich & McLaughlin, Reference Colich and McLaughlin2022) are disproportionately more likely to experience early pubertal timing (Colich & McLaughlin, Reference Colich and McLaughlin2022). Therefore, social context and diversity may converge with the unique reproductive endocrine features of puberty to promote greater risk of psychopathology during the pubertal transition (Mendle et al., Reference Mendle, Beltz, Carter and Dorn2019).

Although null findings exist (Meadows et al., Reference Meadows, Brown and Elder2006), a number of studies report increased stress exposure for female compared to male adolescents (Ge et al., Reference Ge, Lorenz, Conger, Elder and Simons1994, Reference Ge, Conger and Elder2001). This applies particularly to interpersonal stress (Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Mermelstein and Roesch2007), possibly due to social gender roles (McLeod & Kessler, Reference McLeod and Kessler1990; Turner et al., Reference Turner, Wheaton and Lloyd1995). Female adolescents also experience increased rates of severe stressful life events, including physical and sexual abuse, which also contributes to long-term modifications in stress reactivity (LeMoult et al., Reference LeMoult, Ordaz, Kircanski, Singh and Gotlib2015, Reference LeMoult, Humphreys, Tracy, Hoffmeister, Ip and Gotlib2020). Exposure to stressful life events is strongly associated with an increased risk of affective disorders, including depression (Hammen, Reference Hammen2005; Monroe & Reid, Reference Monroe and Reid2008; Paykel, Reference Paykel2003). The association of stress exposure and depression has been shown to be stronger in female adolescents compared to male adolescents (Ge et al., Reference Ge, Lorenz, Conger, Elder and Simons1994; Shih et al., Reference Shih, Eberhart, Hammen and Brennan2006). Further, stressful life events proximal to reproductive transition events have been shown to be strong predictors of the emergence of reproductive mood disorders (Bromberger et al., Reference Bromberger, Kravitz, Chang, Cyranowski, Brown and Matthews2011). As such, in initially euthymic perimenopausal women, the number of recent stressful life events in combination with greater estradiol variability over the menopause transition predicted the emergence of depression (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman and Girdler2016). However, this relationship between hormone fluctuation and affective state change is poorly understood during the pubertal transition, which shares many parallels with the menopause transition and is similarly characterized by substantial reproductive hormone flux and heightened life stress.

Aims of the current study

The present study employed a longitudinal, dimensional approach to examine within-person relationships between life stress, weekly changes in sex hormones (estrone, testosterone, DHEA) and affective state change in peripubertal female adolescents. The present study tests a novel diathesis-stress model of adolescent depression in which we propose that life stress modifies the brain’s sensitivity to hormone fluctuation and precipitates vulnerability (i.e., a diathesis) to depressive symptoms in the presence of normal peripubertal hormone flux (i.e., an acute hormonal stressor). The rationale for examining stress-related hormone sensitivity as a diathesis for affective symptoms is twofold. First, sensitivity to hormonal flux during specific reproductive events has been shown to trigger affective symptoms in women who are more susceptible to stress and, second, sex hormones are powerful neuroregulators of frontal and limbic neural networks (Ottowitz et al., Reference Ottowitz, Derro, Dougherty, Lindquist, Fischman and Hall2008) and the HPA stress axis, each of which is implicated in depression (Pandya et al., Reference Pandya, Altinay, Malone and Anand2012). Specifically, the present analysis examines recent life stress as a moderator of the relationship between weekly hormone deviations and specific affective symptom constructs (e.g., interpersonal depressive symptoms, low positive affect, somatic symptoms) (Figure 1). The initial investigation of peripubertal hormone and mood relationships was restricted to general dysphoric mood and did not examine hormone-induced changes in distinct symptom domains (i.e., anxiety, irritability, etc.). This additional decomposition of affective symptoms is warranted given that previously reported hormone-related mood disturbances are symptom and hormone specific (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler2016; Schiller et al., Reference Schiller, Walsh, Eisenlohr-Moul, Prim, Dichter, Schiff, Bizzell, Slightom, Richardson, Belger, Schmidt and Rubinow2022; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Nieman, Danaceau, Adams and Rubinow1998). Consistent with hormone–mood relationships observed in other female reproductive transitions (i.e., premenstrual phase, perimenopause), weekly deviations in hormones (estrone, testosterone, DHEA) in the context of high life stress were expected to be associated with greater depressive symptoms and distress.

Figure 1. Diathesis-stress model of hormone-related mood dysregulation and affective illness. Illustration of the proposed model in which life stress modifies the brain’s sensitivity to hormone fluctuation and precipitates vulnerability (i.e., a diathesis) to depressive symptoms in the presence of normal peripubertal hormone flux (i.e., an acute hormonal stressor).

Method

Participants

Thirty-five participants assigned female at birth, between the ages of 11 and 14 years, and who were undergoing a healthy pubertal transition were recruited from the local community using flyers, mass emails to university staff and students, word-of-mouth, and online posts on middle school parent sites. The protocol was approved by the local Institutional Review Board and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Parents provided written consent and adolescent participants provided written assent to be involved in the study. Participants received a $150.00 Visa gift card for full compliance.

Procedure

The study was conducted between 2018 and 2020 and consisted of three phases, including (a) screening, (b) enrollment, and (c) weekly hormone and mood measurements, described in more detail below and in Andersen et al. (Reference Andersen, Fiacco, Gordon, Kozik, Baresich, Rubinow and Girdler2022). Participants included in the present analysis were a subsample of a larger study (n = 52) who all had weekly (vs. daily) mood assessments and morning (vs. afternoon) salivary hormone collections. The present analysis with this subsample (n = 35) permitted the investigation of life stress and its effect on hormone-related changes in distinct symptom constructs of depression with refined collection parameters.

Screening

Parents of interested participants completed an online screening form to assess eligibility. Participants were recruited based on parental and self-reported pubertal stage (Tanner Stage 3 or 4, breast development and pubic hair growth started, but not complete – as assessed by the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS) at screening and enrollment) and were premenarchal or within 1-year post-menarche. Non-English speakers and participants with psychotic or bipolar disorders, active suicidal ideation, or severe symptoms that would interfere with participation were excluded. To obtain a wide range and adequate variance of depressive symptoms, current self-reported clinical diagnoses of depression or anxiety were permitted if no suicidal ideation and no symptoms of psychosis or mania were reported at enrollment. Participants did not use hormonal agents or contraceptives, or mood-altering medications (e.g., antidepressants). If eligible, the screening was followed by a phone call with the parent to discuss the specifics of the study procedure and confirm eligibility before scheduling the enrollment session.

Enrollment

At enrollment, participants and their parents completed questionnaires assessing personal and family medical history, including self-reported clinical diagnoses, demographic characteristics, mood, stressful life events, and pubertal stage. An abbreviated Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-V (SCID) was administered to screen for psychosis symptoms or bipolar disorder. Height and weight measurements were collected to calculate age-corrected BMI according to Centers for Disease Control and Prevention guidelines, as BMI consistently increases with pubertal maturation (Bini et al., Reference Bini, Celi, Berioli, Bacosi, Stella, Giglio, Tosti and Falorni2000). Parental reports of highest educational attainment and total household income were collected and assessed as indicators of socioeconomic status. Participants and their parents were instructed on best practices for collecting saliva samples and were given clearly labeled collection vials.

Weekly hormone and mood assessments

Following the enrollment session, participants completed eight consecutive weeks of saliva and mood assessments. Using the unstimulated passive drool technique, participants provided 3 mL of saliva into cryovials immediately upon awakening to capture the expected peak hormone levels for 8 consecutive weeks (Hucklebridge et al., Reference Hucklebridge, Hussain, Evans and Clow2005; Janfaza et al., Reference Janfaza, Sherman, Larmore, Brown-Dawson and Klein2006; Kuzawa et al., Reference Kuzawa, Georgiev, McDade, Bechayda and Gettler2016). To prevent contamination and dilution, participants were instructed not to visit the dentist within 2 days before the next collection, refrain from drinking (water included) and brushing their teeth 30 min prior to their collection, and refrain from eating 1 hr before the collection. Saliva samples were immediately placed in the participant’s home freezer before being transferred to a −80°C laboratory freezer every 1–3 weeks. Weekly compliance checks were completed along with the surveys to determine if there were any changes in activity level, diet, sleep, or medication use prior to the saliva collection.

Measures

Enrollment assessments

Pubertal development was self-reported using a combination of the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988) and Tanner line staging, in which participants selected line drawings of breast development and pubic hair growth that best matched their own (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Whincup, Hindmarsh, Lampe, Odoki and Cook2001). Line drawings corresponded to Tanner stage criteria, with 1 indicating no development (“breasts are flat”; “no hair”) and 5 indicating complete development (“only the nipple sticks out beyond the breast”; “hair has spread over the thighs”). Participants rated menarche status, height or growth spurt, breast development, pubic hair growth and appearance of acne on the PDS using a 4-point scale ranging from (a) no development/no menses, (b) barely begun, (c) definitely underway, to (d) completed/menses. The PDS score was calculated as the mean of the five responses. The category PDS score corresponds to the sum of the items pertaining to pubic hair growth, breast development and menarche status, as described previously (Carskadon & Acebo, Reference Carskadon and Acebo1993). Category scores range from 3 prepubertal to 12 postpubertal. PDS and Tanner line staging scores were averaged for breast development and pubic hair growth. Self-reported PDS for female breast and pubic hair development has shown excellent agreement with a physical examination (85% within 1 Tanner stage) (Schmitz et al., Reference Schmitz, Hovell, Nichols, Irvin, Keating, Simon, Gehrman and Jones2004) and good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha for PDS in the present sample was .70).

Recent stressful life events (SLE) were assessed using a modified version of the Coddington Life Events Scale (CLES) (Coddington, Reference Coddington1972). Participants were asked to indicate whether or not they had experienced any of the 25 stressful experiences listed in the measure in the past year by checking “yes” or “no” for each item. Samples items include “death of a family member,” “end of romantic relationship,” “serious personal injury/illness,” “change in acceptance by peers,” “move or change in residence,” and “parents’ divorce.” A total score was calculated from the aggregated number of stressful life events that the participant reported. The number of stressful life events (continuous) was standardized using a z-score.

Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ; Costello & Angold, Reference Costello and Angold1988) is a 33-item self-report inventory to assess adolescent depressive symptoms. Participants rate how they have been feeling or acting over the past 2 weeks by indicating whether each statement is not true (0), somewhat true (1) or mostly true (2) for them. The total score reflects the sum of the 33 items and ranges from 0 to 68, with larger scores reflecting greater depressive symptoms. Cronbach’s alpha for MFQ in the present sample was .96.

Weekly mood assessments

The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC) is a 20-item assessment for child and adolescent depressive symptoms (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Andrews, Lewinsohn and Hops1990). Items are scored 0 (not at all) to 3 (a lot) with four items reverse coded for a range between 0 and 60. Cronbach’s alpha for all 20 items in the present sample was .95. Previous studies on reproductive mood disorders have reported that the psychological relevance of hormone change may be specific to certain symptom domains (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler2016; Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Peltier, Grummisch and Sykes Tottenham2019). As such, the current study employed a dimensional approach to elucidate specific mood and hormone relationships. To refine the investigation of depressive symptom domains, four predefined subscales were computed (reviewed in Shafer, Reference Shafer2006), including interpersonal (two items, α (present sample) = .862), low positive affect (four items, α (present sample) = .852), depressed affect (seven items, α (present sample) = .932), and somatic symptoms (seven items, α (present sample) = .855).

The Perceived Stress Scale (for children) is a 10-item self-report assessment to evaluate perceived coping and distress associated with stressful situations (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Summary scores were calculated after reversing 4 items. Higher scores represent higher subjective stress. Consistent with previous reports, two factors were confirmed in the current sample: a positive-worded factor associated with perceived coping and a negative-worded factor indexing perceived distress (Ezzati et al., Reference Ezzati, Jiang, Katz, Sliwinski, Zimmerman and Lipton2014; Hewitt et al., Reference Hewitt, Flett and Mosher1992) both of which have been found to predict depression symptoms. Correlations among scale items ranged from r = .210 to r = .742, with Cronbach’s α = .896 for all 10 items, α = .780 for the coping factor, and α = .889 for the distress factor.

A subjective stress rating was collected weekly. Each week, participants were asked to indicate whether they experienced “any problems” over the last week by checking a box next to relevant items or filling in their own response. The seven items included in the list were: (a) performed poorly on an exam or assignment, (b) received a bad report card, (c) felt overbooked with extracurricular activities, (d) got in a fight with a friend(s), (e) didn’t get along with parents, (f) experienced bullying, (g) didn’t get along with siblings, and (h) other. They were also asked to enter the most stressful event that happened each week and how stressed it made them feel on a sliding bar from 1 (not very stressful) to 10 (extremely stressful).

Salivary hormone collection and analysis

Salivary samples were assayed at ZRT Laboratory (Beaverton, OR) using liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) to achieve the most sensitive and accurate quantification of salivary hormones. Average inter-assay precision was 7.27% for estrone, 9.77% for testosterone, and 7.03% for DHEA, with the average intra-assay coefficients of variance as follows: 9.47% for estrone, 4.20% for testosterone, and 7.27% for DHEA. Minimum detection limits were 0.4 pg/mL for estrone, 3.2 pg/mL for testosterone, and 17.1 pg/mL for DHEA. Measurements that fell below assay sensitivity were assigned a value that was one-half the limit of detection. Estrone was selected over estradiol (E2) for the present analyses because estrone rises before E2 during the pubertal transition (Biro et al., Reference Biro, Pinney, Huang, Baker, Walt Chandler and Dorn2014) allowing for easier detection and has been shown to be highly correlated with E2 in previous reports (r = 0.80, p < .0001) (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Fiacco, Gordon, Kozik, Baresich, Rubinow and Girdler2022). Lower concentrations of E2 are expected in peripubertal females – especially premenarche (Janfaza et al., Reference Janfaza, Sherman, Larmore, Brown-Dawson and Klein2006).

Analytic plan

Data coding and preparation

Analyses were performed in SAS On Demand for Academics. Hormone measurements were centered within-person (CWP) to extract within-person hormone variance [i.e., (hormone level at this sample) – (participant’s average hormone level across all samples)]. As such, greater values of hormone CWP reflect increased levels relative to a participant’s individual mean (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, DeWall, Girdler and Segerstrom2015).

Weekly hormone and mood relationships

Separate multilevel models were performed to predict each affective symptom outcome (CES-DC-depressed, CES-DC-interpersonal, CES-DC-low positive affect, CES-DC-somatic, PSS-coping, PSS-distress) from weekly deviations from an individual’s average hormone level (DHEA, testosterone, estrone), number of stressful life events (SLE, sample standardized), and all associated interactions. “Week” was specified as a repeated statement to account for serial correlation of weekly hormone and mood assessments, and a random slope within-person was applied with a variance component structure. Null multilevel models (without predictors) were used to calculate the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for each predictor and outcome variable (see Table 1). Given the small sample size, restricted maximum likelihood estimation (REML) was implemented to account for potential biases in random-effect variances and the Kenward–Roger correction was used to address biases in standard error estimation (McNeish & Matta, Reference McNeish and Matta2018). Age, race, parental education (as a proxy for socioeconomic status), and BMI did not significantly contribute to the models and were therefore excluded from final analyses.

Table 1. Demographic and sample characteristics

Note. Means are presented ± standard deviation. BMI: body mass index; LGBTQIA+: lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer, intersex, asexual, and more; PDS: Pubertal Development Scale average category score (averaged PDS and tanner line staging score for breast development and pubic hair growth), Primary parent completed education level; CES-DC: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children; PSS: Perceived Stress Scale; DHEA: dehydroepiandrosterone.

The effect of stress was further probed by calculating the simple slopes of hormone (CWP) at various levels of stress for each combination of symptom and hormone pair. The simple slopes were calculated from three levels of SLE: mean SLE = 5.029, mean SLE + standard deviation (SD) = 7.578, and mean SLE − SD = 2.479.

Power analysis

With 35 participants that completed CES-DC ratings and hormone collections, the current study had 86% power with α = .05 to detect small effects (f = .15) of within-person weekly hormone and mood associations (8 repeated measures with .50 correlation).

Results

Descriptive information

Demographic and sample characteristics are presented in Table 1. Thirty-five participants (mean age = 12.48, SD = 1.01) provided weekly mood ratings and salivary hormone samples. The majority of participants identified as White (74.3%), with 11.4% identifying as multiracial, 11.4% as Hispanic/Latino or Spanish, and 2.9% Black. Participants’ parents were highly educated (graduated or postgraduate) and were affluent middle to upper class, representative of the local community. Participants were self-reported mid-puberty (PDS category score: mean = 8.33, SD = 2.30), with 51.4% (18/35) of the sample pre-menarche and 48.6% (17/35) of participants post-menarche within 1 year. Pictorial rating of breast (mean = 3.23, SD = 0.69) and pubic hair development (mean = 2.94, SD = 0.87) subjectively confirmed peripubertal status. One participant identified as male. Pubertal status, BMI, MFQ, number of stressful life events and average estrone, testosterone and DHEA levels did not differ by race or ethnicity, nor were these variables associated with parental education (as a proxy for socioeconomic status). Pubertal status was correlated with BMI (r s = .484, p = .004) and associated with greater estrone (r s = .535, p = .001), testosterone (r s = .477, p = .004), and DHEA (r s = .398, p = .018) levels.

The frequency of stressful life events ranged from 2 to 11 events, with an average of 5.03 (SD = 2.55). As expected, there was a wide range of depressive symptom severity reported on the MFQ, ranging from 0 to 53 [mean = 14.43, SD = 13.31, a score of 29 or above is considered clinically significant (Burleson Daviss et al., Reference Burleson Daviss, Birmaher, Melhem, Axelson, Michaels and Brent2006)], with increased symptom severity associated with a greater number of stressful life events (r s = .385, p = .022). Further, more stressful life events were related to higher DHEA levels (r s = .380, p = .024). The most frequently experienced weekly stress was associated with academic performance (26.84%), followed by conflicts with parents (16.67%) and “other” (16.67%). Descriptions of “other” included being excluded from a sleepover, feeling jealous, not getting enough sleep, getting braces, a sick pet, etc. There were no major life events reported during the 8-week collection period.

Study adherence was high, with only 13 weekly surveys missed (4.6%, 267/280) and 5 saliva samples missed (1.8%, 275/280) across all participants. Estrone and testosterone were below the limit of detection in 4% (E1:12/275; T:10/275) of the samples, and DHEA was undetectable in 2% (6/275) of the samples collected.

Intraclass correlations of repeated hormones and outcomes

Intraclass correlations indicated that the majority of the variance in symptoms could be attributed to stable between-person differences (trait-like; ICCs between 0.58 and 0.77). Variability in estrone was primarily at the within-person level (ICC = 0.36), whereas 64% of testosterone and 54% of DHEA variance was trait-like (Table 1).

Interactive effects of stressful life events and hormone change on symptoms

Results of models examining the moderating role of stressful life events on the association between within-person deviations in hormones (CWP) and symptom expression for all outcomes are reported in Table 2. P-values presented in Table 2 are FDR corrected and Cohen’s f 2 was calculated for mixed-effects models using recommendations proposed by Selya et al. (Reference Selya, Rose, Dierker, Hedeker and Mermelstein2012).

Table 2. Fixed effects of stressful life events, within-person hormone deviations, and their interactions on symptoms

Note. Estimates are presented with standard error (SE) and p value. SLE: stressful life events; zSLE: standardized stressful life events score; CWP: Centered Within Person; E1: estrone; T: testosterone; DHEA: dehydroepiandrosterone; CES-DC: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children; PSS: perceived stress scale. *FDR adjusted p-values.

Stressful life events and within-person weekly hormone deviations

Estrone

Stressful life events interacted with estrone CWP to predict somatic (F(1,230) = 6.32, p = .013), interpersonal (F(1,230) = 6.78, p = .01), and depressed affect (F(1,230) = 5.62, p = .019) symptoms. Specifically, within-person elevations of estrone predicted greater somatic (t(230.2) = 2.67, p = 0.008) and interpersonal (t(230.1) = 2.31, p = 0.022) symptoms in the context of high stress (SLE + SD), and lower-than-usual estrone predicted greater depressed affect (t(230.1) = −2.14, p = 0.033) at low stress (SLE − SD).

Testosterone

Within-person deviations in testosterone interacted with stressful life events to predict somatic (F(1,230) = 9.16, p = .003), depressed affect (F(1,230) = 11.81, p = .001), and perceived distress (F(1,230) = 15.74, p < .001) symptoms. Probing the impact of stress revealed differential effects of within-person deviations in testosterone on somatic symptoms and perceived distress, with higher-than-usual testosterone predicting greater symptoms at high stress (somatic: t(230.2) = 2.65, p = 0.009; distress: t(230.2) = 3.04, p = 0.003), and lower-than-usual testosterone predicting greater symptoms at low stress (somatic: t(230.1) = −2.11, p = 0.036; distress: t(230.2) = −3.12, p = 0.002) (Figure 2b and c). Additionally, low (t(230.1) = −4.05, p < 0.001) to average (t(230.1) = −2.72, p = 0.007) stress was associated with greater depressed affect following lower-than-usual testosterone.

Figure 2. Stress unmasks differential effects of within-person hormone deviations on affective symptoms. The stress context created a divergence in DHEA-related depressed affect (a), testosterone-related somatic symptoms (b), and perceived distress (c). Stressful life events are presented as a gradient (with a low number of events indicated by yellow and a high number of events represented by dark blue colors). The regression lines represent tertile groups with 90% confidence intervals. DHEA: dehydroepiandrosterone, CES-DC: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children, PSS: perceived stress scale.

DHEA

DHEA CWP interacted with stressful life events to predict somatic (F(1,230) = 8.88, p = .003), low positive affect (F(1,230) = 5.05, p = .026), depressed affect (F(1,230) = 7.90, p = .005), interpersonal symptoms (F(1,230) = 6.75, p = .010), and perceived distress (F(1,230) = 6.02, p = .015). A similar pattern of stress–hormone interactions was found for DHEA, with higher-than-usual DHEA predicting greater depressed affect symptoms at high stress (t(230.3) = 2.04, p = 0.042), and lower-than-usual DHEA predicting greater symptoms at low stress (t(230.1) = −1.98, p = 0.049) (Figure 2a). At high stress, within-person elevations in DHEA predicted increased symptom ratings for low positive affect (t(230.3) = 2.31, p = 0.022), interpersonal symptoms (t(230.3) = 3.00, p = 0.003), perceived distress (t(230.5) = 2.84, p = 0.005), and somatic symptoms (t(230.3) = 4.43, p < 0.001; average: t(230.1) = 2.83, p = 0.005).

Discussion

Summary of results

The present study used a dimensional approach with weekly measures of sex hormones and depressive symptom ratings to determine the role of recent stressful life events in modifying the relationship between hormone flux and depressive symptoms in peripubertal females. Weekly hormone (estrone, testosterone, and DHEA) deviations predicted differential changes in key constructs of adolescent depression and perceived stress. Results indicated that stress exposure provides a context that unmasks directional effects of hormone flux on affect state change in peripubertal females. Specifically, a consistent pattern emerged with within-person elevations in hormones predicting greater affective symptoms in high stress and decreases in hormones predicting greater symptom severity in low stress contexts. Stress-related hormone effects on affective symptoms appear to be hormone-specific. Namely, DHEA interacted with stressful life events to broadly impact symptom expression, whereas estrone and testosterone had more restricted effects on affective symptoms. The present study supports a diathesis-stress model of adolescent depression in which exposure to life stress proximal to the pubertal transition modifies vulnerable neural networks, making them more sensitive to normal peripubertal hormone flux, which in turn, promotes the emergence of adolescent mood dysregulation and affective illness.

DHEA deviations predicted a range of depressive symptoms

DHEA interacted with stressful life events to predict all symptom outcomes except perceived coping ability, and higher levels of DHEA were associated with a greater number of stressful life events. DHEA and cortisol are both products of the HPA stress axis that begin to rise with adrenarche and progressively increase during the pubertal transition and exert opposing physiological actions (Kamin & Kertes, Reference Kamin and Kertes2017). Because of DHEA’s reported antagonistic effects on glucocorticoids (i.e., cortisol), the preferential production of DHEA over cortisol is thought to buffer against stress-related mood impairment (Karishma & Herbert, Reference Karishma and Herbert2002). At low stress, elevated DHEA in the present study predicted less severe symptom ratings, particularly depressed affect. This is consistent with previous reports that DHEA has antidepressant-like effects, with administration of DHEA improving depressive symptoms by over 50% in patients with midlife-onset depression (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Daly, Bloch, Smith, Danaceau, Simpson St. Clair, Murphy, Haq and Rubinow2005). More recently, DHEA administration was found to reduce amygdala and hippocampus engagement and enhance limbic connectivity during an emotion regulation paradigm, which in turn, was associated with improved negative effect (Sripada et al., Reference Sripada, Marx, King, Rajaram, Garfinkel, Abelson and Liberzon2013). However, at high stress in the present analyses, higher-than-usual DHEA predicted greater symptom expression, including somatic and interpersonal symptoms, depressed and low positive affect, and perceived distress. The greater symptom expression with DHEA elevations in the high stress context resembles the higher DHEA levels in response to acute stress (Dutheil et al., Reference Dutheil, de Saint Vincent, Pereira, Schmidt, Moustafa, Charkhabi, Bouillon-Minois and Clinchamps2021), also observed among adolescents with affective symptoms (Shirtcliff et al., Reference Shirtcliff, Zahn-Waxler, Klimes-Dougan and Slattery2007). Given DHEA’s antiglucocorticoid properties, DHEA may be protective against the deleterious effects of cortisol during the pubertal transition, which is characterized by heightened stress exposure (Flinn et al., Reference Flinn, Nepomnaschy, Muehlenbein and Ponzi2011). DHEA relative to other hormones, including cortisol, could be investigated in future studies to advance our understanding of stress-related mood sensitivity to peripubertal DHEA changes.

Estrone deviations predicted depressed affect, somatic and interpersonal symptoms of depression

The interaction of estrone and stressful life events predicted somatic symptoms, depressed affect, and interpersonal depression symptoms. When probing the effects of different levels of stress, estrone surges were associated with increased interpersonal and somatic symptoms at high stress and decreased depressed affect at low stress. The relevance of estrogen fluctuations and interpersonal symptoms has also been reported in the menopausal transition (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman and Girdler2016), similarly characterized by substantial ovarian hormone flux and heightened stress exposure. In fact, estradiol variability was found to predict greater sensitivity to rejection during psychosocial stress in perimenopausal women. Moreover, in women who had experienced a greater number of recent stressful life events, rejection sensitivity predicted the emergence of depression symptoms one year later (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman and Girdler2016). Importantly, rejection sensitivity is a proximal risk factor for clinical depression and transdiagnostic factor shared across psychopathologies (Kupferberg et al., Reference Kupferberg, Bicks and Hasler2016; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Helms, Rudolph, Hastings, Nock and Prinstein2018; Slavich & Irwin, Reference Slavich and Irwin2014, Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, O’donovan, Epel and Kemeny2010). The results of the present study further support the pathophysiologic relevance of estrogens, specifically estrone, in the interpersonal symptoms of depression, and for the first time, demonstrated a relationship between estrone change and increased interpersonal depressive symptoms during the pubertal transition. The specificity of symptoms highlights the importance of conducting dimensional, symptom-specific research.

Divergent effect of testosterone-related affect dysregulation in the context of stress

The stress context created a divergence in testosterone-related affective symptoms, particularly for somatic symptoms and perceived distress. Participants exhibited increased symptoms with lower-than-usual testosterone levels at low stress, yet testosterone elevations were associated with increased symptoms at high stress. The role of testosterone in depression has been investigated particularly in regard to pharmacological use (with testosterone treatment showing antidepressant effects) and primarily in male samples (Zarrouf et al., Reference Zarrouf, Artz, Griffith, Sirbu and Kommor2009). In adolescents, and especially female adolescents, the relationship between depression and testosterone is understudied. One previous longitudinal study investigated 630 female participants annually through adolescence for up to 7 years (Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Worthman, Shanahan, Costello and Angold2019). Higher serum testosterone concentrations predicted diagnoses of depressive disorders significantly, even after controlling for factors such as pubertal status or age. Moreover, testosterone has been shown to have puberty-unique influences on neurofunction and neuroarchitecture in brain regions associated with the development of affective disorders (Bramen et al., Reference Bramen, Hranilovich, Dahl, Chen, Rosso, Forbes, Dinov, Worthman and Sowell2012). We recently demonstrated that a significant proportion of female adolescents showed meaningful associations between fluctuations of testosterone and mood (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Fiacco, Gordon, Kozik, Baresich, Rubinow and Girdler2022). The results reported here take these investigations one step further and highlight the essential role of stressful life events on the testosterone–mood relationship. However, the biological mechanisms contributing to the interaction of life stress and mood sensitivity to testosterone flux have yet to be identified. Research on other sex steroids, such as progesterone, suggest that dysregulated receptor functioning can cause hormone sensitivity and HPA axis dysregulation in some individuals (as reviewed for progesterone by Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Girdler, Meltzer-Brody, Stika, Thurston, Clark, Prairie, Moses-Kolko, Joffe and Wisner2015). Future research could explore this as a potential mechanism underlying sensitivity to testosterone flux.

The impact of stressful life events on hormone-induced affective symptoms

The results not only support the interaction of sex hormones and mood in the pubertal transition, but they also suggest that life stress exposure has an essential moderating effect on this interaction. One theory suggests that this moderation is biologically underpinned by dysregulation of the HPA axis. Early and chronic life stress can alter HPA axis functioning with long-term effects on mental health (Ho & King, Reference Ho and King2021). These HPA axis alterations have been specifically demonstrated for severe forms of life stress in the form of trauma exposure (Steudte-Schmiedgen et al., Reference Steudte-Schmiedgen, Kirschbaum, Alexander and Stalder2016) and for life stress experienced early in life, which is especially relevant to adolescents (Young et al., Reference Young, Doom, Farrell, Carlson, Englund, Miller, Gunnar, Roisman and Simpson2021). The pubertal transition is a unique window of vulnerability to the impact of stress-related mood impairment and contributes to differential stress responses following early life stress (Flannery et al., Reference Flannery, Gabard-Durnam, Shapiro, Goff, Caldera, Louie, Gee, Telzer, Humphreys, Lumian and Tottenham2017; King et al., Reference King, Colich, LeMoult, Humphreys, Ordaz, Price and Gotlib2017; Nelson & Gabard-Durnam, Reference Nelson and Gabard-Durnam2020; Sisk & Gee, Reference Sisk and Gee2022). The HPA and HPG axes interact on various levels to regulate sex steroids, and in turn, sex steroids modulate HPA function (Oyola & Handa, Reference Oyola and Handa2017). Changes in HPA activity during adolescence [possibly through estrogen modifications of the GABAergic system in the amygdala (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Sabuwalla and Huot2004)] may contribute to hormonal sensitivity and precipitate depressive symptoms in susceptible individuals. Results from the present study suggest that experiencing a greater number of stressful life events proximal to the pubertal transition can modify hormone–mood relationships. However, the positive relationship between stressful life events and depressive symptoms at enrollment in the present study suggest that greater depressive symptoms at enrollment could have influenced the report of stressful life events, particularly stressful events that are more subjective (e.g., change in acceptance by peers). Consequently, it remains unclear whether the type or severity of stressor within the context of baseline depressive symptoms differentially impacts hormone-related mood dysregulation.

In summary, early life stress, via modifications of developing neural circuitry, may give rise to hormone sensitivity, thus enhancing vulnerability for affective symptoms in the context of normal peripubertal hormone fluctuations. Stress-related alterations of HPA functioning and the strong interaction of HPA and HPG axes may underlie the relationship between sex hormone fluctuation and mood. However, the neurophysiological mechanisms supporting this relationship are poorly understood and remains a promising pursuit of future research.

Limitations

Despite the strengths of the longitudinal, dimensional approach employed in the current study, there are several limitations to address. While serum hormone measurement is typically preferred to saliva methods because of its greater detection sensitivity, salivary hormone collections offer a noninvasive alternative with superior feasibility and compliance in adolescent participants. Given the current study’s objective to determine the relationship between hormone change and mood does not require the ability to detect the absolute lowest hormone level, the trade-off between ultra-low sensitivity and feasibility is appropriate. To account for potential confounds in saliva hormone measurement (time of day, food and water intake, etc.), we provided participants and their parents with detailed instructions and required sample collection to occur first thing upon awakening. Furthermore, steroid concentrations measured in saliva provide integrative information on free, unbound estradiol and testosterone and thereby measure the biologically active component of the hormone (Gao et al., Reference Gao, Stalder and Kirschbaum2015; Yasuda et al., Reference Yasuda, Honma, Furuya, Yoshii, Kamiyama, Ide, Muto and Horie2008). The present study is also limited by the observation interval, which occurred weekly, and the relatively small sample size to examine moderation effects. While weekly observations may be sufficient to demonstrate how hormone flux can influence depressive symptoms from week to week (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman and Girdler2016, Reference Gordon, Sander, Eisenlohr-Moul and Sykes Tottenham2020; Lozza-Fiacco et al., Reference Lozza-Fiacco, Gordon, Andersen, Kozik, Neely, Schiller, Munoz, Rubinow and Girdler2022), daily measures may more precisely asses hormone flux and should be considered in future studies.

For the same reasons that make the pubertal transition a critical developmental window for examining the impact of hormones on mental illness trajectories, the peripubertal endocrine environment is associated with unique study design challenges. It is difficult to study specific hormones of interest, particularly progesterone and its metabolites, because of the limited sensitivity of the available analysis methods for the relatively low levels expected during the first gynecological year. While valuable guidelines exist for determining ovulation status and cycle phasing in adult participants (Schmalenberger et al., Reference Schmalenberger, Tauseef, Barone, Owens, Lieberman, Jarczok, Girdler, Kiesner, Ditzen and Eisenlohr-Moul2021), these recommendations have only limited utility for identifying menstrual cycle phase and ovulation status in peripubertal participants, in which anovulatory and irregular cycles are common (Carlson & Shaw, Reference Carlson and Shaw2019; Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Deuja and Saha2016). Typical hormone thresholds (i.e., progesterone rise, LH peak) for determining cycle phase in adults are not applicable because of overall lower peripubertal hormone concentrations, and counting methods are not relevant when shorter luteal phases are expected (Carlson & Shaw, Reference Carlson and Shaw2019; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Kangarloo, Adams, Sluss, Welt, Chandler, Zava, McGrath, Umbach, Hall and Shaw2019). Currently, there are no clear guidelines for determining ovulation status and cycle phasing during the first gynecological year, and thus, the impact of the menstrual cycle on hormone–mood relationships during the pubertal transition remains an important area of investigation.

Future directions

Future research is needed to examine the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the relationship between hormone change, stress and depressive symptoms. Because sex hormones (e.g., estradiol, testosterone) influence the regulation of the HPA stress system and the abundance of sex hormone receptors in limbic brain regions engaged during emotion, the probability exists that the HPA stress response may mediate the hormone–mood relationship. Moreover, the biological mechanisms supporting the relationship between psychopathology and sex hormones (i.e., DHEA and testosterone) and their fluctuation are poorly understood – not only in adolescents but across the female reproductive lifespan and should be a focus of future studies to close these existing research gaps.

The results from the present study confirm and further highlight the need to include stress exposure in studies linking sex hormones and affective symptoms. As shown in trauma research (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Lockwood, Phelps, Wade, Creamer, Bryant, McFarlane, Silove, Rees, Chapman, Slade, Mills, Teesson and O†Donnell2013; López-Martínez et al., Reference López-Martínez, Serrano-Ibáñez, Ruiz-Párraga, Gómez-Pérez, Ramírez-Maestre and Esteve2018), not only the absolute number of stressful life events, but also the type of stressor is a promising factor to investigate (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Humphreys, Belsky and Ellis2021). Results also highlight the importance of using a dimensional approach. Limiting the investigation to total depression symptom scores could dilute or mask significant effects only apparent on the symptom level. Accordingly, we recommend future studies similarly employ a dimensional and symptom-specific approach to investigate key constructs of depression in adolescents. This dimensional approach will also allow more specific exploration of the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the etiology of affective illness. Finally, the current results provide a foundation for understanding the impact of life stress on hormone–mood relationships in a low-risk sample reflecting the socioeconomic, racial, and ethnic composition of the local community. A larger and more diverse sample of adolescents could, for instance, be employed to elucidate unique psychosocial and neuroendocrine features of the pubertal transition experienced by participants who identify differently from their sex assigned at birth. Including a wide range of depressive symptoms was not only important to achieve adequate variability, but it also allowed for greater generalizability of a developmental window in which elevated stress and depression are common. That said, the diathesis model tested in the present analyses could inform future investigations of the biological, social, environmental, and cultural factors that converge to make the pubertal transition a unique window of vulnerability for psychopathology.

Conclusion

The present study contributes to our understanding of peripubertal hormone–mood relationships by demonstrating that life stress differentially impacts hormone-related changes in distinct symptom constructs of depression. This study reveals how life stress predisposes sensitivity to hormone fluctuation in peripubertal female adolescents, ultimately resulting in depressive symptoms in response to normative hormone flux. Results indicated that sensitivity to within-person deviations in DHEA, testosterone, and estrone predict affective symptoms, and that mood sensitivity to weekly hormone deviations is stress context specific. Given the role of sex hormones in regulating neural networks involved in cognitive and emotional processing, the combination of increased sex hormones and their variability during the pubertal transition and more frequent stress exposure may promote heightened emotional reactivity and arousal and thus promote greater risk for psychopathology. The impact of stressful life events on the unique peripubertal neuroendocrine environment should continue to be investigated to inform advancements in the early detection and intervention of affective illness.

Funding statement

This study was supported by the NIMH Grant K01MH121575 (to EA), the NIH T32 postdoctoral fellowship MH093315 (to EA), the NIH Clinical Translational Science Award pilot grant UL1TR002489 (to EA), and a Foundation of Hope for Research and Treatment of Mental Illness grant (to EA). The “Stiftung der Deutschen Wirtschaft” provided doctoral funding for HK.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Footnotes

Elizabeth Andersen and Hannah Klusmann are shared first authors.

References

Andersen, E., Fiacco, S., Gordon, J., Kozik, R., Baresich, K., Rubinow, D., & Girdler, S. (2022). Methods for characterizing ovarian and adrenal hormone variability and mood relationships in peripubertal females. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 141, 1547. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105747 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angold, A. (1993). Puberty onset of gender differences in rates of depression: A developmental, epidemiologic and neuroendocrine perspective. Journal of Affective Disorders, 29(2-3), 145158. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0327(93)90029-J CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Erkanli, A., & Worthman, C. M. (1999). Pubertal changes in hormone levels and depression in girls. Psychological Medicine, 29(5), 10431053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apter, D. (1980). Serum steroids and pituitary hormones in female puberty: A partly longitudinal study. Clinical Endocrinology, 12(2), 107120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2265.1980.tb02125.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apter, D., Cacciatore, B., Alfthan, H., & Stenman, U. (1989). Serum luteinizing hormone concentrations increase 100-fold in females from 7 years of age to adulthood, as measured by time-resolved immunofluorometric assay. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 68(1), 5357. https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem-68-1-53 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balzer, B. W. R., Duke, S. A., Hawke, C. I., & Steinbeck, K. S. (2015). The effects of estradiol on mood and behavior in human female adolescents: A systematic review. European Journal of Pediatrics, 174(3), 289298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-014-2475-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bini, V., Celi, F., Berioli, M. G., Bacosi, M. L., Stella, P., Giglio, P., Tosti, L., & Falorni, A. (2000). Body mass index in children and adolescents according to age and pubertal stage. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 54(3), 214218, https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600922,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biro, F. M., Pinney, S. M., Huang, B., Baker, E. R., Walt Chandler, D., & Dorn, L. D. (2014). Hormone changes in peripubertal girls. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 99(10), 38293835. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2013-4528 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 267277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2353 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloch, M., Schmidt, P. J., Danaceau, M. A., Murphy, J., Nieman, L. K., & Rubinow, D. R. (2000). Effects of gonadal steroids in women with a history of postpartum depression. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(6), 924930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramen, J. E., Hranilovich, J. A., Dahl, R. E., Chen, J., Rosso, C., Forbes, E. E., Dinov, I. D., Worthman, C. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2012). Sex matters during adolescence: Testosterone-related cortical thickness maturation differs between boys and girls. PLoS ONE, 7(3), e33850. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033850 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromberger, J. T., Kravitz, H. M., Chang, Y. F., Cyranowski, J. M., Brown, C., & Matthews, K. A. (2011). Major depression during and after the menopausal transition: Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation (SWAN). Psychological Medicine, 41(9), 18791888. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171100016X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burleson Daviss, W., Birmaher, B., Melhem, N. A., Axelson, D. A., Michaels, S. M., & Brent, D. A. (2006). Criterion validity of the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire for depressive episodes in clinic and non-clinic subjects: Criterion validity of Mood and Feelings Questionnaire. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(9), 927934. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01646.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, L. J., & Shaw, N. D. (2019). Development of ovulatory menstrual cycles in adolescent girls. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 32(3), 249253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpag.2019.02.119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carskadon, M. A., & Acebo, C. (1993). A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. Journal of Adolescent Health, 14(3), 190195. https://doi.org/10.1016/1054-139X(93)90004-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coddington, R. D. (1972). The significance of life events as etiologic factors in the diseases of children. II. A study of a normal population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 16(3), 205213.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colich, N. L., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2022). Accelerated pubertal development as a mechanism linking trauma exposure with depression and anxiety in adolescence. Current Opinion in Psychology, 46, 101338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101338 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Worthman, C., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (2019). Early pubertal timing and testosterone associated with higher levels of adolescent depression in girls. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(12), 11971206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (1988). Scales to assess child and adolescent depression: Checklists, screens, and nets. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(6), 726737. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-198811000-00011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Carvalho Tofoli, S. M., Von Werne Baes, C., Martins, C. M. S., & Juruena, M. (2011). Early life stress, HPA axis, and depression. Psychology & Neuroscience, 4(2), 229234. https://doi.org/10.3922/j.psns.2011.2.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutheil, F., de Saint Vincent, S., Pereira, B., Schmidt, J., Moustafa, F., Charkhabi, M., Bouillon-Minois, J. B., & Clinchamps, M. (2021). DHEA as a biomarker of stress: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 688367. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.688367 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., DeWall, C. N., Girdler, S. S., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2015). Ovarian hormones and borderline personality disorder features: Preliminary evidence for interactive effects of estradiol and progesterone. Biological Psychology, 109, 3752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Rubinow, D. R., Schiller, C. E., Johnson, J. L., Leserman, J., & Girdler, S. S. (2016). Histories of abuse predict stronger within-person covariation of ovarian steroids and mood symptoms in women with menstrually related mood disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 67, 142152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ezzati, A., Jiang, J., Katz, M. J., Sliwinski, M. J., Zimmerman, M. E., & Lipton, R. B. (2014). Validation of the Perceived Stress Scale in a community sample of older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29(6), 645652. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4049 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flannery, J. E., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Shapiro, M., Goff, B., Caldera, C., Louie, J., Gee, D. G., Telzer, E. H., Humphreys, K. L., Lumian, D. S., & Tottenham, N. (2017). Diurnal cortisol after early institutional care—Age matters. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 160166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.03.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flinn, M. V., Nepomnaschy, P. A., Muehlenbein, M. P., & Ponzi, D. (2011). Evolutionary functions of early social modulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis development in humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(7), 16111629. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, D., Lockwood, E., Phelps, A., Wade, D., Creamer, M., Bryant, R. A., McFarlane, A., Silove, D., Rees, S., Chapman, C., Slade, T., Mills, K., Teesson, M., & O†Donnell, M. (2013). Trauma at the hands of another: Distinguishing PTSD patterns following intimate and nonintimate interpersonal and noninterpersonal trauma in a nationally representative sample. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74(2), 21205. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.13m08374 Google Scholar
Gao, W., Stalder, T., & Kirschbaum, C. (2015). Quantitative analysis of estradiol and six other steroid hormones in human saliva using a high throughput liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry assay. Talanta, 143, 353358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.talanta.2015.05.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, X., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (2001). Pubertal transition, stressful life events, and the emergence of gender differences in adolescent depressive symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 37(3), 404417. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, X., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., & Simons, R. L. (1994). Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30(4), 467483, https://doi-org.libproxy.lib.unc.edu/10.1037/0012-1649.30.4.467 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, J. L., Girdler, S. S., Meltzer-Brody, S. E., Stika, C. S., Thurston, R. C., Clark, C. T., Prairie, B. A., Moses-Kolko, E., Joffe, H., & Wisner, K. L. (2015). Ovarian hormone fluctuation, neurosteroids, and HPA axis dysregulation in perimenopausal depression: A novel heuristic model. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(3), 227236. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14070918 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, J. L., Peltier, A., Grummisch, J. A., & Sykes Tottenham, L. (2019). Estradiol fluctuation, sensitivity to stress, and depressive symptoms in the menopause transition: A pilot study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1319. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01319 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, J. L., Rubinow, D. R., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Leserman, J., & Girdler, S. S. (2016). Estradiol variability, stressful life events, and the emergence of depressive symptomatology during the menopausal transition. Menopause, 23(3), 257266. https://doi.org/10.1097/GME.0000000000000528 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, J. L., Sander, B., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., & Sykes Tottenham, L. (2020). Mood sensitivity to estradiol predicts depressive symptoms in the menopause transition. Psychological Medicine, 51(10), 17331741. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000483 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunn, H. M., Tsai, M. C., McRae, A., & Steinbeck, K. S. (2018). Menstrual patterns in the first gynecological year: A systematic review. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 31(6), 557565.e6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpag.2018.07.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlat, E. J., Stange, J. P., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2014). Early pubertal timing as a vulnerability to depression symptoms: Differential effects of race and sex. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(4), 527538. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9798-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1(1), 293319. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, B. L., Mermelstein, R., & Roesch, L. (2007). Sex differences in adolescent depression: Stress exposure and reactivity models. Child Development, 78(1), 279295. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Mosher, S. W. (1992). The Perceived Stress Scale: Factor structure and relation to depression symptoms in a psychiatric sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 14(3), 247257. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00962631 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, T. C., & King, L. S. (2021). Mechanisms of neuroplasticity linking early adversity to depression: Developmental considerations. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 517. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01639-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hucklebridge, F., Hussain, T., Evans, P., & Clow, A. (2005). The diurnal patterns of the adrenal steroids cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) in relation to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(1), 5157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.04.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James-Todd, T., Tehranifar, P., Rich-Edwards, J., Titievsky, L., & Terry, M. B. (2010). The impact of socioeconomic status across early life on age at menarche among a racially diverse population of girls. Annals of Epidemiology, 20(11), 836842. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janfaza, M., Sherman, T. I., Larmore, K. A., Brown-Dawson, J., & Klein, K. O. (2006). Estradiol levels and secretory dynamics in normal girls and boys as determined by an ultrasensitive bioassay: A 10 year experience. Journal of Pediatric Endocrinology and Metabolism, 19(7), 901910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamin, H. S., & Kertes, D. A. (2017). Cortisol and DHEA in development and psychopathology. Hormones and Behavior, 89, 6985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.11.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karishma, K. K., & Herbert, J. (2002). Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) stimulates neurogenesis in the hippocampus of the rat, promotes survival of newly formed neurons and prevents corticosterone-induced suppression: DHEA in hippocampus. European Journal of Neuroscience, 16(3), 445453. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02099.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, L. S., Colich, N. L., LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Ordaz, S. J., Price, A. N., & Gotlib, I. H. (2017). The impact of the severity of early life stress on diurnal cortisol: The role of puberty. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77, 6874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupferberg, A., Bicks, L., & Hasler, G. (2016). Social functioning in major depressive disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 313332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuzawa, C. W., Georgiev, A. V., McDade, T. W., Bechayda, S. A., & Gettler, L. T. (2016). Is there a testosterone awakening response in humans? Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 2(2), 166183. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-015-0038-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Tracy, A., Hoffmeister, J. A., Ip, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Meta-analysis: Exposure to early life stress and risk for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(7), 842855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.10.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J., Ordaz, S. J., Kircanski, K., Singh, M. K., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Predicting first onset of depression in young girls: Interaction of diurnal cortisol and negative life events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4), 850859. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000087 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Martínez, A. E., Serrano-Ibáñez, E. R., Ruiz-Párraga, G. T., Gómez-Pérez, L., Ramírez-Maestre, C., & Esteve, R. (2018). Physical health consequences of interpersonal trauma: A systematic review of the role of psychological variables. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 19(3), 305322. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838016659488 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lozza-Fiacco, S., Gordon, J. L., Andersen, E. H., Kozik, R. G., Neely, O., Schiller, C., Munoz, M., Rubinow, D. R., & Girdler, S. S. (2022). Baseline anxiety-sensitivity to estradiol fluctuations predicts anxiety symptom response to transdermal estradiol treatment in perimenopausal women – A randomized clinical trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 143, 105851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105851 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Humphreys, K. L., Belsky, J., & Ellis, B. J. (2021). The value of dimensional models of early experience: Thinking clearly about concepts and categories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(6), 14631472. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621992346 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, J. D., & Kessler, R. C. (1990). Socioeconomic status differences in vulnerability to undesirable life events. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 31(2), 162. https://doi.org/10.2307/2137170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeish, D., & Matta, T. (2018). Differentiating between mixed-effects and latent-curve approaches to growth modeling. Behavior Research Methods, 50(4), 13981414. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0976-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meadows, S. O., Brown, J. S., & Elder, G. H. (2006). Depressive symptoms, stress, and support: Gendered trajectories from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35(1), 8999. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-005-9021-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendle, J., Beltz, A. M., Carter, R., & Dorn, L. D. (2019). Understanding puberty and its measurement: Ideas for research in a new generation. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(1), 8295. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12371 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M., & Reid, M. W. (2008). Gene-environment interactions in depression research: Genetic polymorphisms and life-stress polyprocedures. Psychological Science, 19(10), 947956. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02181.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Namavar Jahromi, B., Pakmehr, S., & Hagh-Shenas, H. (2011). Work stress, premenstrual syndrome and dysphoric disorder: Are there any associations? Iranian Red Crescent Medical Journal, 13(3), 199202.Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., & Gabard-Durnam, L. J. (2020). Early adversity and critical periods: Neurodevelopmental consequences of violating the expectable environment. Trends in Neurosciences, 43(3), 133143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2020.01.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ottowitz, W. E., Derro, D., Dougherty, D. D., Lindquist, M. A., Fischman, A. J., & Hall, J. E. (2008). Analysis of amygdalar-cortical network covariance during pre- versus post-menopausal estrogen levels: Potential relevance to resting state networks, mood, and cognition. Neuro Endocrinology Letters, 29(4), 467474.Google ScholarPubMed
Owens, S. A., Helms, S. W., Rudolph, K. D., Hastings, P. D., Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2018). Interpersonal stress severity longitudinally predicts adolescent girls’ depressive symptoms: The moderating role of subjective and HPA axis stress responses. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(5), 895905. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0483-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyola, M. G., & Handa, R. J. (2017). Hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal and hypothalamic-pituitary–gonadal axes: Sex differences in regulation of stress responsivity. Stress, 20(5), 476494. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2017.1369523 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pandya, M., Altinay, M., Malone, D. A., & Anand, A. (2012). Where in the brain is depression? Current Psychiatry Reports, 14(6), 634642. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-012-0322-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patton, G. C., Olsson, C., Bond, L., Toumbourou, J. W., Carlin, J. B., Hemphill, S. A., & Catalano, R. F. (2008). Predicting female depression across puberty: A two-nation longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(12), 14241432. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181886ebe CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paykel, E. S. (2003). Life events and affective disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 6166. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0447.108.s418.13.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, R. E., Andrews, J. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hops, H. (1990). Assessment of depression in adolescents using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2(2), 122128. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.2.2.122 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SAMHSA (2020). 2020 NSDUH Detailed Tables | CBHSQ Data. Retrieved November 1, 2021, from https://www.samhsa.gov/data/report/2020-nsduh-detailed-tables Google Scholar
Sander, B., Muftah, A., Sykes Tottenham, L., Grummisch, J. A., & Gordon, J. L. (2021). Testosterone and depressive symptoms during the late menopause transition. Biology of Sex Differences, 12(1), 44. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-021-00388-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiller, C. E., Johnson, S. L., Abate, A. C., Schmidt, P. J., & Rubinow, D. R. (2016). Reproductive steroid regulation of mood and behavior. In Comprehensive physiology (pp. 11351160). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c150014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiller, C. E., Walsh, E., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Prim, J., Dichter, G. S., Schiff, L., Bizzell, J., Slightom, S. L., Richardson, E. C., Belger, A., Schmidt, P., & Rubinow, D. R. (2022). Effects of gonadal steroids on reward circuitry function and anhedonia in women with a history of postpartum depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 314, 176184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.06.078 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmalenberger, K. M., Tauseef, H. A., Barone, J. C., Owens, S. A., Lieberman, L., Jarczok, M. N., Girdler, S. S., Kiesner, J., Ditzen, B., & Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A. (2021). How to study the menstrual cycle: Practical tools and recommendations. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 123, 104895. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104895 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, P. J., Berlin, K. L., Danaceau, M. A., Neeren, A., Haq, N. A., Roca, C. A., & Rubinow, D. R. (2004). The effects of pharmacologically induced hypogonadism on mood in healthy men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(10), 9971004. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.10.997 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, P. J., Daly, R. C., Bloch, M., Smith, M. J., Danaceau, M. A., Simpson St. Clair, L., Murphy, J. H., Haq, N., & Rubinow, D. R. (2005). Dehydroepiandrosterone monotherapy in midlife-onset major and minor depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(2), 154162. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.154 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, P. J., Dor, R. B., Martinez, P. E., Guerrieri, G. M., Harsh, V. L., Thompson, K., Koziol, D. E., Nieman, L. K., & Rubinow, D. R. (2015). Effects of estradiol withdrawal on mood in women with past perimenopausal depression: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(7), 714726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, P. J., Martinez, P. E., Nieman, L. K., Koziol, D. E., Thompson, K. D., Schenkel, L., Wakim, P. G., & Rubinow, D. R. (2017). Premenstrual dysphoric disorder symptoms following ovarian suppression: Triggered by change in ovarian steroid levels but not continuous stable levels. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(10), 980989. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16101113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, P. J., Nieman, L. K., Danaceau, M. A., Adams, L. F., & Rubinow, D. R. (1998). Differential behavioral effects of gonadal steroids in women with and in those without premenstrual syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine, 338(4), 209216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitz, K. E., Hovell, M. F., Nichols, J. F., Irvin, V. L., Keating, K., Simon, G. M., Gehrman, C., & Jones, K. L. (2004). A validation study of early adolescents’ pubertal self-assessments. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 24(4), 357384. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431604268531 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selya, A. S., Rose, J. S., Dierker, L. C., Hedeker, D., & Mermelstein, R. J. (2012). A practical guide to calculating cohen’s f2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafer, A. B. (2006). Meta-analysis of the factor structures of four depression questionnaires: Beck, CES-D, Hamilton, and Zung. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 123146. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharma, S., Deuja, S., & Saha, C. G. (2016). Menstrual pattern among adolescent girls of Pokhara Valley: A cross sectional study. BMC Women’s Health, 16(1), 74. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12905-016-0354-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shih, J. H., Eberhart, N. K., Hammen, C. L., & Brennan, P. A. (2006). Differential exposure and reactivity to interpersonal stress predict sex differences in adolescent depression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 35(1), 103115. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3501_9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shirtcliff, E., Zahn-Waxler, C., Klimes-Dougan, B., & Slattery, M. (2007). Salivary dehydroepiandrosterone responsiveness to social challenge in adolescents with internalizing problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(6), 580591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sisk, L. M., & Gee, D. G. (2022). Stress and adolescence: Vulnerability and opportunity during a sensitive window of development. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 286292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.10.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., & Irwin, M. R. (2014). From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: A social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 774815. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035302 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., O’donovan, A., Epel, E. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2010). Black sheep get the blues: A psychobiological model of social rejection and depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 3945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2009). Heightened stress responsivity and emotional reactivity during pubertal maturation: Implications for psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 21(1), 8797. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000066 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sripada, R. K., Marx, C. E., King, A. P., Rajaram, N., Garfinkel, S. N., Abelson, J. L., & Liberzon, I. (2013). DHEA enhances emotion regulation neurocircuits and modulates memory for emotional stimuli. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 38(9), 17981807. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.79 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steudte-Schmiedgen, S., Kirschbaum, C., Alexander, N., & Stalder, T. (2016). An integrative model linking traumatization, cortisol dysregulation and posttraumatic stress disorder: Insight from recent hair cortisol findings. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 124135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, B. Z., Kangarloo, T., Adams, J. M., Sluss, P. M., Welt, C. K., Chandler, D. W., Zava, D. T., McGrath, J. A., Umbach, D. M., Hall, J. E., & Shaw, N. D. (2019). Healthy post-menarchal adolescent girls demonstrate multi-level reproductive axis immaturity. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 104(2), 613623. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2018-00595 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. J., Whincup, P. H., Hindmarsh, P. C., Lampe, F., Odoki, K., & Cook, D. G. (2001). Performance of a new pubertal self-assessment questionnaire: A preliminary study. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 15(1), 8894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, R. J., Wheaton, B., & Lloyd, D. A. (1995). The epidemiology of social stress. American Sociological Review, 60(1), 104. https://doi.org/10.2307/2096348 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, E. F., Sabuwalla, Z., & Huot, R. (2004). Pubertal neuromaturation, stress sensitivity, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 16(4), 807824. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579404040027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yasuda, M., Honma, S., Furuya, K., Yoshii, T., Kamiyama, Y., Ide, H., Muto, S., & Horie, S. (2008). Diagnostic significance of salivary testosterone measurement revisited: Using liquid chromatography/mass spectrometry and enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay. Journal of Men’s Health, 5(1), 5663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jomh.2007.12.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, E. S., Doom, J. R., Farrell, A. K., Carlson, E. A., Englund, M. M., Miller, G. E., Gunnar, M. R., Roisman, G. I., & Simpson, J. A. (2021). Life stress and cortisol reactivity: An exploratory analysis of the effects of stress exposure across life on HPA-axis functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 33(1), 301312. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001779 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zarrouf, F. A., Artz, S., Griffith, J., Sirbu, C., & Kommor, M. (2009). Testosterone and depression: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 15(4), 289305. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.pra.0000358315.88931.fc CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Diathesis-stress model of hormone-related mood dysregulation and affective illness. Illustration of the proposed model in which life stress modifies the brain’s sensitivity to hormone fluctuation and precipitates vulnerability (i.e., a diathesis) to depressive symptoms in the presence of normal peripubertal hormone flux (i.e., an acute hormonal stressor).

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic and sample characteristics

Figure 2

Table 2. Fixed effects of stressful life events, within-person hormone deviations, and their interactions on symptoms

Figure 3

Figure 2. Stress unmasks differential effects of within-person hormone deviations on affective symptoms. The stress context created a divergence in DHEA-related depressed affect (a), testosterone-related somatic symptoms (b), and perceived distress (c). Stressful life events are presented as a gradient (with a low number of events indicated by yellow and a high number of events represented by dark blue colors). The regression lines represent tertile groups with 90% confidence intervals. DHEA: dehydroepiandrosterone, CES-DC: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children, PSS: perceived stress scale.