Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T19:50:34.922Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Functional connectivity based brain signatures of behavioral regulation in children with ADHD, DCD, and ADHD-DCD

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 December 2021

Christiane S. Rohr*
Affiliation:
Child and Adolescent Imaging Research Program, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Owerko Centre, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Mathison Centre for Mental Health, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Hotchkiss Brain Institute, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Department of Radiology, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
Signe L. Bray
Affiliation:
Child and Adolescent Imaging Research Program, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Owerko Centre, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Mathison Centre for Mental Health, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Hotchkiss Brain Institute, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Department of Radiology, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Department of Pediatrics, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
Deborah M. Dewey
Affiliation:
Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Owerko Centre, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Hotchkiss Brain Institute, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Department of Pediatrics, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada Department of Community Health Sciences, The University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Christiane S. Rohr, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Behavioral regulation problems have been associated with daily-life and mental health challenges in children with neurodevelopmental conditions such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Here, we investigated transdiagnostic brain signatures associated with behavioral regulation. Resting-state fMRI data were collected from 115 children (31 typically developing (TD), 35 ADHD, 21 DCD, 28 ADHD-DCD) aged 7–17 years. Behavioral regulation was measured using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function and was found to differ between children with ADHD (i.e., children with ADHD and ADHD-DCD) and without ADHD (i.e., TD children and children with DCD). Functional connectivity (FC) maps were computed for 10 regions of interest and FC maps were tested for correlations with behavioral regulation scores. Across the entire sample, greater behavioral regulation problems were associated with stronger negative FC within prefrontal pathways and visual reward pathways, as well as with weaker positive FC in frontostriatal reward pathways. These findings significantly increase our knowledge on FC in children with and without ADHD and highlight the potential of FC as brain-based signatures of behavioral regulation across children with differing neurodevelopmental conditions.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Behavioral regulation is a complex socio-emotional executive function that involves inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and emotion control processes. Inhibitory control is the ability to suppress interfering distractions and prepotent motor responses (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Nigg, Reference Nigg2000). Cognitive flexibility, which is often measured using set-shifting, is the readiness with which one can switch from one task or mindset to another (Armbruster et al., Reference Armbruster, Ueltzhöffer, Basten and Fiebach2012; Diamond, Reference Diamond2013). Finally, emotion control is the process by which we influence the emotions we experience, when we experience them, and how we experience and express them (Gross, Reference Gross2002; Ochsner et al., Reference Ochsner, Silvers and Buhle2012).

Many children with neurodevelopmental conditions, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and developmental coordination disorder (DCD), have trouble regulating their behavior (Green & Payne, Reference Green and Payne2018; Posner et al., Reference Posner, Kass and Hulvershorn2014; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014; Tal Saban et al., Reference Tal Saban, Ornoy and Parush2014). They may be sensitive to external affective cues, making it hard for them to ignore distractions and follow instructions given by teachers or parents (Blair & Raver, Reference Blair and Raver2015; Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Rosen et al., Reference Rosen, Walerius, Fogleman and Factor2015). They may also display frequent and intense shifts in emotions, and have trouble recovering from negative events (Rosen et al., Reference Rosen, Walerius, Fogleman and Factor2015). This struggle with behavioral regulation not only impacts children’s social relationships and performance at school, but also results in greater daily-life and mental health challenges overall (Barkley & Fischer, Reference Barkley and Fischer2010; Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, Faraone, Surman, Petty, Clarke, Batchelder and Biederman2011).

In children and young adults with ADHD (Barkley, Reference Barkley1997; Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Barkley, Smallish and Fletcher2005), up to 50% have difficulty regulating their behavior and display high levels of emotional lability (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Steinhausen, Baldursson, Dalsgaard, Lorenzo and Ralston2006; Sobanski et al., Reference Sobanski, Banaschewski, Asherson, Buitelaar, Chen and Franke2010; Stringaris & Goodman, Reference Stringaris and Goodman2009). Evidence of treatment success with medication is limited (Lenzi et al., Reference Lenzi, Cortese, Harris and Masi2018), and many clinical trials have failed to address the difficulties in behavioral regulation that have been associated with ADHD in children (Posner et al., Reference Posner, Kass and Hulvershorn2014; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014). A handful of studies also suggest that children with DCD, a neurodevelopmental condition that is characterized by impaired motor coordination that significantly interferes with activities of daily living, school performance, as well as leisure and play activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), may have problems with behavioral regulation (Crane et al., Reference Crane, Sumner and Hill2017; Rahimi-Golkhandan et al., Reference Rahimi-Golkhandan, Steenbergen, Piek and Wilson2014; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Wade, Veldhuizen, Missiuna, Timmons and Cairney2019; van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Jansen, Reijneveld, Flapper and Smits-Engelsman2016). To date, research that has examined behavioral regulation in pediatric populations has focused on “pure” neurodevelopmental conditions, including ADHD or DCD, and has not rigorously screened participants for comorbidities, although they frequently occur (Dewey et al., Reference Dewey, Kaplan, Crawford and Wilson2002; Fliers et al., Reference Fliers, Franke, Lambregts-Rommelse, Altink, Buschgens, Nijhuis-van der Sanden and Buitelaar2009). As such, closer examination of a neurodiverse group of children with ADHD, DCD, ADHD-DCD, and typically developing (TD) children will provide us with a better understanding of the spectrum of behavioral regulation.

Reliable brain-based markers of ADHD or DCD that support diagnostic phenotypes have been elusive due to the heterogeneity of these conditions. Examining the spectrum of expression of a specific feature, such as behavioral regulation, transdiagnostically may be more promising in identifying brain-based markers of these conditions (Ameis et al., Reference Ameis, Lerch, Taylor, Lee, Viviano, Pipitone and Anagnostou2016; Lake et al., Reference Lake, Finn, Noble, Vanderwal, Shen, Rosenberg and Constable2019; Uddin et al., Reference Uddin, Dajani, Voorhies, Bednarz and Kana2017). The neural substrates of behavioral regulation have been extensively studied in neurotypical adults (Morawetz et al., Reference Morawetz, Bode, Derntl and Heekeren2017) and adults with affective disorders (Picó-Pérez et al., Reference Picó-Pérez, Radua, Steward, Menchón and Soriano-Mas2017), but less is known about the neural expression of behavioral regulation in pediatric populations. A handful of studies with relatively small sample sizes (N < 50) in children with ADHD have shown that behavioral regulation is associated with alterations in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), including orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), as well as in limbic and reward areas such as the amygdala, insula, and accumbens (Hulvershorn et al., Reference Hulvershorn, Mennes, Castellanos, Di Martino, Milham, Hummer and Roy2014; Passarotti et al., Reference Passarotti, Sweeney and Pavuluri2010; Posner et al., Reference Posner, Maia, Fair, Peterson, Sonuga-Barke and Nagel2011, Reference Posner, Rauh, Gruber, Gat, Wang and Peterson2013). Considering that problems in behavioral regulation are common in children with ADHD (Posner et al., Reference Posner, Kass and Hulvershorn2014; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014) and also reported in children with DCD (Crane et al., Reference Crane, Sumner and Hill2017; Rahimi-Golkhandan et al., Reference Rahimi-Golkhandan, Steenbergen, Piek and Wilson2014; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Wade, Veldhuizen, Missiuna, Timmons and Cairney2019; van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Jansen, Reijneveld, Flapper and Smits-Engelsman2016), and the widespread repercussions suboptimal behavioral regulation can have throughout childhood and into adulthood, systematic characterization of the interactions of these areas with the rest of the brain, or their functional connectivity (FC), transdiagnostically, has enormous potential for the diagnosis and development of individually tailored treatment for behavioral regulation difficulties in children with various neurodevelopmental conditions (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg and Leibenluft2014). Examining distributed FC patterns, that is, FC patterns spanning across multiple brain networks, provides a more holistic perspective of the associations between brain functions and behaviors than can be gleaned from analyzing brain activity or FC of a single region alone. Indeed, looking at FC patterns transdiagnostically and across multiple brain networks has been useful in improving our understanding of inattention and hyperactivity in children with and without ADHD (Elton et al., Reference Elton, Alcauter and Gao2014; Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Finn, Scheinost, Papademetris, Shen, Constable and Chun2016), and behavioral regulation in children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder (Rohr et al., Reference Rohr, Kamal and Bray2020).

The primary aim of this study was to provide a comprehensive picture of the FC signatures associated with behavioral regulation. To accomplish this, we investigated the FC signatures underlying behavioral regulation transdiagnostically in a unique cohort of TD children, children with ADHD, children with comorbid ADHD-DCD and children with DCD without any known comorbidities. We used the behavioral regulation index score on the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), a parent report measure, as our primary outcome and examined associations between behavioral regulation and 10 prefrontal, limbic and striatal regions of interest in resting-state fMRI data of TD children and children with ADHD, DCD, or ADHD-DCD. Behaviorally, we hypothesized that children with a neurodevelopmental condition would evidence more problems in behavioral regulation than TD children. Neurally, we hypothesized that the FC of prefrontal, limbic, and striatal regions would show transdiagnostic associations with behavioral regulation.

Methods and materials

This research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki for experiments involving human subjects. It was approved by the Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board of the University of Calgary. Written consent and verbal assent were obtained from parents or guardians, and participants, respectively.

Participants

Recruitment and screening

Participants were recruited from local schools and through community advertisements in locations such as hospitals and physician’s offices in Calgary, Alberta, Canada. TD children and children diagnosed with ADHD, DCD, or ADHD-DCD, as well as children with attention and/or motor difficulties, were eligible, provided they had not been diagnosed with another neurodevelopmental or psychiatric disorder, a neurological, metabolic or genetic condition, and were not born preterm (<36 weeks) or with very low birth weight (<1500 g). Potential participants were screened for contraindications for MRI and other medical problems that would prevent participation.

Neuropsychological assessment for diagnosis

Recruited participants who met the above criteria were invited to participate in a detailed neuropsychological assessment. Data were collected over several years. Children were classified as ADHD or DCD in keeping with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - Fourth Edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), because the DSM-IV was still the standard diagnostic manual in Canada when data collection began and these criteria were used throughout the study. Parents completed the ADHD module of the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents – IV (DICA-IV) computerized interview (Reich et al., Reference Reich, Weltner and Herjanic1997), which evaluates inattention and hyperactivity on several dimensions of behavior and activities of daily living. A score of “1” indicates significant impairment with respect to attention (A criterion) or hyperactivity (B criterion), and a score of “0” indicates that there is no evidence of symptoms. On the Conners’ Parent Rating Scale – Revised (CPRS-R; Conners et al., Reference Conners, Sitarenios, Parker and Epstein1998), parents rate a range of behaviors associated with ADHD and behavior problems in children. The mean T-score is 50 (SD = 10) and children with scores above 60 can be indicative of ADHD. Children were classified as ADHD if they met the diagnostic criteria on the DICA-IV (Reich et al., Reference Reich, Weltner and Herjanic1997), or had a T-score above the 95th percentile on the CPRS-R (Conners et al., Reference Conners, Sitarenios, Parker and Epstein1998) and were diagnosed by a physician as having ADHD based on DSM-IV-TR criteria. The Movement Assessment Battery for Children - Second Edition (MABC-II) is a valid standardized motor assessment that evaluates motor performance across three domains: manual dexterity, aiming and catching and balance skills (Schoemaker et al., Reference Schoemaker, Niemeijer, Flapper and Smits-Engelsman2012; Van Waelvelde et al., Reference Van Waelvelde, Peersman, Lenoir and Smits Engelsman2007). The mean standard score on this measure is 10 (SD = 3) and higher scores on this measure indicate better performance. The Developmental Coordination Questionnaire (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Kaplan, Crawford, Campbell and Dewey2000) is a valid parent report that can be used to screen for motor problems in children that affect daily functioning. Higher scores on this measure indicate better motor functioning. Children were classified as DCD if they displayed an impairment in motor function (i.e., scored ≤ 16th percentile on the MABC-II) (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Sudgen and Barnett2007), were reported by their parents as exhibiting motor difficulties that interfered significantly with daily functioning on the Developmental Coordination Questionnaire (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Kaplan, Crawford, Campbell and Dewey2000), did not evidence a visual impairment or other neurological/medical condition that would affect movement and did not display an intellectual impairment as evidenced by performance on a standardized measure of cognitive function, i.e. the Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999). The WASI (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) is a short standardized assessment that provides a valid and reliable (reliability of 0.90) measure of intelligence. It has a mean of 100 (SD = 15) and higher scores indicate a better performance. Participants completed all four WASI subtests (Block Design, Vocabulary, Matrix Reasoning and Similarities). Handedness was determined based on the preferred hand identified and used by the child when performing fine motor tasks on standardized measures of motor function (i.e., MABC-II) (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Sudgen and Barnett2007). Children meeting criteria for both ADHD and DCD were classified as ADHD-DCD. Children in the TD group did not meet criteria for ADHD or DCD. Children who were prescribed stimulant treatment for ADHD were asked to refrain from taking their medication on the day they underwent MRI scanning.

Final sample

A total of 149 participants who met criteria underwent resting state fMRI. Of these, 6 did not complete the diagnostic assessment measures; 1 (ADHD-DCD) was found to have a diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder; 14 (6 TD, 3 DCD, 3 ADHD, 2 ADHD-DCD) did not complete the cognitive assessment; and 4 (1 ADHD-DCD, 1 ADHD, 2 DCD) did not complete the MRI scan. Of the remaining participants, nine had excessive head motion on their fMRI scan (>5 mm maximum absolute displacement). Participants’ data were further evaluated for outliers on behavioral measures, defined as > 3 SD from the mean. No participant was excluded due to this criterion. The final sample consisted of 115 participants; characteristics are provided in Table 1.

Table 1. Participant characteristics

Note. Means and standard deviations (in brackets) are provided for the total sample, as well as for TD participants and participants with ADHD, DCD, and ADHD-DCD and the children without and with ADHD, separately. Motion (mm) refers to the absolute maximum displacement at any timepoint in the resting-state fMRI scan prior to motion mitigation and denoising procedures. N = number of participants; FSIQ = Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; Behavioral regulation index scores, as well as scores on the subscales (inhibition, shifting, and emotion control) are given as T-scores. denotes a significant difference between children with and without ADHD at p < .05.

Behavioral regulation assessment

Behavioral regulation was assessed with the BRIEF (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000), a standardized parent report measure of executive function behaviors for children aged 5–18 years. The BRIEF provides a composite behavioral regulation index score, which includes three subdomains of behavioral regulation: “inhibit”, “shift”, and “emotion control”. The “inhibit” subscale assesses the ability to resist impulses and to stop one’s own behavior” (sample item: “acts wilder or sillier than others in groups (birthday parties, recess)”). The “shift” subscale assesses the ability to move freely from one situation, activity, or problem to another; to tolerate change, and to switch or alternate attention (sample item: “resists or has trouble accepting a different way to solve a problem with schoolwork, friends, chores, etc.”). Finally, the “emotion control” subscale assesses the ability to regulate emotional responses appropriately (sample item: “overreacts to small problems”). Together, scores in these subscales make up the behavioral regulation index score. Normed T-scores with a mean of 50 (SD = 10) were used in the analyses, with higher scores indicating more problems in behavioral regulation.

MRI data acquisition parameters

Data were acquired at the Seaman Family MR Research Centre at the University of Calgary across two MRI systems due to a system upgrade. Sixty-seven scans were collected on a 3T GE Signa VH/i (Waukesha, WI) with an eight-channel phased-array radiofrequency head coil and 48 scans were collected on a GE 750 with an eight-channel phased-array head coil. Children were instructed to keep their eyes on a fixation cross at the center of the screen. Functional images were acquired using a gradient-echo EPI sequence in 40 axial slices (120 volumes, TR = 2500 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 70, matrix size 64 × 64, voxel size 3.44 × 3.44 × 3 mm3; duration: 5 min) in the first round of acquisition, and in 26 axial slices (140 volumes, TR = 2500 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 70, matrix size 64 × 64, voxel size 3.44 × 3.44 × 4 mm3; duration: 5.8 min) in the second round of acquisition. Anatomical scans were acquired using a T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence (TR = 1000 ms, TE = 2.5 ms, FA = 18, voxel size 0.9 × 0.9 × 4 mm3 in the first round of acquisition and TR = 7.4 ms, TE = 3.1 ms, FA = 13, voxel size 1 × 1 × 0.8 mm3 in the second round of acquisition).

MRI data preprocessing

Data preprocessing used functions from FSL (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg and Matthews2004) and AFNI (Cox, Reference Cox1996) and integrated “best-in-breed” tools for each preprocessing step covered in the workflow akin to the approach taken by fMRIprep (Esteban et al., Reference Esteban, Markiewicz, Blair, Moodie, Isik, Erramuzpe and Gorgolewski2019). The specific functions are denoted in brackets. Anatomical data were deobliqued (3drefit), oriented into FSL space (RPI) (3dresample) and skull-stripped (3dSkullStrip and 3dcalc). Functional data were also first deobliqued (3drefit) and oriented into FSL space (RPI) (3dresample). The pipeline further consisted of motion correction (MCFLIRT), skull-stripping (3dAutomask and 3dcalc), spatial smoothing (6 mm Gaussian kernel full-width at half-maximum) (fslmaths), grand-mean scaling (fslmaths), registration to the participant’s anatomical scan (FLIRT), and normalization to the McConnell Brain Imaging Center NIHPD asymmetrical (natural) pediatric template optimized for ages 4.5–18.5 years (Fonov et al., Reference Fonov, Evans, Botteron, Almli, McKinstry, Collins and Group2011) (FLIRT), followed by normalization to 2 × 2 × 2 mm MNI152 standard space (FLIRT).

Head motion and physiological confound mitigation procedure

A four-step process was used to address motion and physiological confounds in the data. First, motion estimates derived from the preprocessing were utilized to exclude participants with excessive head motion; scans were excluded if they exhibited >5 mm absolute maximum displacement. Second, AROMA was employed, an ICA-based cleaning method (Pruim et al., Reference Pruim, Mennes, van Rooij, Llera, Buitelaar and Beckmann2015), which allows for the retention of the remaining “true” neural signal within an affected volume (Kaufmann et al., Reference Kaufmann, Alnæs, Doan, Brandt, Andreassen and Westlye2017). AROMA is an automated procedure that uses a small but robust set of theoretically motivated temporal and spatial features (time series and power spectrum) to distinguish between “real” neural signals and motion artifacts. We chose a threshold that is conservative about what is retained (“aggressive”) to decrease the chance of false positives. Noise components identified by AROMA were removed from the data. Third, images were de-noised by regressing out the six motion parameters, as well as signal from white matter, cerebral spinal fluid and the global signal, as well as their first-order derivatives (Parkes et al., Reference Parkes, Fulcher, Yücel and Fornito2018). While there is currently no gold standard (Murphy & Fox, Reference Murphy and Fox2017) regarding the removal of the global signal, it was removed here based on evidence that it relates strongly to respiratory and other motion-induced signals, which persist through common denoising approaches including ICA and models that approximate respiratory variance (Power et al., Reference Power, Plitt, Gotts, Kundu, Voon, Bandettini and Martin2018). Motion (defined as each participant’s absolute maximum displacement) was substantially reduced following this procedure (before: 1.5 ± 1.2 mm; after: 0.07 ± 0.03 mm). As a final step, described below, head motion, defined as absolute maximum displacement, was included in the analysis models as a covariate of no interest. Including motions as a covariate in a regression model can reduce motion-related group differences (Power et al., Reference Power, Schlaggar and Petersen2015). This approach was chosen for our pediatric sample to minimize a residual influence of motion on the results as numerical differences in motion were noted among diagnostic groups.

Analysis of demographic, diagnostic, and behavioral measures

ANOVAs were used to examine differences in demographics and diagnostic measures among the four participant groups: TD children and children with ADHD, DCD, or ADHD-DCD. ANCOVAs were then used to assess differences in behavioral regulation, controlling for any observed differences in demographics among the four diagnostic groups as covariates of no interest. As described in the Results section below, there were no differences in behavioral regulation between TD children and children with DCD, and no differences between children with ADHD and ADHD-DCD; therefore, we focused the analysis on children with ADHD (ADHD and ADHD-DCD) versus children without ADHD (TD children and children with DCD). T-tests were utilized to assess differences in demographics, head motion and behavioral regulation between these groups. Finally, Pearson correlations were computed to assess the relationship between demographics, head motion and behavioral regulation scores. These analyses were carried out using SPSS 22 (Chicago, IL).

Analysis of fMRI data

To examine the associations between FC of the regions of interest and behavioral regulation scores across the brain for the entire sample of children, 10 regions were selected based on a well-known model of behavioral regulation (Ochsner et al., Reference Ochsner, Silvers and Buhle2012) and ADHD meta-analyses (Cortese et al., Reference Cortese, Castellanos, Eickhoff, D'Acunto, Masi, Fox and Eickhoff2016; Frodl & Skokauskas, Reference Frodl and Skokauskas2012; Hoogman et al., Reference Hoogman, Bralten, Hibar, Mennes, Zwiers, Schweren and Franke2017) (see Figure 1 and Table S1 for details). Each region’s FC map was then computed using AFNI. First, the average time course was extracted for each region (3dROIstats) and entered into a voxel-wise correlation with every other voxel in the brain using cross-correlation (3dfim+). Resultant whole-brain FC maps were normalized using Fisher’s r-to-z transform (z = .5[ln(1+r)-ln(1−r)]) for comparison across individuals (3dcalc). Group-level statistical testing was conducted with FLAME 1, a mixed-effects analysis in FSL’s FEAT using automatic outlier deweighing. In a regression analysis, the behavioral regulation index T-score was converted to a z-score and entered into a model that included z-scored age, FSIQ, sex, scanner and motion as nuisance covariates, to assess the association between FC and behavioral regulation across the entire sample of children. Voxel-wise thresholding was set at z-score >2.3, and cluster correction was conducted using Gaussian Random Field theory with p < .05. The p-values for these results were then Bonferroni-corrected for twenty comparisons (i.e., the number of seeds that were examined; significance set at p < .0025).

Figure 1. Seed regions of interest. To examine how FC associates with behavioral regulation scores across the brain and how FC differs between groups, 10 ROIs were selected in limbic areas (i.e., amygdala and insula), prefrontal areas (i.e., dorsolateral, dorsomedial, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex; orbitofrontal cortex and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex), and striatal areas (i.e., caudate, putamen, and accumbens). Regions were anatomically defined using probabilistic parcellation units provided through FSL with the Harvard-Oxford Atlas and thresholded at 50% probability, meaning any given voxel within the seed mask had a > 50% probability of lying within the specified region. Masks were binarized.

Assessment of specificity to behavioral regulation

To evaluate whether our correlation analyses captured behavioral regulation dimensionally or were driven by the categorical difference in scores due to ADHD diagnosis, we performed a post hoc correlation analysis accounting for diagnostic status through an added nuisance covariate.

Results

Sample characteristics

Characteristics for the sample are provided in Table 1 and results for all comparative tests on demographic, diagnostic and behavioral measures can be found in Tables S2–S4. There were no significant differences in behavioral regulation scores between TD children and children with DCD, or between children with ADHD and children with ADHD-DCD. Group comparisons were therefore carried out only on the combined groups of children with ADHD (ADHD and ADHD-DCD, n = 63) versus children without ADHD (TD and DCD, n = 52). Significant differences between children with and without ADHD existed in sex (p = .0002), IQ (p = .035) and the distribution across scanners (p = .006), but not in motion (neither before cleaning nor after; both p > .11). Adjusting for these covariates (i.e., sex, IQ, and distribution across scanners), results still showed significant differences between children with and without ADHD in behavioral regulation (p = .000054), reflecting greater challenges with behavioral regulation for children with ADHD. No correlations were observed between behavioral regulation and age, FSIQ or motion (neither before cleaning nor after; all p > .23).

Transdiagnostic functional connectivity associated with behavioral regulation

A total of eight FC patterns across four seeds were associated with behavioral regulation across all participants (Table 2) in the regression analysis. These were seeds in vmPFC, sgACC, OFC, and accumbens. FC associated with behavioral regulation followed four main patterns: (1) FC within medial-prefrontal areas; (2) FC between medial-prefrontal and lateral-prefrontal areas; (3) FC between medial-prefrontal areas and limbic-striatal areas; and (4) FC between accumbens and visual areas (Figure 2). Overall, greater behavioral regulation problems were associated with stronger negative FC, but also with weaker positive FC in 25% (n = 2) of the behavioral regulation-associated patterns.

Figure 2. Predominant FC patterns that associated with behavioral regulation scores across all participants. Stronger negative FC was associated with greater behavioral regulation problems between the right and left nucleus accumbens and bilateral visual cortex 1–2 (a; FC map and plot of left nucleus accumbens is shown), as well as between the right and left vmPFC and a cluster spanning left vlPFC and dlPFC (b; FC map and plot of right vmPFC is shown). Weaker positive FC was associated with greater behavioral regulation problems between left OFC and a cluster spanning right vACC and nucleus accumbens (c), as well as between left sgACC and a dorsal striatal cluster (d). Negative associations between FC and behavioral regulation scores are depicted in blue-light blue. Colored arrows with –/+ signs indicate the mean direction of FC between seed and cluster regions as identified via the seed’s FC map. Correlation plots show all values adjusted for age, IQ, sex, scanner, and motion. Results are corrected for multiple comparisons at p < .0025 (20 seeds, 10 in each hemisphere). ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; dlPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; R = right hemisphere; V1–V2 = visual cortex 1–2; vACC = ventral anterior cingulate cortex; vlPFC = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; vmPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

Table 2. Associations between behavioral regulation index scores and FC across all participants

Note. BIL = bilateral; dlPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; dmPFC = dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; L = left; Lat = Laterality; OFC = orbitofrontal cortex; R = right; rACC = rostral anterior cingulate cortex; ROI = region of interest; sgACC = subgenual anterior cingulate cortex; V1–V2 = visual cortex 1–2; V2–V3 = visual cortex 2–3; vACC = ventral anterior cingulate cortex; vlPFC = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; vmPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex; TP = temporal pole. Direction of Association refers to the direction of the association with behavioral regulation. Direction of FC refers to the direction of FC between seed region and connectivity cluster. Results are corrected for multiple comparisons at p < .0025 (20 seeds, 10 in each hemisphere).

Specificity to behavioral regulation

All FC patterns detected in the regression analysis remained associated with behavioral regulation after controlling for ADHD diagnosis, suggesting that these effects were not driven by diagnostic status.

Discussion

Poorer behavioral regulation is a known issue for children with neurodevelopmental conditions such as ADHD and is associated with greater daily-life challenges and an increased risk for psychiatric comorbidities (Barkley & Fischer, Reference Barkley and Fischer2010; Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, Faraone, Surman, Petty, Clarke, Batchelder and Biederman2011). In this study, which examined behavioral regulations across diagnostic groups (i.e., transdiagnostically), the strength of distributed patterns of FC among prefrontal, limbic, striatal, and visual brain areas was associated with children’s individual differences in behavioral regulation, and these associations remained significant after taking ADHD diagnostic status into account. Specifically, we found that FC within medial-prefrontal areas and FC between medial-prefrontal and limbic or striatal areas was significantly associated with behavioral regulation. Likewise, behavioral regulation was associated with FC between medial-prefrontal and lateral-prefrontal areas as well as with FC between reward and visual areas. However, children with a diagnosis of ADHD (i.e., children with ADHD or ADHD-DCD) had significantly more problems in behavioral regulation than TD children and children with “pure” DCD. These findings suggest that selected subsets of FC data involving frontostriatal, limbic, and visual pathways may have utility as brain-based signatures of behavioral regulation problems across children with and without ADHD despite significant differences in behavioral regulation scores.

Transdiagnostically, FC associated with behavioral regulation fell within four main seed regions − vmPFC, sgACC, OFC, and accumbens − and greater behavioral regulation problems tended to be associated either with weaker positive or with stronger negative FC. For instance, stronger negative FC between vmPFC and vlPFC/dlPFC associated with greater behavioral regulation problems, and this pattern existed bilaterally. vmPFC anatomically connects to dlPFC via vlPFC, and while individual differences in gray matter volume in vlPFC and dlPFC predicted regulatory success in a self-control study (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Tusche, Manoharan, Hutcherson, Hare and Plassmann2018), and functional activity in these regions was associated with an object’s attributed value (Hutcherson et al., Reference Hutcherson, Plassmann, Gross and Rangel2012), they may have distinct roles in behavioral regulation processes. For instance, the downregulation of cravings has been found to selectively modulate dlPFC activity, while the upregulation of cravings has been found to modulate vmPFC activity (Hutcherson et al., Reference Hutcherson, Plassmann, Gross and Rangel2012). vlPFC was functionally connected to vmPFC and dlPFC during both regulation processes, and it has been theorized that vlPFC may help to implement changes to the circuitry generated by the initiation of a behavioral regulation strategy (Hutcherson et al., Reference Hutcherson, Plassmann, Gross and Rangel2012). Refining the notion of distinct roles for the vmPFC and dlPFC further, it has been suggested that the vmPFC integrates affective valuations (made by amygdala and accumbens, rather than vmPFC itself) with inputs from prefrontal control centers like vlPFC and dlPFC that provide information about current behavioral goals (Hare et al., Reference Hare, Camerer and Rangel2009; Ochsner et al., Reference Ochsner, Silvers and Buhle2012). Thus, it seems reasonable to assume that this FC pathway between medial and lateral PFC may reflect a behavioral regulation process that integrates valuation and current behavioral goals.

Stronger negative FC between accumbens and primary visual areas also associated with greater behavioral regulation problems, and again the pattern existed bilaterally. FC between accumbens and primary visual areas has been observed during reward processing (Weiland et al., Reference Weiland, Welsh, Yau, Zucker, Zubieta and Heitzeg2013) and accumbens and visual areas have been jointly activated in reward-directed action and inhibition of action, (Le et al., Reference Le, Zhang, Zhornitsky, Wang and Li2020) and response to incentives (Gorka et al., Reference Gorka, Fuchs, Grillon and Ernst2018). Accumbens receives projections from dopamine-releasing neurons, making it rich in dopamine (Ikemoto, Reference Ikemoto2010). Dopamine is thought to code for learned associations and mediate approach behavior toward a reward; it is known to be actively involved in behavioral regulation tasks requiring cognitive flexibility, (Klanker et al., Reference Klanker, Feenstra and Denys2013) and plays an important role in processing rewarding and reinforcing stimuli (e.g., food) (Olsen, Reference Olsen2011) as well as in reward anticipation (Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Rohr, Dewey, McCrimmon and Bray2017) and outcome prediction (Bray & O'Doherty, Reference Bray and O'Doherty2007; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Cho, Vinette, Rivard, Rohr, Ten Eycke and Bray2019). Reward and behavioral regulation are arguably linked, with the term cognitive reward control being used to describe the regulation of one’s behavior towards hedonic stimuli like food (Brandl et al., Reference Brandl, Le Houcq Corbi, Mulej Bratec and Sorg2019). This is especially true in children (Power et al., Reference Power, Olivera, Hill, Beck, Hopwood, Garcia and Hughes2016). Thus, it stands to reason that this may be a visual reward FC pathway used in responding to incentives, as well as in shifting of reward-directed action and inhibition of that action.

Greater behavioral regulation problems were also associated with weaker positive FC in two patterns associated with behavioral regulation. Both FC patterns centered on frontostriatal reward pathways repeatedly shown to be heavily affected in ADHD (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Carlisi, Christakou, Murphy, Chantiluke, Giampietro and Rubia2018). Both also involved the ACC and it should be noted that ACC FC is crucial in monitoring for potential conflicts and prepotent responses (Egner et al., Reference Egner, Etkin, Gale and Hirsch2008; Etkin et al., Reference Etkin, Prater, Hoeft, Menon and Schatzberg2010; Rohr et al., Reference Rohr, Villringer, Solms-Baruth, van der Meer, Margulies and Okon-Singer2016). Behavioral regulation has been associated with FC between the OFC and accumbens/vACC, and animal studies have shown that hemodynamic signals of, and neuronal projections between, OFC and accumbens are related to inhibition-related processes that are part of reinforcement learning (Groman et al., Reference Groman, Keistler, Keip, Hammarlund, DiLeone, Pittenger and Taylor2019; Werlen et al., Reference Werlen, Shin, Gastambide, Francois, Tricklebank, Marston and Walton2019). Behavioral regulation has also been associated with FC between sgACC and putamen/pallidum and activity in both structures has been found to be aberrant during reward prediction in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (Hauser et al., Reference Hauser, Iannaccone, Dolan, Ball, Hättenschwiler, Drechsler and Brem2017), a disorder often comorbid with ADHD and that like ADHD is a “disorder of control” (Brem et al., Reference Brem, Grünblatt, Drechsler, Riederer and Walitza2014). Further, volume in both structures has been found to be different in adult and pediatric individuals with ADHD (Frodl & Skokauskas, Reference Frodl and Skokauskas2012) and OCD (Ahmed et al., Reference Ahmed, Ras and Seedat2012; Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Keshavan, Diwadkar, Nutche, Macmaster, Easter and Rosenberg2008).

Individual differences in behavioral regulation have been repeatedly found to be associated with individual features in FC (Ferri et al., Reference Ferri, Schmidt, Hajcak and Canli2016; Fitzgerald et al., Reference Fitzgerald, Klumpp, Langenecker and Phan2019; Rohr et al., Reference Rohr, Villringer, Solms-Baruth, van der Meer, Margulies and Okon-Singer2016). Taking individual differences into account can help expose the underlying neural substrates of complex cognitive skills, emotions and social competencies, and has proven useful in the investigation of both neurotypical (Goldfarb et al., Reference Goldfarb, Chun and Phelps2016; Rohr et al., Reference Rohr, Okon-Singer, Craddock, Villringer and Margulies2013, Reference Rohr, Dreyer, Aderka, Margulies, Frisch, Villringer and Okon-Singer2015; Vossel et al., Reference Vossel, Weidner, Moos and Fink2016) and clinical populations (Nebel et al., Reference Nebel, Eloyan, Nettles, Sweeney, Ament, Ward and Mostofsky2015; van Dongen et al., Reference van Dongen, von Rhein, O'Dwyer, Franke, Hartman, Heslenfeld and Buitelaar2015; von Rhein et al., Reference von Rhein, Cools, Zwiers, van der Schaaf, Franke, Luman and Buitelaar2015), as traits and abilities associated with neurodevelopmental conditions such as ADHD exist in the neurotypical population, falling on a spectrum (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Nigg and Fair2014; van Dongen et al., Reference van Dongen, von Rhein, O'Dwyer, Franke, Hartman, Heslenfeld and Buitelaar2015). Examination of individual differences also allows for more statistical power in studies that include children with neurodevelopmental conditions such ADHD and DCD, which often struggle with small, heterogenous samples (Fair et al., Reference Fair, Bathula, Nikolas and Nigg2012; Nigg, Reference Nigg2005; Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Sergeant, Nigg and Willcutt2008; Uddin et al., Reference Uddin, Dajani, Voorhies, Bednarz and Kana2017).

Unlike several recent studies (Crane et al., Reference Crane, Sumner and Hill2017; Rahimi-Golkhandan et al., Reference Rahimi-Golkhandan, Steenbergen, Piek and Wilson2014; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Wade, Veldhuizen, Missiuna, Timmons and Cairney2019; van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Jansen, Reijneveld, Flapper and Smits-Engelsman2016), we found no elevation of behavioral regulation scores in children with “pure” DCD. This may be because we rigorously screened for comorbid ADHD; up to 50% of children with DCD meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD but only 5% are diagnosed (McLeod et al., Reference McLeod, Langevin, Dewey and Goodyear2016). We also found that children with ADHD-DCD showed elevated scores on behavioral regulation; therefore, it is likely that the behavioral regulation problems that have previously been identified in children with DCD are due to comorbidity with ADHD rather than DCD itself.

The current study has several distinct strengths, which include appropriate preprocessing techniques, and the use of a reliable and validated measure of behavioral regulation in a relatively large group that included children who met diagnostic criteria for ADHD, DCD and ADHD-DCD, as well as TD children. The measure used to assess behavioral regulation is well validated (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000) and although parent-reports are subjective, they capture a measure of behavior integrated over a longer time frame than can be observed in a laboratory visit and have better test−retest reliability (Enkavi et al., Reference Enkavi, Eisenberg, Bissett, Mazza, MacKinnon, Marsch and Poldrack2019). The study also has several weaknesses, including a relatively short scan time and differences between our groups of children with and without ADHD in (1) sex ratios, (2) IQ, and (3) distribution across scanners. We have done our best to account for these by including sex, IQ, and scanner as covariates in all analyses. While a short scan time is of benefit from an acquisition perspective, longer scan times may strengthen the reliability of FC estimates (Birn et al., Reference Birn, Molloy, Patriat, Parker, Meier, Kirk and Prabhakaran2013). It is also important to note that different task-based paradigms of behavioral regulation may yield additional insights to the resting-state paradigm employed here; we chose to investigate how an index score of behavioral regulation associates with FC across multiple brain networks to provide a more holistic perspective of the relationship between brain connectivity and behavioral regulation. Finally, while our FC maps were calculated using cross-correlation, a stronger measure than Pearson correlation, future work may be complemented by alternative FC measures that capture different aspects of FC (Mohanty et al., Reference Mohanty, Sethares, Nair and Prabhakaran2020).

Our findings significantly increase our knowledge on behavioral regulation and its underlying neural expression across a neurodiverse spectrum of children with and without ADHD, including children with DCD and combined ADHD-DCD. They suggest that behavioral regulation problems in DCD are likely attributable to comorbidity with ADHD. Children’s individual differences in behavioral regulation further associated with FC across diagnostic groups. Specifically, they associated with pathways between medial and lateral PFC, which may reflect a behavioral regulation process that integrates valuation and current behavioral goals. Children’s individual differences in behavioral regulation also associated with FC in frontostriatal reward pathways and visual reward pathways used in shifting of reward-directed action and inhibition of that action. Overall, our results highlight the utility of directly examining variables of potential clinical interest, such as behavioral regulation, and their associations with FC across children with differing neurodevelopmental conditions.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research awarded to DD (MOP 88588), as well as NSERC CREATE I3T and Alberta Innovates Postdoctoral Fellowships awarded to CR. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. The authors thank all children and families who participated in this study.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Ahmed, F., Ras, J., & Seedat, S. (2012). Volumetric structural magnetic resonance imaging findings in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder and obsessive compulsive disorder: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 568. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00568 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ameis, S. H., Lerch, J. P., Taylor, M. J., Lee, W., Viviano, J. D., Pipitone, J.Anagnostou, E. (2016). A diffusion tensor imaging study in children with ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, OCD, and matched controls: Distinct and non-distinct white matter disruption and dimensional brain-behavior relationships. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(12), 12131222. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111435 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, D.C. Google Scholar
Armbruster, D. J., Ueltzhöffer, K., Basten, U., & Fiebach, C. J. (2012). Prefrontal cortical mechanisms underlying individual differences in cognitive flexibility and stability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(12), 23852399. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00286 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 6594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R. A., & Fischer, M. (2010). The unique contribution of emotional impulsiveness to impairment in major life activities in hyperactive children as adults. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(5), 503513.Google ScholarPubMed
Becker, A., Steinhausen, H. C., Baldursson, G., Dalsgaard, S., Lorenzo, M. J., Ralston, S. J.ADORE Study Group (2006). Psychopathological screening of children with ADHD: Strengths and difficulties questionnaire in a pan-European study. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(Suppl 1), I56I62, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-006-1008-7,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birn, R. M., Molloy, E. K., Patriat, R., Parker, T., Meier, T. B., Kirk, G. R.Prabhakaran, V. (2013). The effect of scan length on the reliability of resting-state fMRI connectivity estimates. Neuroimage, 83, 550558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.099 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. The Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711731. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandl, F., Le Houcq Corbi, Z., Mulej Bratec, S., & Sorg, C. (2019). Cognitive reward control recruits medial and lateral frontal cortices, which are also involved in cognitive emotion regulation: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroimage, 200, 659673. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.07.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bray, S., & O'Doherty, J. (2007). Neural coding of reward-prediction error signals during classical conditioning with attractive faces. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97(4), 30363045. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01211.2006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brem, S., Grünblatt, E., Drechsler, R., Riederer, P., & Walitza, S. (2014). The neurobiological link between OCD and ADHD. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, 6(3), 175202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-014-0146-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D., & Epstein, J. N. (1998). The revised Conners' Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(4), 257268. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1022602400621 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cortese, S., Castellanos, F. X., Eickhoff, C. R., D'Acunto, G., Masi, G., Fox, P. T.Eickhoff, S. B. (2016). Functional decoding and meta-analytic connectivity modeling in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 80(12), 896904. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research, 29(3), 162173. https://doi.org/Doi10.1006/Cbmr.1996.0014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crane, L., Sumner, E., & Hill, E. L. (2017). Emotional and behavioural problems in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: Exploring parent and teacher reports. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 70, 6774. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, D., Kaplan, B. J., Crawford, S. G., & Wilson, B. N. (2002). Developmental coordination disorder: Associated problems in attention, learning, and psychosocial adjustment. Human Movement Science, 21(5-6), 905918. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-9457(02)00163-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. The Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egner, T., Etkin, A., Gale, S., & Hirsch, J. (2008). Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cerebral Cortex, 18(6), 14751484. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elton, A., Alcauter, S., & Gao, W. (2014). Network connectivity abnormality profile supports a categorical-dimensional hybrid model of ADHD. Human Brain Mapping, 35(9), 45314543. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22492 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enkavi, A. Z., Eisenberg, I. W., Bissett, P. G., Mazza, G. L., MacKinnon, D. P., Marsch, L. A.Poldrack, R. A. (2019). Large-scale analysis of test-retest reliabilities of self-regulation measures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(12), 54725477. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818430116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esteban, O., Markiewicz, C. J., Blair, R. W., Moodie, C. A., Isik, A. I., Erramuzpe, A.Gorgolewski, K. J. (2019). fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nature Methods, 16(1), 111116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A., Prater, K., Hoeft, F., Menon, V., & Schatzberg, A. (2010). Failure of anterior cingulate activation and connectivity with the amygdala during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(5), 545554. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070931 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fair, D. A., Bathula, D., Nikolas, M. A., & Nigg, J. T. (2012). Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with ADHD. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(17), 67696774. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1115365109 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferri, J., Schmidt, J., Hajcak, G., & Canli, T. (2016). Emotion regulation and amygdala-precuneus connectivity: Focusing on attentional deployment. Cognitive Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(6), 9911002. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0447-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2005). Executive functioning in hyperactive children as young adults: Attention, inhibition, response perseveration, and the impact of comorbidity. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(1), 107133. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2701_5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitzgerald, J. M., Klumpp, H., Langenecker, S., & Phan, K. L. (2019). Transdiagnostic neural correlates of volitional emotion regulation in anxiety and depression. Depression and Anxiety, 36(5), 453464. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22859 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fliers, E. A., Franke, B., Lambregts-Rommelse, N. N., Altink, M. E., Buschgens, C. J., Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W.Buitelaar, J. K. (2009). Undertreatment of motor problems in children with ADHD. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 15(2), 8590. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-3588.2009.00538.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonov, V., Evans, A. C., Botteron, K., Almli, C. R., McKinstry, R. C., Collins, D. L.Group, B. D. C. (2011). Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage, 54(1), 313327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., & Skokauskas, N. (2012). Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(2), 114126. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01786.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, A. R., Keshavan, M. S., Diwadkar, V., Nutche, J., Macmaster, F., Easter, P. C.Rosenberg, D. R. (2008). Gray matter differences between pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder patients and high-risk siblings: A preliminary voxel-based morphometry study. Neuroscience Letters, 435(1), 4550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfarb, E. V., Chun, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2016). Memory-guided attention: Independent contributions of the hippocampus and striatum. Neuron, 89(2), 317324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorka, A. X., Fuchs, B., Grillon, C., & Ernst, M. (2018). Impact of induced anxiety on neural responses to monetary incentives. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(11), 11111119. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D., & Payne, S. (2018). Understanding organisational ability and self-regulation in children with developmental coordination disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 5(1), 3442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-018-0129-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groman, S. M., Keistler, C., Keip, A. J., Hammarlund, E., DiLeone, R. J., Pittenger, C.Taylor, J. R. (2019). Orbitofrontal circuits control multiple reinforcement-learning processes. Neuron, 103(4), 734746.e733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281291. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0048577201393198 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646648. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1168450 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, T. U., Iannaccone, R., Dolan, R. J., Ball, J., Hättenschwiler, J., Drechsler, R.Brem, S. (2017). Increased fronto-striatal reward prediction errors moderate decision making in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 47(7), 12461258. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716003305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, S., Sudgen, D., & Barnett, A. (2007). Movement assessment battery for children: 2nd Edition (MABC-2). London, United Kingdom.Google Scholar
Hoogman, M., Bralten, J., Hibar, D. P., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P., Schweren, L. S. J.Franke, B. (2017). Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: A cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry, 4(4), 310319. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulvershorn, L. A., Mennes, M., Castellanos, F. X., Di Martino, A., Milham, M. P., Hummer, T. A.Roy, A. K. (2014). Abnormal amygdala functional connectivity associated with emotional lability in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(3), 351361.e351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.11.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutcherson, C. A., Plassmann, H., Gross, J. J., & Rangel, A. (2012). Cognitive regulation during decision making shifts behavioral control between ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal value systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(39), 1354313554. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6387-11.2012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ikemoto, S. (2010). Brain reward circuitry beyond the mesolimbic dopamine system: A neurobiological theory. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(2), 129150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.02.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufmann, T., Alnæs, D., Doan, N. T., Brandt, C. L., Andreassen, O. A., & Westlye, L. T. (2017). Delayed stabilization and individualization in connectome development are related to psychiatric disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 20(4), 513515. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4511 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klanker, M., Feenstra, M., & Denys, D. (2013). Dopaminergic control of cognitive flexibility in humans and animals. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 201. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00201 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lake, E. M. R., Finn, E. S., Noble, S. M., Vanderwal, T., Shen, X., Rosenberg, M. D.Constable, R. T. (2019). The functional brain organization of an individual allows prediction of measures of social abilities transdiagnostically in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 86(4), 315326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.02.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le, T. M., Zhang, S., Zhornitsky, S., Wang, W., & Li, C. R. (2020). Neural correlates of reward-directed action and inhibition of action. Cortex, 123, 4256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenzi, F., Cortese, S., Harris, J., & Masi, G. (2018). Pharmacotherapy of emotional dysregulation in adults with ADHD: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 359367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, M., Nigg, J. T., & Fair, D. A. (2014). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 16, 235266. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2013_249 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, K. R., Langevin, L. M., Dewey, D., & Goodyear, B. G. (2016). Atypical within- and between-hemisphere motor network functional connections in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage Clinical, 12, 157164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.06.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohanty, R., Sethares, W. A., Nair, V. A., & Prabhakaran, V. (2020). Rethinking measures of functional connectivity via feature extraction. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1298. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-57915-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morawetz, C., Bode, S., Derntl, B., & Heekeren, H. R. (2017). The effect of strategies, goals and stimulus material on the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 72, 111128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, K., & Fox, M. D. (2017). Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage, 154, 169173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.052 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nebel, M. B., Eloyan, A., Nettles, C. A., Sweeney, K. L., Ament, K., Ward, R. E.Mostofsky, S. H. (2015). Intrinsic visual-motor synchrony correlates with social deficits in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 79(8), 633641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.08.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2005). Neuropsychologic theory and findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The state of the field and salient challenges for the coming decade. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 14241435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, L. J., Carlisi, C. O., Christakou, A., Murphy, C. M., Chantiluke, K., Giampietro, V.Rubia, K. (2018). Frontostriatal dysfunction during decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(8), 694703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.03.009 Google ScholarPubMed
Ochsner, K. N., Silvers, J. A., & Buhle, J. T. (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: A synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251(1), E124. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x 1251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsen, C. M. (2011). Natural rewards, neuroplasticity, and non-drug addictions. Neuropharmacology, 61(7), 11091122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.03.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkes, L., Fulcher, B., Yücel, M., & Fornito, A. (2018). An evaluation of the efficacy, reliability, and sensitivity of motion correction strategies for resting-state functional MRI. Neuroimage, 171, 415436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.073 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passarotti, A. M., Sweeney, J. A., & Pavuluri, M. N. (2010). Differential engagement of cognitive and affective neural systems in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(1), 106117. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617709991019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picó-Pérez, M., Radua, J., Steward, T., Menchón, J. M., & Soriano-Mas, C. (2017). Emotion regulation in mood and anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of fMRI cognitive reappraisal studies. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 79(Pt B), 96104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.06.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, J., Kass, E., & Hulvershorn, L. (2014). Using stimulants to treat ADHD-related emotional lability. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16(10), 478. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0478-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, J., Maia, T. V., Fair, D., Peterson, B. S., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., & Nagel, B. J. (2011). The attenuation of dysfunctional emotional processing with stimulant medication: An fMRI study of adolescents with ADHD. Psychiatry Research, 193(3), 151160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.02.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, J., Rauh, V., Gruber, A., Gat, I., Wang, Z., & Peterson, B. S. (2013). Dissociable attentional and affective circuits in medication-naïve children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 213(1), 2430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, J. D., Plitt, M., Gotts, S. J., Kundu, P., Voon, V., Bandettini, P. A.Martin, A. (2018). Ridding fMRI data of motion-related influences: Removal of signals with distinct spatial and physical bases in multiecho data. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(9), E2105E2114. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1720985115 Google ScholarPubMed
Power, J. D., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2015). Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage, 105, 536551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.044 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, T. G., Olivera, Y. A., Hill, R. A., Beck, A. D., Hopwood, V., Garcia, K. S.Hughes, S. O. (2016). Emotion regulation strategies and childhood obesity in high risk preschoolers. Appetite, 107, 623627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.09.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruim, R. H., Mennes, M., van Rooij, D., Llera, A., Buitelaar, J. K., & Beckmann, C. F. (2015). ICA-AROMA: A robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. Neuroimage, 112, 267277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rahimi-Golkhandan, S., Steenbergen, B., Piek, J. P., & Wilson, P. H. (2014). Deficits of hot executive function in developmental coordination disorder: Sensitivity to positive social cues. Human Movement Science, 38, 209224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.09.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reich, W., Weltner, Z., & Herjanic, B. (1997). The diagnostic interview for children and adolescents-IV. Multi-Health Systems, North Tonawanda, NY.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, M. C., Wade, T. J., Veldhuizen, S., Missiuna, C., Timmons, B., & Cairney, J. (2019). Emotional and behavioral problems in 4- and 5-year old children with and without motor delays. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 7, 474. https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2019.00474 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohr, C. S., Dreyer, F. R., Aderka, I. M., Margulies, D. S., Frisch, S., Villringer, A.Okon-Singer, H. (2015). Individual differences in common factors of emotional traits and executive functions predict functional connectivity of the amygdala. Neuroimage, 120, 154163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohr, C. S., Kamal, S., & Bray, S. (2020). Building functional connectivity neuromarkers of behavioral self-regulation across children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 41, 100747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100747 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohr, C. S., Okon-Singer, H., Craddock, R. C., Villringer, A., & Margulies, D. S. (2013). Affect and the brain’s functional organization: A resting-state connectivity approach. PLoS One, 8(7), e68015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohr, C. S., Villringer, A., Solms-Baruth, C., van der Meer, E., Margulies, D. S., & Okon-Singer, H. (2016). The neural networks of subjectively evaluated emotional conflicts. Human Brain Mapping, 37(6), 22342246. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23169 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, P. J., Walerius, D. M., Fogleman, N. D., & Factor, P. I. (2015). The association of emotional lability and emotional and behavioral difficulties among children with and without ADHD. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, 7(4), 281294. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-015-0175-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. D., Finn, E. S., Scheinost, D., Papademetris, X., Shen, X., Constable, R. T.Chun, M. M. (2016). A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 165171. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, L., Tusche, A., Manoharan, N., Hutcherson, C., Hare, T., & Plassmann, H. (2018). Neuroanatomy of the vmPFC and dlPFC predicts individual differences in cognitive regulation during dietary self-control across regulation strategies. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(25), 57995806. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3402-17.2018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoemaker, M. M., Niemeijer, A. S., Flapper, B. C., & Smits-Engelsman, B. C. (2012). Validity and reliability of the movement assessment battery for children-2 checklist for children with and without motor impairments. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 54(4), 368375. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04226.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuetze, M., Cho, I. Y. K., Vinette, S., Rivard, K. B., Rohr, C. S., Ten Eycke, K.Bray, S. L. (2019). Learning with individual-interest outcomes in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 38, 100668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100668 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuetze, M., Rohr, C. S., Dewey, D., McCrimmon, A., & Bray, S. (2017). Reinforcement learning in autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 378. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02035 2035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P., Stringaris, A., Nigg, J., & Leibenluft, E. (2014). Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(3), 276293. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H.Matthews, P. M. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage, 23, S208S219. https://doi.org/Doi10.1016/J.Neuroimage.2004.07.051 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobanski, E., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P., Buitelaar, J., Chen, W., Franke, B.et al. (2010). Emotional lability in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Clinical correlates and familial prevalence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(8), 915923. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02217.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Sergeant, J. A., Nigg, J., & Willcutt, E. (2008). Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Nosologic and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17(2), 367384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.008 ix.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spencer, T. J., Faraone, S. V., Surman, C. B., Petty, C., Clarke, A., Batchelder, H.Biederman, J. (2011). Toward defining deficient emotional self-regulation in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using the child behavior checklist: A controlled study. Postgraduate Medicine, 123(5), 5059. https://doi.org/10.3810/pgm.2011.09.2459 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stringaris, A., & Goodman, R. (2009). Mood lability and psychopathology in youth. Psychological Medicine, 39(8), 12371245. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708004662 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tal Saban, M., Ornoy, A., & Parush, S. (2014). Executive function and attention in young adults with and without developmental coordination disorder--a comparative study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(11), 26442650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uddin, L. Q., Dajani, D. R., Voorhies, W., Bednarz, H., & Kana, R. K. (2017). Progress and roadblocks in the search for brain-based biomarkers of autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 7(8), e1218. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.164 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, M., Jansen, D. E., Reijneveld, S. A., Flapper, B. C., & Smits-Engelsman, B. C. (2016). Identification of emotional and behavioral problems by teachers in children with developmental coordination disorder in the school community. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 51-52, 4048. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2016.01.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Dongen, E. V., von Rhein, D., O'Dwyer, L., Franke, B., Hartman, C. A., Heslenfeld, D. J.Buitelaar, J. (2015). Distinct effects of ASD and ADHD symptoms on reward anticipation in participants with ADHD, their unaffected siblings and healthy controls: A cross-sectional study. Molecular Autism, 6, 48. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-015-0043-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Waelvelde, H., Peersman, W., Lenoir, M., & Smits Engelsman, B. C. (2007). The reliability of the movement assessment battery for children for preschool children with mild to moderate motor impairment. Clinical Rehabilitation, 21(5), 465470. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269215507074052 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Rhein, D., Cools, R., Zwiers, M. P., van der Schaaf, M., Franke, B., Luman, M.Buitelaar, J. (2015). Increased neural responses to reward in adolescents and young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(5), 394402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.02.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vossel, S., Weidner, R., Moos, K., & Fink, G. R. (2016). Individual attentional selection capacities are reflected in interhemispheric connectivity of the parietal cortex. Neuroimage, 129, 148158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Weiland, B. J., Welsh, R. C., Yau, W. Y., Zucker, R. A., Zubieta, J. K., & Heitzeg, M. M. (2013). Accumbens functional connectivity during reward mediates sensation-seeking and alcohol use in high-risk youth. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 128(1-2), 130139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.08.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werlen, E., Shin, S. L., Gastambide, F., Francois, J., Tricklebank, M. D., Marston, H. M.Walton, M. E. (2019). Amphetamine disrupts haemodynamic correlates of prediction errors in nucleus accumbens and orbitofrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology, 45, 793803. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-019-0564-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, B. N., Kaplan, B. J., Crawford, S. G., Campbell, A., & Dewey, D. (2000). Reliability and validity of a parent questionnaire on childhood motor skills. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 54(5), 484493. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.54.5.484 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant characteristics

Figure 1

Figure 1. Seed regions of interest. To examine how FC associates with behavioral regulation scores across the brain and how FC differs between groups, 10 ROIs were selected in limbic areas (i.e., amygdala and insula), prefrontal areas (i.e., dorsolateral, dorsomedial, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex; orbitofrontal cortex and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex), and striatal areas (i.e., caudate, putamen, and accumbens). Regions were anatomically defined using probabilistic parcellation units provided through FSL with the Harvard-Oxford Atlas and thresholded at 50% probability, meaning any given voxel within the seed mask had a > 50% probability of lying within the specified region. Masks were binarized.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Predominant FC patterns that associated with behavioral regulation scores across all participants. Stronger negative FC was associated with greater behavioral regulation problems between the right and left nucleus accumbens and bilateral visual cortex 1–2 (a; FC map and plot of left nucleus accumbens is shown), as well as between the right and left vmPFC and a cluster spanning left vlPFC and dlPFC (b; FC map and plot of right vmPFC is shown). Weaker positive FC was associated with greater behavioral regulation problems between left OFC and a cluster spanning right vACC and nucleus accumbens (c), as well as between left sgACC and a dorsal striatal cluster (d). Negative associations between FC and behavioral regulation scores are depicted in blue-light blue. Colored arrows with –/+ signs indicate the mean direction of FC between seed and cluster regions as identified via the seed’s FC map. Correlation plots show all values adjusted for age, IQ, sex, scanner, and motion. Results are corrected for multiple comparisons at p < .0025 (20 seeds, 10 in each hemisphere). ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; dlPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; R = right hemisphere; V1–V2 = visual cortex 1–2; vACC = ventral anterior cingulate cortex; vlPFC = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; vmPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

Figure 3

Table 2. Associations between behavioral regulation index scores and FC across all participants