Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-01T01:19:04.626Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 July 2008

Karen L. Bierman*
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Robert L. Nix
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Mark T. Greenberg
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Clancy Blair
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Celene E. Domitrovich
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Karen Bierman, Pennsylvania State University, 251 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Despite their potentially central role in fostering school readiness, executive function (EF) skills have received little explicit attention in the design and evaluation of school readiness interventions for socioeconomically disadvantaged children. The present study examined a set of five EF measures in the context of a randomized-controlled trial of a research-based intervention integrated into Head Start programs (Head Start REDI). Three hundred fifty-six 4-year-old children (17% Hispanic, 25% African American; 54% girls) were followed over the course of the prekindergarten year. Initial EF predicted gains in cognitive and social–emotional skills and moderated the impact of the Head Start REDI intervention on some outcomes. The REDI intervention promoted gains on two EF measures, which partially mediated intervention effects on school readiness. We discuss the importance of further study of the neurobiological bases of school readiness, the implications for intervention design, and the value of incorporating markers of neurobiological processes into school readiness interventions.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This project was supported by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grants HD046064 and HD43763. We appreciate the teachers, students, parents, and program personnel who served as partners in this project in the Huntingdon, Blair, and York County Head Start Programs of Pennsylvania. In addition, this work reflects the particular efforts and talents of Gloria Rhule, Harriet Darling, Julia Gest, the REDI intervention staff, and the entire REDI research team.

References

Adams, M. J., Foorman, B. R., Lundberg, I., & Beeler, T. (1998). Phonological sensitivity in young children: A classroom curriculum. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (2001). The EF and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 129.Google Scholar
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.Google Scholar
Bierman, K. L., Domitrovich, C. E., Nix, R. L., Gest, S. D., Welsh, J. A., Greenberg, M. T., et al. (2007). Promoting academic and social–emotional school readiness: The Head Start REDI Program. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C. (2006). How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 109160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647680.Google Scholar
Blair, C., Zelazo, P. D., & Greenberg, M. T. (2005). The measurement of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 561571.Google Scholar
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2007). Tools of the mind: The Vygotskian approach to early childhood education (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Kipp, K. (1996). Parental investment theory and gender differences in the evolution of inhibitory mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 163188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brophy, M., Taylor, E., & Hughes, C. (2002). To go or not to go: Inhibitory control in “hard to manage” children. Infant and Child Development, 11, 125140.Google Scholar
Brownell, R. (2000). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Manual. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 273293.Google Scholar
Campbell, S. B., & Stauffenberg, C. (2008). Child characteristics and family processes that predict behavioral readiness for school. In Crouter, A. & Booth, A. (Eds.), Early disparities in school readiness: How families contribute to transitions into school (pp. 225258). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 595616.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual difference in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 10321053.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cauffman, E., Steinberg, L., & Piquero, A. R. (2004). Psychological, neuropsychological, and physiological correlates of serious anti-social behavior in adolescence: The role of self-control. Criminology, 43, 133175.Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 703722.Google Scholar
Chang, F., & Burns, B. M. (2005). Attention in preschoolers: Associations with effortful control and motivation. Child Development, 76, 247263.Google Scholar
Children's Defense Fund. (2005). The state of America's children 2005. Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2002). The impact of social experience on neurobiological systems: Illustration from a constructivist view of child maltreatment. Cognitive Development, 17, 14071428.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Usher, B. A., & Cargo, A. P. (1993). Cognitive risk and its association with risk for disruptive behavior disorder in preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 154164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (CPPRG). (1995). Teacher Social Competence Scale Technical Report. Retrieved from the Fast Track Project website: http://www.fasttrackproject.orgGoogle Scholar
Coolahan, K., Fantuzzo, J., Mendez, J., & McDermott, P. (2000). Preschool peer interactions and readiness to learn: Relationships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors and conduct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 458465.Google Scholar
Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and overt aggression in preschool. Developmental Psychology, 33, 579588.Google Scholar
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 20372078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1996). The development of children's theory of mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 2531.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., & Burton, R. (2003). Social and emotional prevention and intervention programming for preschoolers. New York: Kluwer/Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. A. (1997). Reactive and effortful process in the organization of temperament. Developmental Psychopathology, 9, 633652.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), The frontal lobes (pp. 466503). London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Diamond, A., Carlson, S. M., & Beck, D. M. (2005). Preschool children's performance in task switching on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task: Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 689729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to do as I say, not as I do. Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 315334.Google Scholar
Domitrovich, C. E., Cortes, R., & Greenberg, M. T. (2007). Improving young children's social and emotional competence: A randomized trial of the preschool PATHS curriculum. Journal of Primary Prevention, 28, 6791.Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P. (1999). Development in the preschool years: The typical path. In Nuttall, E. V., Romero, I., & Kalesnik, J. (Eds.), Assessing and screening preschoolers: Psychological and educational dimensions (2nd ed., pp. 924). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. D., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 465486.Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172179.Google Scholar
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483527.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, D. F., & Rogoff, B. (1997). Mothers' and toddlers' coordinated joint focus of attention: Variations with maternal dysphoric symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 33, 113119.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, M. T. (2006). Promoting resilience in children and youth: Preventive interventions and their interface with neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 139150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche, C. A. (1993). Promoting social and emotional development in deaf children: The PATHS Project. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Riggs, N. (2004). The PATHS Curriculum: Theory and research on neuro-cognitive development and school success. In Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., Wang, M. C., & Walberg, H. J., (Eds.), Building academic success on social and emotional learning: What does the research say? (pp. 170188) New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Havighurst, S. S., Harley, A., & Prior, M. (2004). Building preschool children's emotional competence: A parenting program. Early Education and Development, 15, 423448.Google Scholar
Howes, C., Hamilton, C. E., & Philipsen, L.C. (1998). Stability and continuity of child–caregiver and child–peer relationships. Child Development, 69, 418426.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131142..Google Scholar
Hughes, J. N., & Kwok, O. (2006). Classroom engagement mediates the effect of teacher–student support on elementary students' peer acceptance: A prospective analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 465480.Google Scholar
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177186.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and Implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krull, J. L., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2001). Multilevel modeling of individual and group level mediated effects. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 36, 249277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, G. W., Price, J. M., & Hart, C. H. (1988). Predicting preschoolers' peer status from their playground behaviors. Child Development, 59, 986992.Google Scholar
Lengua, L. J., Honorado, E., & Bush, N. R. (2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 4055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li-Grining, C. P. (2007). Effortful control among low-income preschoolers in three cities: Stability, change, and individual differences. Developmental Psychology, 43, 208221.Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J. (2006). Development, assessment, and promotion of preliteracy skills. Early Education and Development, 17, 91114.Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596613.Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2007). TOPEL: Test of Preschool Early Literacy. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2006). The impact of kindergarten learning-related skills on academic trajectories at the end of elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 471490.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wagner, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of EF and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). The neuropsychology of conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 135151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Early Literacy Panel. (2005). Report on a synthesis of early predictors of reading. Louisville, KY: Author.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2003). Narrative and self, myth and memory: Emergence of the cultural self. In Fivush, R. & Haden, C. A. (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 328). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nigg, J. T., Quamma, J. P., Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche, C. A. (1999). A two-year longitudinal study of neuropsychological and cognitive performance in relation to behavioral problems and competencies in elementary school children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 5163.Google Scholar
Normandeau, S., & Guay, F. (1998). Preschool behavior and first grade school achievement: The mediational role of cognitive self-control. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 111121.Google Scholar
O'Connor, T. G., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Kreppner, J. M., Keaveney, L., & the English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team (2000). The effects of global severe privation on cognitive competence: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Child Development, 71, 376390.Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., & Klorman, R. (1998). Stress, memory, and emotion: Developmental considerations from the study of child maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 679688.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Peterson, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neurosocience, 13, 2542.Google Scholar
Riggs, N. R., Blair, C. B., & Greenberg, M. T. (2003). Concurrent and 2-year longitudinal relations between executive function and the behavior of 1st and 2nd grade children. Child Neuropsychology, 9, 267276.Google Scholar
Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Pentz, M. A. (2006). The meditational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social–emotional prevention program in elementary school students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science, 7, 91102.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Hershey, K. L. (1995). Temperament, attention, and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (pp. 315340). Oxford: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 573594.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M., Fauth, R. C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Childhood poverty: Implications for school readiness and early childhood education. In Spodek, B. & Saracho, O. N. (Eds.), Handbook of research on the education of children (2nd ed., pp. 323346.) Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sanchez, M. M., Ladd, C. O., & Plotsky, P. M. (2001). Early adverse experiences as a risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 419449.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.) (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Smith-Donald, R., Raver, C. C., Hayes, T., & Richardson, B. (2007). Preliminary construct and concurrent validity of the preschool self-regulation assessment (PSRA) for field-based research. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 173187.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In Leinhardt, S. (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290312). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.Google Scholar
Sobel, M. E. (1986). Some new results on indirect effects and their standard errors in covariance structure models. In Tuma, N. (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 159186). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.Google Scholar
Vaughn, B. E., Vollenweider, M., Bost, K. K., & Azria-Evans, , Snider, J. B. (2003). Negative interactions and social competence for preschool children in two samples: Reconsidering the interpretation of aggressive behavior for young children. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 49, 245278.Google Scholar
Victor, J. B., Halverson, C. F. Jr. & Montague, R. B. (1985). Relations between reflection–impulsivity and behavioral impulsivity in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 21, 141148.Google Scholar
Vitaro, F., Gagnon, C., & Tremblay, R. E. (1990). Predicting stable peer rejection from kindergarten to grade one. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 257264.Google Scholar
Wasik, B. A., & Bond, M. A. (2001). Beyond the pages of a book: Interactive book reading and language development in preschool classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 243250.Google Scholar
Wasik, B. A., Bond, M. A., & Hindman, A. (2006). The effects of a language and literacy intervention on Head Start children and teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 6374.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—III. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Werthamer-Larsson, L., Kellam, S., & Wheeler, L. (1991). Effect of first-grade classroom environment on shy behavior, aggressive behavior, and concentration problems. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 585602.Google Scholar
White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles, D. J., Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). Measuring impulsivity and examining its relation to delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 192205.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in daycare and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 30, 679689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 1127.Google Scholar
Zill, N., Resnick, G., Kiim, K., O'Donnell, K., Sorongon, A., McKey, R. H., et al. (2003). Head Start FACES 2000: A whole-child perspective on program performance. Retrieved from USDHHS Administration for Child and Family webpage: http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/opre/hs/faces/reports/faces00Google Scholar