Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T18:21:29.486Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Autism and ostensive communication: The relevance of metarepresentation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Alan M. Leslie*
Affiliation:
MRC Cognitive Development Unit, University College London
Francesca Happé
Affiliation:
MRC Cognitive Development Unit, University College London
*
Requests for reprints should be sent to: Alan M. Leslie, MRC Cognitive Development Unit, 17 Gordon Street, London WC1H 0AH.

Abstract

Recent empirical and theoretical work on both normal and pathological development has led to the formulation of the metarepresentational conjecture for autism. This account of autistic development links the core impairments in imaginative abilities, communicative skills, and social competence to dysfunction of a single set of underlying cognitive mechanisms. In this context, Mundy and Sigman (1989) argue that the impairment of early gestural communication in autism cannot be accounted for by the metarepresentational theory. On the contrary, this early communication impairment in autism is highly consistent with a faulty theory-of-mind module. In normal development, this module is available from around the first year to handle estensive communication – shared pretense being a prime, though slightly later, example. We briefly consider the role in communication of perceptual processes that may trigger metarepresentation. This early theory-of-mind module, with its metarepresentational processes, provides the specific developmental basis for later versions of the child's theory of mind.

Type
Open Peer Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Attwood, A. H., Frith, U., & Hermelin, B. (1988). The understanding and use of interpersonal gestures by autistic and Down's syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18,241–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 379402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (in press). Are autistic children ‘be-haviorists’? An examination of their mental-physical and appearance-reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, G. (1972). A theory of play and fantasy. In Bateson, G., Steps to an ecology of mind (pp. 177193). Aylesbury: Chandler.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a “theory of mind”? In Lamb, M. E. & Sherod, L. R., (Eds.), Infant social cognition (pp. 333373). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1976). From communication to language–A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasser, V., Ulbaek, I., & Premack, D. (1989). The perception of intention. Science, 243, 365367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (1989). Autism. Oxford: Blackwell.Google ScholarPubMed
Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66, 377388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P., & Muncer, A. (1988, 07). The autistic child's understanding of beliefs and desires. Paper presented to the British Psychological Society Developmental Conference, Coleg Harlech.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1984). Early childhood autism and the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14,85–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (in press). On acquiring knowledge about people, and the capacity to pretend: A response to Leslie. Psychological Review.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1983, 07). Pretend play and representation in the second year of life. Paper presented to BPS Developmental Conference, Oxford.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1988a). Some implications of pretense for mechanisms underlying the child's theory of mind. In Astington, J., Harris, P., & Olson, D. (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 1946). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1988b). The necessity of illusion: Perception and thought in infancy. In Weiskrantz, L. (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 185210). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (in press). The theory of mind impairment in autism: Evidence for a modular mechanism of development? In Whiten, A. (Ed.), The emergence of mindreading. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (in press). Prospects for a cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson's choice. Psychological Review.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, P. (1984). The (four) whys and wherefores of play in cats: A review of functional, evolutionary, developmental and causal issues. In Smith, P. K. (Ed.), Play in animals and humans (pp. 7194). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 17, 349364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: Knowledge, belief and communication. Child Development, 60, 689700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreibman, L. (1988). Autism. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.Google ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1979). Nim. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children's reasoning about beliefs. Cognition, 30, 239277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed