Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T04:03:57.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Another look at imitation in autism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Tony Charman*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University College London
Simon Baron-Cohen
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychology and Child Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, University of London
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Tony Charman, Department of Psychology (Philips House), University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT. UK.

Abstract

Several authors have recently suggested that imitation may be a developmental “precursor” of a theory of mind and have linked impaired imitation in children with autism to their failure to develop a theory of mind. The present study investigated early-emerging procedural and gestural imitation abilities in children with autism. Children with autism were found to have intact basic-level gestural and procedural imitation. We discuss these results in terms of their relation to a specific developmental delay hypothesis of autism and re-assess the status of imitation as a developmental precursor of a theory of mind.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 139148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual notetaking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In Whiten, A. (Ed.), Natural theories of mind (pp. 233252). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to desire-belief psychology: The development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. (Eds.). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 5982). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839842.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. (1983). The Test for Reception of Grammar. London: Medical Research Council, Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. (1993). Annotation: Autism, executive functions and theory of mind: A neuropsychologi-cal perspective. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 34, 279293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 3746.Google Scholar
Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 281292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and social responsiveness in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 209226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeMyer, M. K. (1976). Motor, perceptual-motor and intellectual disabilities of autistic children. In Wing, L. (Ed.), Early childhood autism, Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
DeMyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. H., Bryson, C. Q., Pontius, W., & Kimberlin, C. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 264287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintillie, D. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. London: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Hammes, J. G. W., & Langdell, T. (1981). Precursors of symbol formation and childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 331346.Google Scholar
Heimann, M., Ullstadius, E., Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Imitation in autism. A preliminary research note. Behavioural Neurology, 5, 219227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertzig, M. E., Snow, M. E., & Sherman, M. (1989). Affect and cognition in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 195199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1993). Symbolic play in autism: A review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 281308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, V., & Prior, M. (1985). Motor imitation abilities and neurological signs in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 3745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Leekam, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Perrett, D., Milders, M., & Brown, S. (1993). Eye-direction detection: A dissociation between geometric and joint-attention skills in autism. Unpublished manuscript, University of Kent, Canterbury, UK.Google Scholar
Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 325339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., & Izard, C. E. (1984). The ontogenesis of human social signals: From biological imperative to symbol utilisation. In Fox, N. A. & Davison, R. J. (Eds.), The psychobiology of affective development (pp. 161206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988a). Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24, 470476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988b). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217225.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modelling and self-practice in infancy. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 139164). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing theories of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 335366). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702709.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behaviour and Development, 15, 479505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, S. B., Cutrer, P. S., Coplin, J. W., & Rodrigue, J. R. (1989). Do autistic children differ from retarded and normal children in Piagetian sensorimotor functioning? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 857864.Google Scholar
Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationships, conceptualisation and gesture imitations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 4562.Google Scholar
Rast, M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1991). Relations between imitation and object permanence in children with Down syndrome. Unpublished manuscript, Washington University, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Raven, J. (1956). The Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., & McEvoy, R. E. (1993, 03). Praxis in high-functioning persons with autism. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development conference, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruttar, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 139161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293302.Google Scholar
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Thatcher, M. (1977). An application of Piaget theory to autistic children (Doctoral dissertation, California Institute of Asian Studies, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 919B.Google Scholar
Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 20, 318337Google ScholarPubMed
Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. McV. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A. M., & Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364377Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992). Mental disorders: A glossary and guide to their classification in accordance with the 10th revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10). Geneva: Author.Google Scholar