Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T08:06:38.588Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Compulsivity is measurable across distinct psychiatric symptom domains and is associated with familial risk and reward-related attentional capture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 October 2019

Lucy Albertella*
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Samuel R. Chamberlain*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom Department of Psychiatry, Cambridge and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, United Kingdom
Mike E. Le Pelley
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, UNSW Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Lisa-Marie Greenwood
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, New South Wales, Australia Illawarra Health and Medical Research Institute, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, New South Wales, Australia
Rico SC Lee
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Lauren Den Ouden
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Rebecca A. Segrave
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Jon E. Grant*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, USA
Murat Yücel*
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
*
* Dr Lucy Albertella, Email: [email protected]
* Dr Samuel Chamberlain, Email: [email protected]
Jon E. Grant and Murat Yücel are joint senior authors
Jon E. Grant and Murat Yücel are joint senior authors
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

Compulsivity can be seen across various mental health conditions and refers to a tendency toward repetitive habitual acts that are persistent and functionally impairing. Compulsivity involves dysfunctional reward-related circuitry and is thought to be significantly heritable. Despite this, its measurement from a transdiagnostic perspective has received only scant research attention. Here we examine both the psychometric properties of a recently developed compulsivity scale, as well as its relationship with compulsive symptoms, familial risk, and reward-related attentional capture.

Methods.

Two-hundred and sixty individuals participated in the study (mean age = 36.0 [SD = 10.8] years; 60.0% male) and completed the Cambridge-Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale (CHI-T), along with measures of psychiatric symptoms and family history thereof. Participants also completed a task designed to measure reward-related attentional capture (n = 177).

Results.

CHI-T total scores had a normal distribution and acceptable Cronbach’s alpha (0.84). CHI-T total scores correlated significantly and positively (all p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) with Problematic Usage of the Internet, disordered gambling, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, alcohol misuse, and disordered eating. The scale was correlated significantly with history of addiction and obsessive-compulsive related disorders in first-degree relatives of participants and greater reward-related attentional capture.

Conclusions.

These findings suggest that the CHI-T is suitable for use in online studies and constitutes a transdiagnostic marker for a range of compulsive symptoms, their familial loading, and related cognitive markers. Future work should more extensively investigate the scale in normative and clinical cohorts, and the role of value-modulated attentional capture across compulsive disorders.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Compulsivity refers to the tendency towards undertaking repetitive, habitual actions, whereby the original goal of the act has been lostReference Dalley, Everitt and Robbins1 (cf. Reference Reference Luigjes, Lorenzetti and de Haan2). For example, an individual with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) may repeatedly check that the gas stove has been switched off for hours per time, despite only recently having already checked that it was indeed switched off. While OCD is the classic archetypal disorder of compulsivity, it is increasingly recognized that mental disorders listed in non-OCD DSM diagnostic categories also have compulsive features, notably gambling disorder, substance addictions, and binge-eating disorder.Reference Turton, Chami and Treasure3Reference Chamberlain and Grant5 These conditions collectively share a number of parallels including high rates of comorbid expression. In order to better understand the common etiological and biological predisposing factors towards these compulsive symptom types, it is necessary to identify transdiagnostic markers that cut across conventionally separate conditions. By identifying latent phenotypes that are dimensional in nature, existing in milder forms in the background population, and in more extreme forms across mental disorders, it is hoped that progress can be made in improving early detection, diagnostic classification systems, neurobiological models, and treatment approaches.Reference Cuthbert and Insel6Reference Chamberlain, Tiego and Fontenelle8

The Cambridge–Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale (CHI-T) is a 15-item scale that was recently developed to measure a broad range of compulsive traits. In an initial validation study conducted using in-person clinical assessments, the CHI-T had good psychometric properties, with total scores occupying a normative distribution, and convergent validity being demonstrated against relevant symptoms (correlating with OCD, gambling disorder, and substance use disorder symptoms).Reference Chamberlain and Grant5

To demonstrate the utility of the CHI-T scale as a transdiagnostic measure of compulsivity, it is important to show that it is associated with familial risk of manifest compulsive disorders, ideally also showing it is not associated with noncompulsive disorder(s). Research has consistently shown family history of addiction to be associated with addictive behaviorsReference Dawson, Harford and Grant9Reference King, Abrams and Wilkinson11 and family history of OCD to be associated with OCD diagnosis and/or symptoms.Reference Nestadt, Samuels and Riddle12, Reference Alsobrook, Leckman, Goodman, Rasmussen and Pauls13 However, no study to date has examined the relationship between familial risk and compulsivity transdiagnostically. This is likely related to the historical lack of a compulsivity measure that can be applied across different behavioral domains and that is sensitive to individual variations in compulsivity along a continuum in the general population.

High levels of compulsivity traits would also be expected to share some degree of neurocognitive correlates with addictive and compulsive disorders. Specifically, these neurocognitive markers may reflect processes that put individuals at risk of, or provide a predisposition toward, developing a range of compulsive behaviors. One cognitive risk marker that has been linked in various ways to compulsive addiction-related behaviors is the tendency to show strong attentional biases and approach responses toward reward-related cues, also known as “sign-tracking”.Reference Hearst and Jenkins14 Sign-tracking (tracking the signal), in contrast to goal-tracking, that is, approaching the location of reward delivery (tracking the goal), is thought to reflect an individual’s propensity to attribute incentive salience to Pavlovian signals of reward, such that these reward-signalling cues become attractive in their own right and can powerfully influence subsequent behavior and viewed as reflecting propensity to develop addictive behaviors.Reference Robinson and Flagel15, Reference Flagel, Akil and Robinson16 Importantly, while sign-tracking is generally recognized as a conditioned behavior directed toward reward cues, reward cues are not the only stimuli capable of eliciting a sign-tracking response. Safety signals, that is, stimuli that signal the omission of an expected aversive event (such as shock), also elicit a sign-tracking response, Reference Leclerc and Reberg17 suggesting that they also may be endowed with incentive salience (through their relationship with the absence of threat) and thereby capable of attracting attention and approach responses in their own right. For example, in contamination-based OCD, washing-related stimuli (eg, soap) may become safety signals through their pairing with reduced contamination threat and anxiety. In turn, these stimuli can acquire incentive salience (for sign-trackers), drawing attention and approach in their own right. To the extent that certain individuals attribute incentive salience to Pavlovian cues (reward-related or safety-related), such that associated behaviors may continue independently of the outcome, these individuals may be argued to be at risk of developing maladaptive behaviors. From this perspective, sign-tracking may be viewed as a transdiagnostic risk marker for compulsive or otherwise maladaptive cue-driven behaviors (for a more detailed account of this model, seeReference Albertella and Le Pelley18).

While much of the research on sign-tracking has used animal models, Le Pelley et al.Reference Le Pelley, Pearson, Griffiths and Beesley19 developed a procedure to assess an analogue of sign-tracking in human attention. This involved a visual search task, in which participants searched for and responded to a diamond target among circles on every trial (see Figure 1). Critically, one of the (nontarget) circles could be colored, either blue or orange (all other shapes were grey). The color of this color-singleton circle—referred to as the distractor—related directly to the size of the reward available on the current trial: one color (the high-reward color) signaled that a large reward was available for a correct response, and the other (low-reward) color signaled that a small reward was available. Notably, while the distractor signaled reward magnitude, it was not the target that participants responded to in order to receive that reward; thus distractors had a Pavlovian, but not instrumental, relationship with reward. The key finding was that responses to the target were significantly slower (but no more accurate) for trials with a high-reward distractor compared to trials with a low-reward distractor. This suggests that the signal of high reward was more likely to capture participants’ attention, slowing their response to the target—even though this enhanced capture was counterproductive, because it meant participants earned less on high-reward trials than would otherwise have been the case. This effect of reward on distraction is referred to as value-modulated attentional capture (VMAC) and may be considered to reflect the extent to which reward-signals come to influence behavior; that is, the propensity towards “attentional sign-tracking”. Consistent with the idea that this task may be considered an analogue of sign-tracking in animals, individual differences in VMAC have been linked to addictive behaviors.Reference Albertella, Copeland, Pearson, Watson, Wiers and Le Pelley20

Figure 1. Sequence of trial events in the visual search task. Participants responded to the orientation of the line segment (horizontal or vertical) within the diamond (target). One of the nontarget circles could be a colour singleton distractor. Fast, correct responses to the target received monetary reward, depending on the distractor colour. A distractor rendered in a high-reward colour signalled that this was a bonus trial on which a large reward could be won. If instead the search display contained a distractor rendered in a low-reward colour (or did not contain a colour singleton distractor), then the trial was a standard trial on which only a small reward was available. Slower response times (RTs) on trials with a high-reward distractor than trials with a low-reward distractor demonstrate value-modulated attentional capture (VMAC).

Thus, the aims of the current study were to: (i) further examine the CHI-T scale, and its relationship with relevant compulsive symptoms; (ii) examine CHI-T’s sensitivity to familial risk of compulsive symptoms; and (iii) understand CHI-T’s relationship with reward-related attentional capture, a cognitive process theorized to be crucial in compulsivity.

Material and Methods

Participants

Adult participants, aged 18–80 years, were recruited via Mechanical Turk, for a study advertised as exploring compulsivity. Mechanical Turk is a commonly used online recruitment tool for collecting data, in which individuals complete tasks for a set fee. It offers potential advantages over other recruitment methods in terms of rapidity of data collection; furthermore, Mechanical Turk workers are demographically diverse.Reference Buhrmester, Kwang and Gosling21, Reference Goodman, Cryder and Cheema22

Prior to taking part, each individual provided consent after reading the study information sheets, and proceeded to complete the online survey. Following completion of the survey, each participant received payment of $9 (US) ($6 plus bonus of $3). Exclusion criteria were: not willing to consent, out of age range, or not completing the survey. All study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee ethically reviewed and approved the study.

Online measures

All measures and tasks were presented using Inquisit. The following demographic information was collected: age, country of birth, gender, and ethnicity. Additionally, the following questionnaires were administered:

  • The Cambridge-Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale (CHI-T).Reference Chamberlain and Grant5 This is a 15-item scale covering broad aspects of compulsivity including the need for completion or perfection, reward-seeking, desire for high standards, and avoidance of situations that are hard to control. For each item, participants selected whether the statement applied to them by selecting “strongly disagree,” “disagree,” “agree,” or “strongly agree,” scored as 0–3 respectively. The measure of interest was the total score.

  • Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory Revised (OCI-R).Reference Foa, Huppert and Leiberg23 This is a previously validated 18-item scale enquiring about OC-related experiences over the preceding month. For each item the individual rated how distressed or bothered they had been by this over the past month (not at all, a little, moderately, a lot, or extremely, scored 0–4 respectively). The measure of interest was the total score.

  • Young’s Internet Addiction Test (IAT) Short Version.Reference Pawlikowski, Altstotter-Gleich and Brand24 This 12-item questionnaire was developed to measure problematic usage of the Internet. For each of 12-items (eg, ‘How often do you find that you stay online more often than intended?) the participants were asked to rate this over the past month. For each item, the response options were: never, rarely, sometimes, often, or very often, scored 0–4 respectively. The measure of interest was the total score.

  • Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI) is a 9-item measure of problem gambling severity (derived from the 31-item Canadian Problem Gambling IndexReference Ferris and Wynne25). For each item, the response option was: never, sometimes, most of the time, or always, scored 0–3 respectively. Total score was the measure of interest.

  • Brief Assessment Tool for Compulsivity Associated Problems (BATCAP).Reference Albertella and Le Pelley18 This is a recently developed tool designed to quantify relevant features of a range of compulsive symptom types, within the auspices of a single convenient instrument. Symptom domains were: alcohol use, gambling, compulsive eating, contamination compulsions, checking compulsions, just right and ordering compulsions, and compulsive Internet use. For each of these types of symptoms, individuals answered 6 questionsFootnote 1 covering time lost, distress, loss of control, functional impact, anxiety if prevented from doing the behavior, and strongest urge. Items 1–5 were adapted from the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive ScaleReference Goodman, Price and Rasmussen26 and Florida Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory.Reference Storch, Bagner and Merlo27 Item 6 was adapted from the Penn Alcohol Craving Scale.Reference Flannery, Volpicelli and Pettinati28 Each was rated on a 5-point scale, scored 0–4 respectively. The measure of interest here was total score for each symptom domain, and participants who had not endorsed a behavior in the past month were given 0 for that domain.

  • Family History of Compulsive Behaviors Scale. This is a 12-item scale designed to measure the presence of a range of compulsive behaviors and conditions, past or current, in first-degree family members. Response options range from 0 (no relatives) to 2 (multiple first-degree relatives) for each item. Six items ask about addiction-related behaviors/conditions (alcohol, gambling, and binge eating) and 7 items ask about OCD and related conditions (OCD subtypes, hoarding, obsessive compulsive personality disorder, body-focused repetitive behaviors, and tics). The measures of interest were the 2 subscale (addictions vs. OCD-related) total scores. As a control measure, we also asked about family history of psychosis and schizophrenia (yes/no). The Family History of Compulsive Behaviors Scale is presented in the supplementary materials.

  • Psychological Distress. Participants completed the brief Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21).Reference Lovibond and Lovibond29 The DASS-21 contains 21 items assessing depression, anxiety, and stress/tension symptoms over the past week. The measure of interest was total score, reflecting general psychological distress.

  • Short UPPS-P Impulsivity Scale (S-UPPS-P).Reference Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey and Karyadi30 This is a 20-item scale that measures impulsivity with 5 subscales: Negative Urgency, the tendency toward impulsive action when experiencing strong negative emotions; Positive Urgency, the tendency toward impulsive action when experiencing strong positive emotions; Lack of Perseverance; Lack of Premeditation; and Sensation Seeking. The current study used the total score, a measure of trait impulsivity, to control for its possible confounding influence on the relationship between CHI-T score and value-modulated attentional capture.Reference Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey and Karyadi

Value-modulated attentional capture task – reward-only variant

The visual search task used a reward-only variant of Le Pelley et al.’sReference Le Pelley, Pearson, Griffiths and Beesley19 (Experiment 2) VMAC procedure, modified to reflect reward-related attentional capture more specificallyFootnote 2.

All stimuli were presented on a black background. Each trial began with a central fixation cross, followed after 500 ms by the search display. The search display comprised 6 shapes—5 circles, and one diamond (the target)—arranged evenly around an imaginary ring (see Figure 1). Color set was blue and orange, with assignment of blue and orange to the roles of high-reward and low-reward colors being counterbalanced across participants. The diamond target contained a white line segment oriented either vertically or horizontally; all other shapes contained the same line segment tilted 45° randomly to the left or right. Participants’ task was to report the orientation of the line within the target as quickly as possible—by pressing either the ‘C’ key (horizontal) or ‘M’ key (vertical)—with faster responses earning more points.

Each trial-block of the task comprised 25 trials: 11 trials featuring a distractor rendered in the high-reward color, 11 trials with a distractor in the low-reward color, and 3 distractor-absent trials (in which all shapes were grey), in random order. For correct responses, on trials with a low-reward distractor and distractor-absent trials, participants won 0.1 points for every ms that their response time (RT) was below 1000 ms (so an RT of 600 ms would earn 40 points). Trials in which the display contained a high-reward distractor were labeled as bonus trials, and points were multiplied by 10 (so an RT of 600 ms would earn 400 points). Correct responses with RT greater than 1000 ms and incorrect responses earned no points. The search display remained on-screen until the participant responded or the trial timed-out (after 2 s). A feedback screen then appeared. On ‘standard’ (low-reward distractor or distractor-absent) trials, if the response was correct, feedback showed the number of points earned on that trial; if the response was incorrect, feedback showed “ERROR”; and if the trial timed-out feedback was “TOO SLOW: Please try to respond faster”. On bonus (high-reward) trials the corresponding feedback was accompanied by a box labeled “10 × bonus trial!.” Target location, distractor location, and target line segment orientation (vertical or horizontal) were randomly determined on each trial.

Participants were informed that the aim of the visual search task was to earn as many points as possible, and that they could receive a bonus $3 based upon their performance. Participants were further informed (1) that when a circle in the high-reward color was present in the search display it would be a bonus trial on which points were multiplied by 10, and (2) that when a circle in the low-reward color was present it would not be bonus trial. Participants completed five 25-trial blocks, taking a break between blocks; during this break they were shown the total number of points they had earned so far.

Typically, overall accuracy in this type of visual search task is relatively high, and analyses focus on differences in response time.Reference Le Pelley, Pearson, Griffiths and Beesley19, Reference Anderson, Laurent and Yantis31, Reference Anderson, Laurent and Yantis32 Following this approach, to assess the effect of reward, we calculated a VMAC score for each participant by subtracting response time on trials with a low-value distractor from response time on trials with a high-value distractor. A higher VMAC score indicates greater distraction by the high-reward distractor relative to the low-reward distractor; that is, a greater influence of reward on attentional capture. Only correct responses were analyzed, and participants with less than 50% overall accuracy were excluded. Since we were interested in the effect of reward on steady-state behavior, we calculated VMAC scores using data from the final 2 blocks (50 trials in total), when participants had had considerable experience of the color–reward relationships—as in previous research using the VMAC task.Reference Albertella, Watson, Yucel and Le Pelley33

Data analysis

Distributions of CHI-T total scores were characterized graphically in terms of any skew and outliers. Psychometric properties of the CHI-T were examined (Cronbach’s alpha). Simple relationships between CHI-T total scores and the other measures of interest were explored using correlation analyses (Spearman’s r). We report correlation p-values uncorrected, two-tailed, but these were only deemed statistically significant if they withstood Bonferroni correction for the number of measures examined per category of interest. In order to identify measures associated with CHI-T scores across the range of manifest compulsive symptom domains, controlling for inter-relationships across such measures (including general distress), secondary analysis was conducted using the statistical technique of partial least squares (PLS).Reference Chamberlain, Cavanagh and Boer34, Reference Cox and Gaudard35 PLS is a versatile multivariate technique that optimally explains relationships between a set of explanatory (X) and output (Y) variables. PLS offers advantages over conventional statistical approaches in that it is robust even when normal assumptions are violated (eg, in situations of collinearity); and is suitable even when there are a relatively large number of variables in comparison to the sample size. The PLS model was fitted using leave-one-out cross-validation (non-linear iterative partial least squares, NIPALS algorithm), and the optimal number of latent factors was selected by minimizing the predictive residual sum of the squares (PRESS). X variables significantly contributing to the model (ie, explaining significant variance in CHI-T scores, Y variable) were identified on the basis of 95% confidence intervals for bootstrap distribution of the standardized model coefficients not crossing zero (N = 2500 bootstraps). All analyses were conducted using JMP Pro software version 13.2.

The relationship between CHI-T and value-modulated attentional capture was assessed using correlation analyses, including partial correlation analysis controlling for psychological distress (DASS-21) and impulsivity (S-UPPS-P). Psychological distress and impulsivity were controlled for due to past research showing that these variables can influence compulsive behaviorsReference Verdejo-García, Lawrence and Clark36Reference Fullana, Mataix‐Cols and Trujillo39 as well as reward-related learning, Reference Anderson, Laurent and Yantis31, Reference Kashyap, Fontenelle and Miguel40, Reference Anderson, Leal, Hall, Yassa and Yantis41 and thereby have confounding potential.

Results

The overall sample comprised 260 individuals, of mean (standard deviation) age 36.0 (10.8) years, being 60.0% male, the overwhelming majority (>95%) of USA origin. The CHI-T total scores yielded a normal distribution with few outliers (n = 4 out of 260); see Figure 2. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.84, with all individual scale items exhibiting strong loading onto all other items (all alpha >0.82). Of the 260 participants, 44 participants did not proceed to the VMAC task. Of those who did, 10 participants did not finish it, and 29 achieved less than 50% accuracy (ie, numerically below chance). The remaining sample (N = 177) was used in the analyses involving VMAC scores.

Figure 2. Distribution of CHI-T total scores in the sample. Left—histogram; middle—box-whisker plot (the red bracket defines the shortest half of the data ie, the densest region); and right—Normal Quantile Plot.

CHI-T total scores were not correlated with age (r = –0.0610, p = 0.3271), nor did they differ as a function of gender (F [df = 1,258] = 0.8708, p = 0.3516), or ethnic group (F [df = 10,249] = 0.8588, p = 0.5725). Correlations between CHI-T total scores and different compulsive symptom types are summarized in Table 1, where it can be seen that CHI-T scores correlated significantly and positively (all p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction) with Problematic Usage of the Internet, disordered gambling, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, alcohol misuse, and disordered eating. CHI-T scores also correlated with psychological distress (DASS-21) as expected (r = 0.4495, p < 0.001). Table 1 also shows the correlations between CHI-T total score and family history of addiction and OCD-related disorders. A higher CHI-T score was significantly associated both with a greater family history score of addictions and a greater family history score of OCD-related disorders (both p < 0.05). CHI-T scores did not differ significantly between those participants with and without a history of psychosis or schizophrenia in a first-degree relative (F [df = 16,225] = 2.5721, p = 0.1101).

Table 1. Correlations between CHI-T total scores and different compulsive symptom domains.

Abbreviations: BATCAP, Brief Assessment Tool for Compulsivity Associated Problems; IAT, Internet addiction test; PGSI, Pathological Gambling Symptoms Inventory; OCI-R, Obsessive Compulsive Inventory Revised; OCRDs, obsessive-compulsive and related disorders.

p-values are uncorrected.

PLS identified an optimal model with one latent factor, accounting for 44.7% of variance in the explanatory (X) measures (ie, compulsive symptom scores, family history of addiction or psychosis, and psychological distress) and 16.8% of variation in CHI-T total scores. Higher levels of each type of compulsive symptom were statistically significant predictors of higher CHI-T scores, as was family history of addiction and OC-related disorders, and general distress ( Figure 3; each p < 0.05 by bootstrap). Family history of psychosis/schizophrenia was not a statistically significant predictor of CHI-T scores.

Figure 3. Standardized model coefficients for PLS model, linking each explanatory (X) variable to CHI-T scores (Y). All explanatory variables were statistically significant predictors of higher CHI-T scores (p < 0.05, bootstrap) except for family history of psychotic spectrum disorder.

Correlation analyses showed a significant association between CHI-T scores and VMAC scores (r = .26, p < .001), with higher trait compulsivity being associated with greater attentional capture by reward-related stimuli. This result remained significant after controlling for the influence of psychological distress and impulsivity (r = .20, p = .008). Figure 4 shows the scatterplot of VMAC score as a function of CHI-T score.

Figure 4. A scatterplot of VMAC score (response time for trials with a distractor that signaled high-reward minus response time for trials with a distractor that signaled low-reward) as a function of CHI-T score.

Discussion

The current study examined the extent to which a transdiagnostic measure of compulsivity, the CHI-T scale, was related to severity of symptoms across compulsive behaviors as well as compulsivity-related familial risk and reward-related attentional capture. This study also served to further explore psychometric properties of the CHI-T, this time applied to an online research study. The key finding was that total scores on the scale were significantly associated with severity of symptoms across a range of compulsive behaviors, including gambling, internet use, alcohol use, binge eating, and OCD-related compulsions. Furthermore, CHI-T scores were associated with familial risk of addictions as well as familial risk of OCD and related conditions, but not familial risk for psychosis/schizophrenia. These relationships were demonstrated using conventional correlations but also controlling for inter-dependence of variables using partial least squares (PLS). Finally, higher CHI-T scores were associated with greater reward-related attentional capture, implicating this as a core cognitive process that may contribute to a range of compulsive tendencies.

The finding that CHI-T score correlated with symptom severity across different compulsive behaviors adds to a previous study of CHI-T by extending its convergent validity with a wider range of behaviors, particularly Internet use and eating, and showing that it can also be used in online studies (whereas the initial validation study was in-person).Reference Chamberlain and Grant5 As predicted, CHI-T was associated with familial risk of addiction and OC related disorders, highlighting the potential use of this scale as a measure that is sensitive to individual variations in compulsivity-related risk. For instance, CHI-T may be useful in examining how different risk factors interact to increase risk of developing a range of compulsive disorders in the general population. This avoids confounds common to studies that use clinical samples (eg, medication, chronicity, etc.). Also, this avoids the problem of having to use different scales for each behavior, which could be differentially sensitive to gauging variations in risk (especially at lower end of the continuum).

Finally, the finding that higher levels of trait compulsivity on the CHI-T were related to greater reward-related attentional capture implicates attentional sign tracking as a cognitive process that may be involved in a range of compulsive symptom types. This finding will allow human compulsivity research to draw upon the wealth of knowledge that has been derived from animal studies on sign-tracking, including the associative processes that underlie it, the neurological underpinnings, factors associated with risk (eg, early trauma, adolescent cannabis use, impulsivity, genetics), and potential targets for behavioral and pharmacological interventions.Reference Robinson and Flagel15, Reference Flagel, Akil and Robinson16, Reference Fraser and Janak43Reference Yager and Robinson46

Several limitations should be considered regarding this study. The survey was conducted online, with all the inherent limitations thereof. For example, online assessment is unlikely to be as accurate as in-person clinical assessment in terms of precise quantification of psychiatric symptoms. Nonetheless, the study demonstrates the feasibility of using the current scale for online research. The survey respondents may have had participation bias, including due to the nature of the study advertisements, and thus the results may not generalize to the background population or other cohorts. Another limitation related to the online method is the relatively high number of participants (around 15%) who did not perform above chance level 50%. The high error rate may be related to the online nature of the study, in which participants are not supervised and thereby may be less attentive than in a strictly controlled lab setting, especially as the cognitive task was administered at the end of a 40-min questionnaire battery. Nonetheless, given that the learning that drives the VMAC effect should draw attention away from the target, then the relatively high error rate is not unexpected, especially in light of the variant used here, in which punishment of incorrect responses did not occur. Future studies using this task may benefit from exploring how errors themselves are related to compulsivity. Another limitation of the study is that we did not obtain more detailed background demographic information about the participants, such as levels of education. Finally, future research will benefit from comparing reward-related attentional capture with other cognitive measures that have commonly been found to be associated with compulsive symptoms in OCD, such as attentional set-shifting deficits and avoidance, habit, and/or reversal learning abnormalities.Reference Gillan, Papmeyer and Morein-Zamir 47Reference Chamberlain, Menzies and Hampshire50

In summary, this study demonstrated that a transdiagnostic compulsivity scale was sensitive to a range of compulsive symptom types, and to family history of compulsive symptoms, controlling for general distress. Transdiagnostic compulsivity was also significantly related to reward-related attentional capture, a cognitive process that may thus play a key role across different compulsive disorders. Because transdiagnostic compulsivity is a relatively neglected research topic, we call for more research in this area, which might also explore biological underpinnings and genetics.

Funding

Murat Yücel has received funding from the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (APP#1117188), the Australian Research Council, the David Winston Turner Endowment Fund, and Monash University. He has also received funding from the law firms in relation to expert witness report/statement. Dr Chamberlain’s role in this study was funded by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Fellowship (Reference 110049/Z/15/Z). Dr Chamberlain consults for Cambridge Cognition, Shire, and Promentis. Dr Chamberlain receives a stipend for his role as Associate Editor at Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews; and at Comprehensive Psychiatry. Dr Grant has received research grants from NIMH, National Center for Responsible Gaming, and Forest and Roche Pharmaceuticals. Dr Grant receives yearly compensation from Springer Publishing for acting as Editor-in-Chief of the Journal of Gambling Studies and has received royalties from Oxford University Press, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., Norton Press, and McGraw Hill. Mike Le Pelley has received funding from the ARC (DP170101715).

Disclosure.

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Supplementary Materials.

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1092852919001330.

Footnotes

1 For the internet use area, participants answered only 3 questions, on time spent, anxiety if prevented, and strongest urge (items 1, 5, and 6 of the BATCAP scale).

2 In Le Pelley et al.’s original version of the task, incorrect responses resulted in loss of the amount that would otherwise have been won as a reward. By contrast, in the current version of this task errors did not result in losses. This modification was made to ensure performance was less likely to be confounded by loss-related sensitivity and/or processes, as these are not central to sign-tracking.

References

Dalley, JW, Everitt, BJ, Robbins, TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011;69(4):680694.Google Scholar
Luigjes, J, Lorenzetti, V, de Haan, S, et al.Defining compulsive behavior. Neuropsychol Rev. 2019;29:413.Google Scholar
Turton, R, Chami, R, Treasure, J. Emotional eating, binge eating and animal models of binge-type eating disorders. Curr Obesity Rep. 2017;6(2):217228.Google Scholar
Yücel, M, Oldenhof, E, Ahmed, SH, et al.A transdiagnostic dimensional approach towards a neuropsychological assessment for addiction: an international Delphi consensus study. Addiction. 2018;114(6):10951109.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Grant, JE. Initial validation of a transdiagnostic compulsivity questionnaire: the Cambridge–Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale. CNS Spectrums. 2018;23:17.Google Scholar
Cuthbert, BN, Insel, TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med. 2013;11:126.Google Scholar
Insel, T, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, et al.Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):748751.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Tiego, J, Fontenelle, L, et al. Fractionation of impulsive and compulsive trans-diagnostic phenotypes and their longitudinal associations. bioRxiv. 2019:570218.Google Scholar
Dawson, DA, Harford, TC, Grant, BF. Family history as a predictor of alcohol dependence. Alcohol. 1992;16(3):572575.Google Scholar
Merikangas, KR, Stolar, M, Stevens, DE, et al.Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(11):973979.Google Scholar
King, SM, Abrams, K, Wilkinson, T. Personality, gender, and family history in the prediction of college gambling. J Gambling Stud. 2010;26(3):347359.Google Scholar
Nestadt, G, Samuels, J, Riddle, M, et al.A family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(4):358363.Google Scholar
Alsobrook, JP II, Leckman, JF, Goodman, WK, Rasmussen, SA, Pauls, DL. Segregation analysis of obsessive‐compulsive disorder using symptom‐based factor scores. Am J Med Genet. 1999;88(6):669675.Google Scholar
Hearst, E, Jenkins, HM. Sign-Tracking: The Stimulus-Reinforcer Relation And Directed Action. Austin: Psychonomic Society; 1974.Google Scholar
Robinson, TE, Flagel, SB. Dissociating the predictive and incentive motivational properties of reward-related cues through the study of individual differences. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65(10):869873.Google Scholar
Flagel, SB, Akil, H, Robinson, TE. Individual differences in the attribution of incentive salience to reward-related cues: implications for addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:139148.Google Scholar
Leclerc, R, Reberg, D. Sign-tracking in aversive conditioning. Learn Motiv. 1980;11(3):302317.Google Scholar
Albertella, L, Le Pelley, ME, Chamberlain SR, et al.Reward-related attentional capture is associated with severity of obsessive-compulsive and addictive behaviours. Psychol Addict Behav. 2019;33(5):495502.Google Scholar
Le Pelley, ME, Pearson, D, Griffiths, O, Beesley, T. When goals conflict with values: Counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli. J Exp Psychol. 2015;144(1):158.Google Scholar
Albertella, L, Copeland, J, Pearson, D, Watson, P, Wiers, RW, & Le Pelley, ME. Selective attention moderates the relationship between attentional capture by signals of nondrug reward and illicit drug use. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2017;175, 99105.Google Scholar
Buhrmester, M, Kwang, T, Gosling, SD. Amazon's mechanical turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6(1):35.Google Scholar
Goodman, JK, Cryder, CE, Cheema, A. Data collection in a flat world: The strengths and weaknesses of Mechanical Turk samples. J Behav Decis Making. 2013;26(3):213224.Google Scholar
Foa, EB, Huppert, JD, Leiberg, S, et al.The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychol Assess. 2002;14(4):485.Google Scholar
Pawlikowski, M, Altstotter-Gleich , C, Brand, M. Validation and psychometric properties of a short version of Young's Internet Addiction Test. Comput Human Behav. 2013;29(3):12121223.Google Scholar
Ferris, JA, Wynne, HJ. The Canadian Problem Gambling Index. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse; 2001.Google Scholar
Goodman, WK, Price, LH, Rasmussen, SA, et al.The yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale: II. Validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46(11):10121016.Google Scholar
Storch, EA, Bagner, D, Merlo, LJ, et al.Florida obsessive‐compulsive inventory: Development, reliability, and validity. J Clin Psychol. 2007;63(9):851859.Google Scholar
Flannery, B, Volpicelli, J, Pettinati, H. Psychometric properties of the Penn alcohol craving scale. Alcohol. 1999;23(8):12891295.Google Scholar
Lovibond, PF, Lovibond, SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Beha Res Ther. 1995;33(3):335343.Google Scholar
Cyders, MA, Littlefield, AK, Coffey, S, Karyadi, KA. Examination of a short English version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale. Addict Behav. 2014;39(9):13721376.Google Scholar
Anderson, BA, Laurent, PA, Yantis, S. Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture. PLoS One. 2011;6(11):e27926.Google Scholar
Anderson, BA, Laurent, PA, Yantis, S. Value-driven attentional capture. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2011;108(25):1036710371.Google Scholar
Albertella, L, Watson, P, Yucel, M, Le Pelley, M. Persistence of value-modulated attentional capture is associated with risky alcohol use. Addict Behav Rep. 2019;10:100195.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Cavanagh, JT, de Boer, P, et al.Treatment-resistant depression and peripheral C-reactive protein. Br J Psychiatry. 2018;214:1119.Google Scholar
Cox, I, Gaudard, M. Discovering Partial Least Squares with JMP. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.; 2013.Google Scholar
Verdejo-García, A, Lawrence, AJ, Clark, L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(4):777810.Google Scholar
Engel, K, Schaefer, M, Stickel, A, Binder, H, Heinz, A, Richter, C. The role of psychological distress in relapse prevention of alcohol addiction. Can high scores on the SCL-90-R predict alcohol relapse? Alcohol Alcohol. 2015;51(1):2731.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Stochl, J, Redden, SA, Grant, JE. Latent traits of impulsivity and compulsivity: toward dimensional psychiatry. Psychol Med. 2018;48(5):810821.Google Scholar
Fullana, , Mataix‐Cols, D, Trujillo, JL, et al.Personality characteristics in obsessive‐compulsive disorder and individuals with subclinical obsessive‐compulsive problems. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43(4):387398.Google Scholar
Kashyap, H, Fontenelle, LF, Miguel, EC, et al.Impulsive compulsivity’in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A phenotypic marker of patients with poor clinical outcome. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46(9):11461152.Google Scholar
Anderson, BA, Leal, SL, Hall, MG, Yassa, MA, Yantis, S. The attribution of value-based attentional priority in individuals with depressive symptoms. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;14(4):12211227.Google Scholar
Ochoa, C, Álvarez‐Moya, EM, Penelo, E, et al.Decision‐making deficits in pathological gambling: The role of executive functions, explicit knowledge and impulsivity in relation to decisions made under ambiguity and risk. Am J Addict. 2013;22(5):492499.Google Scholar
Fraser, KM, Janak, PH. Long‐lasting contribution of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens core, but not dorsal lateral striatum, to sign‐tracking. Eur J Neurosci. 2017;46(4):20472055.Google Scholar
Schoch, H, Huerta, M, Ruiz, C, et al.Adolescent cannabinoid exposure effects on natural reward seeking and learning in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2018;235(1):121134.Google Scholar
Ahrens, AM, Singer, BF, Fitzpatrick, CJ, Morrow, JD, Robinson, TE. Rats that sign-track are resistant to Pavlovian but not instrumental extinction. Behav Brain Res. 2016;296:418430.Google Scholar
Yager, LM, Robinson, TE. A classically conditioned cocaine cue acquires greater control over motivated behavior in rats prone to attribute incentive salience to a food cue. Psychopharmacology. 2013;226(2):217228.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Papmeyer, M, Morein-Zamir , S, et al.Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(7):718726.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW, Gillan, CM, Smith, DG, de Wit, S, Ersche, KD. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(1):8191.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Morein-Zamir , S, Urcelay, GP, et al.Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(8):631638.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Menzies, L, Hampshire, A, et al.Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321(5887):421422.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Sequence of trial events in the visual search task. Participants responded to the orientation of the line segment (horizontal or vertical) within the diamond (target). One of the nontarget circles could be a colour singleton distractor. Fast, correct responses to the target received monetary reward, depending on the distractor colour. A distractor rendered in a high-reward colour signalled that this was a bonus trial on which a large reward could be won. If instead the search display contained a distractor rendered in a low-reward colour (or did not contain a colour singleton distractor), then the trial was a standard trial on which only a small reward was available. Slower response times (RTs) on trials with a high-reward distractor than trials with a low-reward distractor demonstrate value-modulated attentional capture (VMAC).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Distribution of CHI-T total scores in the sample. Left—histogram; middle—box-whisker plot (the red bracket defines the shortest half of the data ie, the densest region); and right—Normal Quantile Plot.

Figure 2

Table 1. Correlations between CHI-T total scores and different compulsive symptom domains.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Standardized model coefficients for PLS model, linking each explanatory (X) variable to CHI-T scores (Y). All explanatory variables were statistically significant predictors of higher CHI-T scores (p < 0.05, bootstrap) except for family history of psychotic spectrum disorder.

Figure 4

Figure 4. A scatterplot of VMAC score (response time for trials with a distractor that signaled high-reward minus response time for trials with a distractor that signaled low-reward) as a function of CHI-T score.

Supplementary material: File

Albertella et al. supplementary material

Albertella et al. supplementary material

Download Albertella et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17.1 KB