Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T10:14:38.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The clinical relevance of attentional bias in substance use disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 May 2013

Matt Field*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
Reshmi Marhe
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Ingmar H. A. Franken
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Address for correspondence: Prof. Matt Field, School of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, L69 7ZA, UK. (Email [email protected])

Abstract

Individuals with substance use disorders typically show an “attentional bias” for substance-related cues: Those cues are able to grab and hold the attention, in preference to other cues in the environment. We discuss the theoretical context for this work before reviewing the measurement of attentional bias, and its relationship to motivational state and relapse to substance use after a period of abstinence. Finally, we discuss the implications of this research for the treatment of substance use disorders. We conclude that attentional bias is associated with subjective craving, and that moment-by-moment fluctuations in attentional bias may precede relapse to substance use. The evidence regarding the predictive relationship between attentional bias assessed in treatment contexts and subsequent relapse is inconsistent. Furthermore, there is currently insufficient evidence to endorse attentional bias modification as a treatment for substance use disorders. Clinical implications and suggestions for future research are highlighted.

Type
Review Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1.Cisler, JM, Koster, EHW. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010; 30(2): 203216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Field, M, Cox, WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008; 97(1–2): 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Franken, IHA. Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003; 27(4): 563579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.Google Scholar
5.Robinson, TE, Berridge, KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1993; 18(3): 247291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Goldstein, RZ, Volkow, ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011; 12(11): 652669.Google Scholar
7.Stacy, AW, Wiers, RW. Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010; 6: 551575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Cox, WM, Fadardi, JS, Pothos, EM. The addiction-stroop test: theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychol Bull. 2006; 132(3): 443476.Google Scholar
9.Noël, X, Colmant, M, Van Der Linden, M, etal. Time course of attention for alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholic patients: the role of initial orienting. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res. 2006; 30(11): 18711877.Google Scholar
10.Ataya, AF, Adams, S, Mullings, E, etal. Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012; 121(1–2): 148151.Google Scholar
11.Field, M, Christiansen, P. Commentary on ‘Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias.’ Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012; 124(3): 189190.Google Scholar
12.Mogg, K, Bradley, BP, Field, M, De Houwer, J. Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. Addiction. 2003; 98(6): 825836.Google Scholar
13.Littel, M, Euser, AS, Munafò, MR, Franken, IHA. Electrophysiological indices of biased cognitive processing of substance-related cues: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012; 36(8): 18031816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Luijten, M, Veltman, DJ, den Brink, WV, etal. Neurobiological substrate of smoking-related attentional bias. Neuroimage. 2011; 54(3): 23742381.Google Scholar
15.Luijten, M, Veltman, DJ, Hester, R, etal. Brain activation associated with attentional bias in smokers is modulated by a dopamine antagonist. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012; 37(13): 27722779.Google Scholar
16.Janes, AC, Pizzagalli, DA, Richardt, S, etal. Brain reactivity to smoking cues prior to smoking cessation predicts ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 67(8): 722729.Google Scholar
17.Nestor, L, McCabe, E, Jones, J, Clancy, L, Garavan, H. Differences in “bottom-up” and “top-down” neural activity in current and former cigarette smokers: evidence for neural substrates which may promote nicotine abstinence through increased cognitive control. Neuroimage. 2011; 56(4): 22582275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Vollstädt-Klein, S, Loeber, S, Richter, A, etal. Validating incentive salience with functional magnetic resonance imaging: association between mesolimbic cue reactivity and attentional bias in alcohol-dependent patients. Addict Biol. 2012; 17(4): 807816.Google Scholar
19.Hester, R, Garavan, H. Neural mechanisms underlying drug-related cue distraction in active cocaine users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009; 93(3): 270277.Google Scholar
20.Janes, AC, Pizzagalli, DA, Richardt, S, etal. Neural substrates of attentional bias for smoking-related cues: an fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35(12): 23392345.Google Scholar
21.Cox, WM, Pothos, EM, Hosier, SG. Cognitive-motivational predictors of excessive drinkers’ success in changing. Psychopharmacology. 2007; 192(4): 499510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Carpenter, KM, Schreiber, E, Church, S, McDowell, D. Drug Stroop performance: relationships with primary substance of use and treatment outcome in a drug-dependent outpatient sample. Addict Behav. 2006; 31(1): 174181.Google Scholar
23.Cox, WM, Hogan, LM, Kristian, MR, Race, JH. Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers’ treatment outcome. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002; 68(3): 237243.Google Scholar
24.Marissen, MAE, Franken, IHA, Waters, AJ, etal. Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following treatment. Addiction. 2006; 101(9): 13061312.Google Scholar
25.Waters, AJ, Shiffman, S, Sayette, MA, etal. Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. Health Psychol. 2003; 22(4): 378387.Google Scholar
26.Powell, J, Dawkins, L, West, R, Powell, J, Pickering, A. Relapse to smoking during unaided cessation: clinical, cognitive and motivational predictors. Psychopharmacology. 2010; 212(4): 537549.Google Scholar
27.Waters, AJ, Shiffman, S, Bradley, BP, Mogg, K. Attentional shifts to smoking cues in smokers. Addiction. 2003; 98(10): 14091417.Google Scholar
28.Field, M, Mogg, K, Mann, B, Bennett, GA, Bradley, BP. Attentional biases in abstinent alcoholics and their association with craving. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013; 27(1): 7180.Google Scholar
29.Marhe, R, Luitjen, M, van de Wetering, BJM, Smits, M, Franken, IHA. Individual differences in anterior cingulate activation associated with attentional bias predict cocaine use after treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013; 38(6): 10851093.Google Scholar
30.Waters, AJ, Marhe, R, Franken, IHA. Attentional bias to drug cues is elevated before and during temptations to use heroin and cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 2012; 219(3): 909921.Google Scholar
31.Marhe, R, Waters, AJ, van de Wetering, BJM, Franken, IHA. Implicit and explicit drug-related cognitions during detoxification treatment are associated with drug relapse: an ecological momentary assessment study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2013; 81(1): 112.Google Scholar
32.Field, M, Munafò, MR, Franken, IHA. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull. 2009; 135(4): 589607.Google Scholar
33.Preston, KL, Vahabzadeh, M, Schmittner, J, etal. Cocaine craving and use during daily life. Psychopharmacology. 2009; 207(2): 291301.Google Scholar
34.Field, M, Powell, H. Stress increases attentional bias for alcohol cues in social drinkers who drink to cope. Alcohol Alcohol. 2007; 42(6): 560566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Schoenmakers, T, Wiers, RW, Field, M. Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers. Psychopharmacology. 2008; 197(1): 169178.Google Scholar
36.Fernie, G, Christiansen, P, Cole, JC, Rose, AK, Field, M. Effects of 0.4g/kg alcohol on attentional bias and alcohol-seeking behaviour in heavy and moderate social drinkers. J Psychopharmacol. 2012; 26(7): 10171025.Google Scholar
37.Field, M, Mogg, K, Bradley, BP. Eye movements to smoking-related cues: effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology. 2004; 173(1): 116123.Google Scholar
38.Field, M, Rush, M, Cole, J, Goudie, A. The smoking Stroop and delay discounting in smokers: effects of environmental smoking cues. J Psychopharmacol. 2007; 21(6): 603610.Google Scholar
39.Cox, WM, Yeates, GN, Regan, CM. Effects of alcohol cues on cognitive processing in heavy and light drinkers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999; 55(1–2): 8589.Google Scholar
40.Field, M, Hogarth, L, Bleasdale, D, etal. Alcohol expectancy moderates attentional bias for alcohol cues in light drinkers. Addiction. 2011; 106(6): 10971103.Google Scholar
41.Jones, A, Hogarth, L, Christiansen, P, etal. Reward expectancy promotes generalized increases in attentional bias for rewarding stimuli. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2012; 65(12): 23332342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Field, M, Eastwood, B. Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology. 2005; 183(3): 350357.Google Scholar
43.Field, M, Duka, T, Eastwood, B, etal. Experimental manipulation of attentional biases in heavy drinkers: do the effects generalise? Psychopharmacology. 2007; 192(4): 593608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Field, M, Duka, T, Tyler, E, Schoenmakers, T. Attentional bias modification in tobacco smokers. Nicotine Tob Res. 2009; 11(7): 812822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Attwood, AS, O'Sullivan, H, Leonards, U, Mackintosh, B, Munafò, MR. Attentional bias training and cue reactivity in cigarette smokers. Addiction. 2008; 103(11): 18751882.Google Scholar
46.McHugh, RK, Murray, HW, Hearon, BA, Calkins, AW, Otto, MW. Attentional bias and craving in smokers: the impact of a single attentional training session. Nicotine Tob Res. 2010; 12(12): 12611264.Google Scholar
47.Fadardi, JS, Cox, WM. Reversing the sequence: reducing alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009; 101(3): 137145.Google Scholar
48.Schoenmakers, TM, de Bruin, M, Lux, IFM, etal. Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010; 109(1–3): 3036.Google Scholar
49.MacLeod, C, Mathews, A. Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012; 8: 189217.Google Scholar