Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T10:45:39.899Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychological Resources and Driving Status: A Study of Current and Former Drivers 55 Years of Age and Older

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 May 2021

Garrett Kafka
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Safe Driving, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario.
Arne Stinchcombe*
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Safe Driving, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario. Department of Recreation and Leisure Studies, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario.
Nadia Mullen
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Safe Driving, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario.
Bruce Weaver
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Safe Driving, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario. Northern Ontario School of Medicine, Thunder Bay, Ontario.
Michel Bédard
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Safe Driving, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario. Northern Ontario School of Medicine, Thunder Bay, Ontario. Centre for Applied Health Research, St. Joseph’s Care Group, Thunder Bay, Ontario. Department of Health Sciences, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario.
*
La correspondance et les demandes de tirés-à-part doivent être adressées à : / Correspondence and requests for offprints should be sent to: Arne Stinchcombe, Ph.D. 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way St. Catharines, Ontario, L2S 3A1Canada ([email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Psychological resources can help individuals adjust to changes associated with aging. In this study, we examined the effect of demographic, health, and psychological resource variables in explaining driving status among adults 55 years and older. A convenience sample of 222 adults between the ages of 55 and 91 years (mean = 72.20 years) completed questionnaires that included measures of driving status, self-rated health, and psychological resources (e.g., life control, life purpose, and locus of control). Multiple logistic regression models that controlled for confounders were constructed with driver status (i.e., current driver or former driver) as the outcome. Former drivers were older, reported being in poorer health, and reported more depression symptoms. After controlling for age and health, current drivers reported higher levels of life control and life purpose and a more internal locus of control. Results highlight the importance of considering psychological resources when examining driving cessation.

Résumé

RÉSUMÉ

Les ressources psychologiques intérieures peuvent aider les individus à s’adapter aux changements survenant lors du vieillissement. Dans cette étude, nous avons examiné l’effet de variables démographiques, liées à la santé et aux ressources psychologiques intérieures pour expliquer le statut de conduite automobile chez des adultes de 55 ans et plus. L’échantillon de convenance comprenait 222 adultes âgés de 55 à 91 ans (moyenne = 72,20). Les participants ont rempli des questionnaires comportant des mesures associées à la conduite automobile, à l’auto-évaluation de la santé et de ressources psychologiques (p. ex. sentiment de contrôle et sens donné à la vie, locus de contrôle). Des modèles de régression logistique multiple tenant compte de facteurs confondants ont été construits avec comme variable principale le statut de conduite automobile (conducteur actuel ou ancien conducteur). Les anciens conducteurs étaient plus âgés, se déclaraient en moins bonne santé et présentaient davantage de symptômes de dépression. Après avoir contrôlé pour l’âge et l’état de santé, il est ressorti des analyses effectuées que les conducteurs actuels déclaraient ressentir des niveaux plus élevés de contrôle et de sens dans leur vie, et un locus de contrôle plus interne. Ces résultats montrent l’importance de prendre en compte les ressources psychologiques lors d’évaluations pouvant mener à la cessation de la conduite automobile.

Type
Article
Copyright
© Canadian Association on Gerontology 2021

Older drivers are the fastest growing segment among licensed drivers in Canada, a trend that is expected to continue with the greying of the Canadian population (Canadian Association of Occupational Therapists, 2009). Driving remains a primary means of community mobility among older Canadians, yet, changes in health and functioning associated with the aging process increase the likelihood of driving cessation (Turcotte, Reference Turcotte2012). Importantly, on average, men will depend on other forms of transportation for 7 years following driving cessation, whereas women drivers of the same age will be dependent for an average of 10 years (Foley, Heimovitz, Gurainik, & Brock, Reference Foley, Heimovitz, Gurainik and Brock2002).

In addition to offering opportunities for community mobility, driving contributes to quality of life and is associated with identity, role, and a sense of freedom (Bauer, Rottunda, & Adler, Reference Bauer, Rottunda and Adler2003; Pachana, Jetten, Gustafsson, & Liddle, Reference Pachana, Jetten, Gustafsson and Liddle2017). It follows that driving cessation is associated with several negative physical and mental health outcomes. A systematic review of 16 studies and meta-analysis of 5 of those studies confirmed associations between driving cessation and negative outcomes related to physical/mental health and social/cognitive functioning (Chihuri et al., Reference Chihuri, Mielenz, DiMaggio, Betz, DiGuiseppi and Jones2016). The meta-analysis demonstrated that driving cessation was associated with close to double the odds of depression among older adults (odds ratio = 1.91, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.61–2.27) and greater odds of long-term care placement and death (Chihuri et al., Reference Chihuri, Mielenz, DiMaggio, Betz, DiGuiseppi and Jones2016).

Driving is a complex task that requires cognitive, sensory, and psychomotor skills (Anstey, Wood, Lord, & Walker, Reference Anstey, Wood, Lord and Walker2005). Research indicates that age-associated changes that affect driving performance begin to emerge by the time an individual reaches 55 years and become more pronounced with age (Persson, Reference Persson1993). Decreases in vision and functional status, and the prevalence of medical conditions are common aging experiences that are associated with driving cessation (Campbell, Bush, & Hale, Reference Campbell, Bush and Hale1993; Marottoli et al., Reference Marottoli, Ostfeld, Merrill, Perlman, Foley and Cooney1993). In addition, sociodemographic variables such as age, gender (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Bush and Hale1993; Marottoli et al., Reference Marottoli, Ostfeld, Merrill, Perlman, Foley and Cooney1993), income (Dellinger, Sehgal, Sleet, & Barrett-Connor, Reference Dellinger, Sehgal, Sleet and Barrett-Connor2001; Gilley et al., Reference Gilley, Wilson, Bennett, Stebbins, Bernard and Whalen1991; Marottoli et al., Reference Marottoli, Ostfeld, Merrill, Perlman, Foley and Cooney1993), and geographic location (Kington, Reuben, Rogowski, & Lillard, Reference Kington, Reuben, Rogowski and Lillard1994) along with confidence in one’s own skills (Brayne et al., Reference Brayne, Dufouil, Ahmed, Dening, Chi and McGee2000) are known to influence an individual’s decision to stop driving.

Numerous studies have examined visual, cognitive, and psychomotor abilities, and medical conditions as determinants of driving cessation (e.g., Ball et al., Reference Ball, Roenker, Wadley, Edwards, Roth and McGwin2006; Kington et al., Reference Kington, Reuben, Rogowski and Lillard1994). For example, Dugan and Lee (Reference Dugan and Lee2013) examined biopsychosocial risk factors for driving cessation by analyzing six waves of the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) study, including only participants who were 65 years of age and older. Their results showed that risk factors for driving cessation included older age, female gender, minority race, poor cognition, physical limitations, and chronic disease (e.g., diabetes). Other research points to the importance of the transportation and policy environment in determining mobility and driving cessation (Kulikov, Reference Kulikov2010; Vivoda, Heeringa, Schulz, Grengs, & Connell, Reference Vivoda, Heeringa, Schulz, Grengs and Connell2016). Yet, psychological resource variables, such as one’s life attitudes and sense of control, have been shown to be related to responses to and management of stressful life events (Taylor, Kemeny, Reed, Bower, & Gruenewald, Reference Taylor, Kemeny, Reed, Bower and Gruenewald2000), and may further account for unexplained variance in driver status.

Life attitudes are conceptualized as individual difference variables that foster subjective well-being by means of a person experiencing good psychological functioning (Kafka & Kozma, Reference Kafka and Kozma2002). Life attitudes include constructs such as death acceptance (i.e., lack of fear or anxiety about death), being in an existential vacuum (i.e., lack of goals), future meaning (i.e., a determination to make one’s life meaningful), goal seeking (i.e., a desire to achieve new goals), life control (i.e., freedom to make life choices), life purpose (i.e., fulfillment in life, having goals and a sense of direction from the past, in the present, and toward the future), and will to make meaning (i.e., striving to find meaning in personal existence) (Reker, Reference Reker2001; Reker & Peacock, Reference Reker and Peacock1981).

The locus of control construct is similar to the life control construct with some notable differences. Both constructs refer to people’s beliefs about control and expectancies in situations; however, the locus of control construct makes reference to a stable characteristic in examining people’s preference for adopting either an internal or an external control approach in life (Rodin, Reference Rodin1986). Moreover, locus of control adds reference to specific factors that affect situations such as luck, fate, or one’s own competence and behaviour (Windle & Woods, Reference Windle and Woods2004). On the other hand, the life control construct refers more to a state, as it asks people to report on their experience of control at a specific time in their lives (Reker, Peacock, & Wong, Reference Reker, Peacock and Wong1987).

Psychological resources have been shown to be associated with coping with changes that can occur with aging (Bjørkløf et al., Reference Bjørkløf, Engedal, Selbæk, Maia, Coutinho and Helvik2016, Reference Bjørkløf, Engedal, Selbæk, Maia, Borza and Benth2018). It follows that psychological resource variables may play a role in supporting individuals to adjust to losses associated with age, including loss of driving privileges. With respect to driving, a handful of studies have examined the role of psychological resource variables. For example, through analysis of the Australian Longitudinal Study on Aging, Windsor, Anstey, Butterworth, Luszcz, and Andrews (Reference Windsor, Anstey, Butterworth, Luszcz and Andrews2007) examined whether driving status, expectancy of control, and other covariates could explain changes in depressive scores from baseline to follow-up. Their results indicated that driving cessation was associated with an increase in symptoms of depression, a finding that was partially explained by decreases in the sense of control reported by former drivers. Similarly, Al-Hassani and Alotaibi (Reference Al-Hassani and Alotaibi2014) conducted a study of 114 community-dwelling older adults 55 years of age and older, and compared drivers and former drivers on a variety of variables including depression symptoms, health, and perceived control. Former drivers were more likely to have higher symptoms of depression and to report greater perceived loss of control over their lives.

Considering that psychological variables are associated with people’s reactions to life events, how they manage stress, and the level of well-being that they experience (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Kemeny, Reed, Bower and Gruenewald2000) they may also help to explain responses to driving cessation. The purpose of this study was to examine differences between drivers and former drivers in terms of demographic and health variables. In light of the previous literature, it was hypothesized that former drivers would be older, have poorer health, and exhibit more symptoms of depression. We also sought to examine the association between psychological resource variables and driving status among adults 55 years of age and older. In particular, we examined the relationship between life control, life purpose, and locus of control and driving status, after controlling for known covariates.

Methods

Participants

We recruited community-dwelling adults 55 years of age and older who were currently driving or who had stopped driving within the last 3 years. Participants were recruited through local community, volunteer, and business organizations in Thunder Bay, Ontario, and Winnipeg, Manitoba. Community partners received recruitment posters to post within their local setting; potential participants were invited to contact members of the research team who confirmed their eligibility. Eligible participants had questionnaire packages distributed to them, and participants returned completed questionnaire packages to the research team by mail or in person. Each questionnaire package included an informed consent document, detailing the goals of the study and how participant confidentiality would be maintained. Participants had the option to complete the questionnaire package using an online version; only 22 participants (9.4%) completed the online version. This study received ethical approval from Lakehead University’s Research Ethics Board (#1460532).

In total, 222 adults participated in this study. At the time of initial recruitment, participants ages ranged from 55 to 91 years (mean age = 72.20 years, standard deviation [SD] = 8.81). Approximately 55 per cent (54.7%) of participants self-identified as female and 45.3 per cent self-identified as male. Of the 222 participants, 193 (86.9%) were drivers and 29 (13.0%) were former drivers. Given the small number of former drivers relative to active drivers in the study, we were limited to controlling for two covariates in the multivariable models (see Statistical analysis section).

Measures

In addition to basic demographic information (e.g., sex, age, education, living arrangements) and driving status (i.e., current driver or former driver), participants completed measures capturing health, locus of control, and life purpose.

Physical and mental health measures

Participants completed a general health questionnaire to describe their health these days (response options were: very good, pretty good, not too good, poor, or very poor; henceforth referred to as health these days) and identify chronic health problems (e.g., stroke, arthritis, high blood pressure).

Participants completed the Short Form-12 Health Survey (SF-12) which consists of 12 items measuring health status and assesses both physical and mental health (Ware, Kosinski, & Keller, Reference Ware, Kosinski and Keller1998). There is a separate Physical Component Summary (PCS-12) and a Mental Component Summary (MCS-12), each with its own scale scores. The SF-12 is the abridged version of the 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) and retains the validity, reliability, and responsiveness of the longer version (Brazier, Jones, & Kind, Reference Brazier, Jones and Kind1993; McHorney, Kosinski, & Ware Jr., Reference McHorney, Kosinski and Ware1994). The PCS-12 and the MCS-12 are scored using norm-based methods. Four items (i.e., 1, 8, 9, and 10) are reverse keyed so that a lower score signifies better health. Indicator variables are developed for the item response choice categories. The indicator variables are weighted using regression coefficients from the United States general population and then are aggregated. A constant regression intercept is added to the aggregated PCS-12 and MCS-12 scores and the scores are then standardized to have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 in the general United States population.

The Activities of Daily Living Questionnaire was administered, consisting of 14 items that assess activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) in order to determine an individual’s functional status. ADL include bathing, toileting, continence, dressing, transferring, eating, and walking, whereas IADL include using the phone, self-administration of medications, shopping, housework, managing finances, and cooking. The total number of ADL is reported here, with higher scores indicating higher functional status.

Depression was captured through the Geriatric Depression Scale Short Form (GDS-SF), a scale consisting of 15 items assessing depression in older people (McDowell & Newell, Reference McDowell and Newell1996; Sheikh & Yesavage, Reference Sheikh and Yesavage1986). The GDS-SF is a shortened version of the GDS (Brink et al., Reference Brink, Yesavage, Lum, Heersema, Adey and Rose1982). Participants are asked to answer items with reference to how they have felt in the past week. Participants are asked to indicate either “Yes” or “No” to items. Higher scores on the GDS indicate a greater number of depressive symptoms.

Psychological resources

A portion of the Life Attitude Profile (LAP) scale was included. In particular, two of the seven dimensions, life purpose and life control, have been deemed suitable for exploring well-being among older adults and were included in the questionnaire package (Reker et al., Reference Reker, Peacock and Wong1987). The nine items in the life purpose dimension measure zest for life, satisfaction, and fulfillment. The six items of the life control dimension measure freedom to make life choices. Participants respond to each item with a “Yes”, “No”, or “Don’t Know”. With regard to psychometric properties, both dimensions have correlated significantly with other measures such as Shostrom’s Personal Orientation Inventory, thereby confirming that the scales have good construct validity (Reker & Peacock, Reference Reker and Peacock1981). Internal consistency is good for both life purpose and life control, with Cronbach’s α reaching 0.83 and 0.78 respectively (Reker & Peacock, Reference Reker and Peacock1981). Test–retest reliability has also been confirmed, with coefficients of 0.83 for life purpose and 0.61 for life control (Reker et al., Reference Reker, Peacock and Wong1987). Higher scores on both domains are indicative of greater psychological resources.

The Internal-External Locus of Control Scale (LOC) (Rotter, Reference Rotter1966) assesses a person’s generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement, and consists of 29 items (Rotter, Reference Rotter1966). An internal locus of control signifies a belief that outcomes are contingent on actions, with the opposite being true for an external locus of control (Lefcourt, Reference Lefcourt1991). The scale has adequate internal consistency, with a coefficient of 0.70 (Kuder–Richardson) and adequate test–retest reliability with a coefficient of 0.72 after 1 month (Rotter, Reference Rotter1966). Higher scores indicate greater external control.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted using SPSS (Version 25). Drivers and former drivers were first compared on relevant variables. Quantitative variables were analyzed using independent samples t tests, and categorical variables were analyzed using χ2 tests of independence. For ordinal categorical variables (income and health these days), the χ2 test for linear-by-linear association was calculated. Given the discrepancy in sample size between drivers (n = 193) and former drivers (n = 29), unequal variance t tests are reported here; in cases in which the variances are not equal across groups and the group sizes differ, Student’s t test can be biased leading to poor statistical conclusions (Delacre, Lakens, & Leys, Reference Delacre, Lakens and Leys2017). To examine the relationship between psychological resource variables and driver status, after controlling for known covariates, three multiple logistic regression models were estimated. Each model included age in years, health these days, and one psychological resource variable. In particular, Model 1 also included life control, Model 2 also include life purpose, and Model 3 also included locus of control. In this study, the number of events (i.e., participants who reporting being former drivers) was 29. To reduce the risk producing over-fitted modelsFootnote 1 , we chose to include only two known covariates of driving status: age (e.g., Foley et al., Reference Foley, Heimovitz, Gurainik and Brock2002) and health (Freeman, Gange, Munoz, & West, Reference Freeman, Gange, Munoz and West2006). Using this approach, the models had 9.7 events-per-variable (EPV), and although still considered liberal (Babyak, Reference Babyak2004), it allowed us to examine the effect of psychological resource factors while still accounting for factors already known to explain driving status while reducing the risk of over-fitting.

Results

Current versus Former Drivers

A description of the sample comparing drivers and former drivers are presented in Table 1. Former drivers were older (mean difference of 5.8 years), had lower levels of education, and were more likely to live alone. Former drivers had lower total ADL scores (t = –2.56, p = 0.015), a greater number of depression symptoms (t = 3.48, p = 0.002), more health conditions (t = 4.57, p < .001), and poorer health these days (χ2 = 10.63, p < 0.001). Former drivers showed poorer scores on the SF-12 Physical Health subscale (t = –4.45, p < 0.001) but no difference between groups was observed on the SF-12 Mental health subscale (t = –0.89, p = 0.379).

Table 1: Comparisons between drivers and former drivers at baseline

Note. SD = standard deviation; ADL = activities of daily living.

In terms of psychological resource variables, former drivers showed lower levels of life control (t = –3.70, p = 0.001) and life purpose (t = –2.28, p = 0.029), subscales of the life attitude profile. Former drivers also showed higher scores on the Locus of Control scale, suggesting that, relative to current drivers, former drivers have a more external locus of control.

Multivariable Models and Psychological Resources

Table 2 shows the results of a series of multiple logistic regressions treating driver status as the outcome variable. Models 1 and 2 included the Life Control and Life Purpose subscales of the LAP, respectively. Model 1 reached statistical significance as did the life control variable in the model. Life Control contributed significantly to the fit of the model explaining driver status, over and above age and health. Greater scores on life control were associated with lower odds of being a former driver. Model 2, also statistically significant, shows that life purpose enhanced the fit of the model explaining driver status, after controlling for age and health. Higher Life Purpose scores were associated with lower odds of being a former driver. Model 3 was statistically significant; the addition of locus of control enhanced the model fit and showed that a more internal locus of control was associated with lower odds of being a former driver. Across all three models, higher age was associated with higher odds of being a former driver. In Models 1 and 3, poorer health was associated with higher odds of being a former driver.

Table 2: Logistic regression models predicting driving status

Note. Some participants were excluded from the models because of missing data. Model 1 with life control included 29 non-drivers and 190 drivers; Model 2 with life purpose included 29 non-drivers and 190 drivers; and Model 3 with locus of control included 27 non-drivers and 189 drivers.

SE = standard error; OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval; BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; AIC = Akaike’s Information Criterion. When comparing models, smaller BIC and AIC values indicate better fit.

Discussion

We sought to examine differences between former drivers and current drivers on demographic variables. In addition, we examined the extent to which psychological resource variables of life control, life purpose, and locus of control were associated with driving status after controlling for age and health. The study participants were a convenience sample of Canadian adults 55 years of age and older.

The results indicate that former drivers were older and reported being in poorer health than current drivers. In addition, former drivers reported a greater number of depression symptoms than current drivers. Group differences in age may also reflect other effects such as differences in health status. With age, declines in functional ability and increases in the number of medical problems can occur, which interferes with a person’s ability to drive (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Bush and Hale1993). These findings are consistent with the literature, which indicates that older drivers may cease driving for health reasons (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Bush and Hale1993; Marottoli et al., Reference Marottoli, Ostfeld, Merrill, Perlman, Foley and Cooney1993) and that driving cessation may be followed by further declines in physical health (Edwards, Lunsman, Perkins, Rebok, & Roth, Reference Edwards, Lunsman, Perkins, Rebok and Roth2009). Similarly, research indicates that former drivers are more likely to be depressed than current drivers. (Chihuri et al., Reference Chihuri, Mielenz, DiMaggio, Betz, DiGuiseppi and Jones2016; Ragland, Satariano, & MacLeod, Reference Ragland, Satariano and MacLeod2005).

The results also showed differences on psychological variables as a function of driving status. Higher scores on life control and life purpose were associated with lower odds of being a former driver, after controlling for age and health. There was also a significant association between locus of control and driver status in the multivariable models such that a more internal locus of control was associated with higher odds of being a current driver. Psychological resources refer to the coping styles that people use to deal with the negative environmental effects that they experience (Lachman & Weaver, Reference Lachman and Weaver1998). Evidence suggests that psychological resources are positively associated with good mental and physical health (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Kemeny, Reed, Bower and Gruenewald2000). When it comes to driving cessation, it may be that psychological resource variables enable a longer driving career through their health promotion effects. For example, an internal health locus of control is a belief that one’s health status is influenced by one’s own behaviour, and is associated with positive outcomes such as increased physical activity, health help-seeking behaviours, and better physical health (Carlisle-Frank, Reference Carlisle-Frank1991; Helmer, Krämer, & Mikolajczyk, Reference Helmer, Krämer and Mikolajczyk2012). Better health is a factor that appears to extend the driving career of older people. Retchin and Anapolle (Reference Retchin and Anapolle1993) suggested that improvements in functional status and a general shift towards adopting healthier and more active lifestyles will extend the driving career of many older people. Alternatively, it is plausible that participants with an internal locus of control are less likely to relinquish driving, because it affords them greater control.

Although the results presented here demonstrate the potential importance of psychological resources in explaining driving status, this study is not without its limitations. First, we were limited in the number of explanatory variables within the multiple logistic regressions, given that only 29 former drivers participated in this study and there was a risk of over-fitting the models. Even the approach we used of including one psychological resource variable at a time across a series of three models, a practice Babyak (Reference Babyak2004) called multiple testing of confounders, is known to produce over-fitted models, and therefore, the results must be interpreted cautiously, recognizing this limitation. In addition, the cross-sectional nature of this study precludes discussion of a causal relationship between psychological resources and driving cessation. On the one hand, it is possible that psychological resources extend the driving lifespan. On the other hand, it is equally possible that higher psychological resources are a consequence of older people continuing to drive. For example, some authors note that that a driver’s licence gives people a sense of independence and control in their lives (Adler & Rottunda, Reference Adler and Rottunda2006; Eisenhandler, Reference Eisenhandler1990; Liddle & McKenna, Reference Liddle and McKenna2003; Whitehead, Howie, & Lovell, Reference Whitehead, Howie and Lovell2006). Whitehead et al. (Reference Whitehead, Howie and Lovell2006) demonstrated that driver’s licence cancellation was a deeply traumatic and shocking experience. Driving had been a normal and accepted part of life, and cancellation of their driver’s licences meant the loss of their independence and sense of control. Further examination of trajectories of driving cessation and health is needed to help elucidate the direction of relationships between these variables.

This study contributes to our understanding of the factors associated with driving cessation. In addition to age and health status, psychological resources contributed to the multivariable models explaining driver status. Given the potential importance of psychological resource variables in explaining driving status, research studies on the process of stopping driving should consider including measures such as life control, life purpose, and locus of control.

Footnotes

1 An over-fitted logistic regression model contains more explanatory variables than can be justified by the number of events. A common rule of thumb suggests that one should have at least 10–15 events per variable. Over-fitted models may lead to overly optimistic results that do not correspond to real relationships in the populations and therefore cannot be replicated.

References

Adler, G., & Rottunda, S. (2006). Older adults’ perspectives on driving cessation. Journal of Aging Studies, 20(3), 227235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2005.09.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al-Hassani, S. B., & Alotaibi, N. M. (2014). The impact of driving cessation on older Kuwaiti adults: Implications to occupational therapy. Occupational Therapy in Health Care, 28(3), 264276. https://doi.org/10.3109/07380577.2014.917779 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstey, K. J., Wood, J., Lord, S., & Walker, J. G. (2005). Cognitive, sensory and physical factors enabling driving safety in older adults. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(1), 4565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babyak, M. A. (2004). What you see may not be what you get: A brief, nontechnical introduction to overfitting in regression-type models. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(3), 411421.Google Scholar
Ball, K. K., Roenker, D. L., Wadley, V. G., Edwards, J. D., Roth, D. L., McGwin, G. Jr., et al. (2006). Can high-risk older drivers be identified through performance-based measures in a Department of Motor Vehicles setting? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 54(1), 7784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, M. J., Rottunda, S., & Adler, G. (2003). Older women and driving cessation. Qualitative Social Work, 2(3), 309325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjørkløf, G. H., Engedal, K., Selbæk, G., Maia, D. B., Borza, T., Benth, J. Š., et al. (2018). Can depression in psychogeriatric inpatients at one year follow-up be explained by locus of control and coping strategies? Aging & Mental Health, 22(3), 379388. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2016.1262817 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjørkløf, G. H., Engedal, K., Selbæk, G., Maia, D. B., Coutinho, E. S. F., & Helvik, A.-S. (2016). Locus of control and coping strategies in older persons with and without depression. Aging & Mental Health, 20(8), 831839. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1040722 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brayne, C., Dufouil, C., Ahmed, A., Dening, T. R., Chi, L.-Y., & McGee, M. (2000). Very old drivers: Findings from a population cohort of people aged 84 and over. International Journal of Epidemiology, 29, 704707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brazier, J., Jones, N., & Kind, P. (1993). Testing the validity of the Euroqol and comparing it with the SF-36 health survey questionnaire. Quality of Life Research: An International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care and Rehabilitation, 2(3), 169180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brink, T. L., Yesavage, J. A., Lum, O., Heersema, P. H., Adey, M., & Rose, T. L. (1982). Screening tests for geriatric depression. Clinical Gerontologist, 1, 3741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, M. K., Bush, T. L., & Hale, W. E. (1993). Medical conditions associated with driving cessation in community-dwelling, ambulatory elders. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 48, S230S234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canadian Association of Occupational Therapists. (2009). National blueprint for injury prevention in older drivers. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE.Google Scholar
Carlisle-Frank, P. (1991). Examining personal control beliefs as a mediating variable in the health-damaging behavior of substance use: An alternative approach. The Journal of Psychology, 125(4), 381397. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1991.10543300 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chihuri, S., Mielenz, T. J., DiMaggio, C. J., Betz, M. E., DiGuiseppi, C., Jones, V. C., et al. (2016). Driving cessation and health outcomes in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 64(2), 332341. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.13931 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delacre, M., Lakens, D., & Leys, C. (2017). Why psychologists should by default use Welch’s t-test instead of student’s t-test. International Review of Social Psychology, 30(1), 92101. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.82 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dellinger, A. M., Sehgal, M., Sleet, D. A., & Barrett-Connor, E. (2001). Driving cessation: What older former drivers tell us. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 49, 431435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dugan, E., & Lee, C. M. (2013). Biopsychosocial risk factors for driving cessation: Findings from the health and retirement study. Journal of Aging and Health, 25(8), 13131328. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264313503493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, J. D., Lunsman, M., Perkins, M., Rebok, G. W., & Roth, D. L. (2009). Driving cessation and health trajectories in older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64(12), 12901295. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glp114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenhandler, S. A. (1990). The asphalt identikit: Old age and the driver’s license. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 30(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foley, D. J., Heimovitz, H. K., Gurainik, J. M., & Brock, D. B. (2002). Driving life expectancy of persons aged 70 years and older in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 12841289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, E. E., Gange, S. J., Munoz, B., & West, S. K. (2006). Driving status and risk of entry into long-term care in older adults. American Journal of Public Health, 96(7), 12541259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilley, D. W., Wilson, R. S., Bennett, D. A., Stebbins, G. T., Bernard, B. A., Whalen, M. E., et al. (1991). Cessation of driving and unsafe motor vehicle operation by dementia patients. Archives of Internal Medicine, 151, 941946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmer, S. M., Krämer, A., & Mikolajczyk, R. T. (2012). Health-related locus of control and health behaviour among university students in North Rhine Westphalia, Germany. BMC Research Notes, 5, 703. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-5-703 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kafka, G. J., & Kozma, A. (2002). The construct validity of Ryff’s Scales of Psychological Well-Being (SPWB) and their relationship to measures of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 57(2), 171190. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014451725204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kington, R., Reuben, D., Rogowski, J., & Lillard, L. (1994). Sociodemographic and health factors in driving patterns after 50 years of age. American Journal of Public Health, 84(8), 13271329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kulikov, E. (2010). The social and policy predictors of driving mobility among older adults. Journal of Aging & Social Policy, 23(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959420.2011.531991 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1998). Sociodemographic variations in the sense of control by domain: Findings from the MacArthur studies of midlife. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 553562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). Locus of control. In Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 413499). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-590241-0.50013-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddle, J., & McKenna, K. (2003). Older drivers and driving cessation. British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 66(3), 125132. https://doi.org/10.1177/030802260306600307 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marottoli, R. A., Ostfeld, A. M., Merrill, S. S., Perlman, G. D., Foley, D. J., & Cooney, , L. M. Jr. (1993). Driving cessation and changes in mileage driven among elderly individuals. Journal of Gerontology, 48(5), S255S260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDowell, I., & Newell, C. (1996). Measuring health: A guide to rating scales and questionnaires, 2nd ed. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McHorney, C. A., Kosinski, M., & Ware, , , J. E. Jr. (1994). Comparisons of the costs and quality of norms for the SF-36 health survey collected by mail versus telephone interview: Results from a national survey. Medical Care, 32(6), 551567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pachana, N. A., Jetten, J., Gustafsson, L., & Liddle, J. (2017). To be or not to be (an older driver): Social identity theory and driving cessation in later life. Ageing & Society, 37(8), 15971608. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X16000507 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Persson, D. (1993). The elderly driver: Deciding when to stop. The Gerontologist, 33(1), 8891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ragland, D. R., Satariano, W. A., & MacLeod, K. E. (2005). Driving cessation and increased depressive symptoms. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 60(3), 399403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reker, G. T. (2001). Prospective predictors of successful aging in community-residing and institutionalized Canadian elderly. Ageing International, 27(1), 4264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-001-1015-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reker, G. T., & Peacock, E. J. (1981). The Life Attitude Profile (LAP): A multidimensional instrument for assessing attitudes toward life. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 13(3), 264273. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0081178 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reker, G. T., Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. (1987). Meaning and purpose in life and well-being: A life-span perspective. Journal of Gerontology, 42(1), 4449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Retchin, S. M., & Anapolle, J. (1993). An overview of the older driver. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 9, 279296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodin, J. (1986). Aging and health: Effects of the sense of control. Science, 233, 12711276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(1), 128. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0092976 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheikh, J., & Yesavage, J. A. (1986). Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist, 5, 169173.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions and health. The American Psychologist, 55(1), 99109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turcotte, M. (2012). Profile of seniors’ transportation habits (No. 11-008–X). Ottawa: Statistics Canada.Google Scholar
Vivoda, J. M., Heeringa, S. G., Schulz, A. J., Grengs, J., & Connell, C. M. (2016). The influence of the transportation environment on driving reduction and cessation. The Gerontologist, 57(5), 824832. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnw088 Google Scholar
Ware, J. E., Kosinski, M., & Keller, S. D. (1998). SF-12: How to score the SF-12 physical and mental health summary scales. 3rd ed. Boston: QualityMetric Incorporated.Google Scholar
Whitehead, B. J., Howie, L., & Lovell, R. K. (2006). Older people’s experience of driver licence cancellation: A phenomenological study. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 53(3), 173180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1630.2006.00564.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windle, G., & Woods, R. T. (2004). Variations in subjective wellbeing: The mediating role of a psychological resource. Ageing & Society, 24(4), 583602. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X04002107 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windsor, T. D., Anstey, K. J., Butterworth, P., Luszcz, M. A., & Andrews, G. R. (2007). The role of perceived control in explaining depressive symptoms associated with driving cessation in a longitudinal study. The Gerontologist, 47(0016–9013; 2), 215223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1: Comparisons between drivers and former drivers at baseline

Figure 1

Table 2: Logistic regression models predicting driving status