Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T20:09:56.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Empathy and Openness: Practices of Intersubjectivity at the Core of the Science of Consciousness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2020

Extract

The general framework of this paper relies on the observation that the practice of science as an experimental research program involves a social network of subjects working together (Latour 1988; Stengers 1994), both as co-researchers and as co-subjects of experiments. We want to take this basic observation seriously in order to explore how the objectivity of scientific results obtained thereby is highly affected and dependent on multifarious ‘intersubjective regulations.’ By intersubjective regulations we mean the different ways in which each subject/ researcher is able to account for his or her experience and share it with other subjects/researchers (comparing it, differentiating between each of them, seeing similarities and even identities, but also producing conflictive accounts) to the point of giving way to a re-styled objectivity founded on such ruled inter-individual practices (Husserl1954, 1973; Depraz 1995; Varela 1999):

More specifically, ‘third-person’ protocols are not neutral, that is, true independently of the very situatedness of each subject in its own individuated space and time (Bitbol 2002), but must take into consideration ‘first-person’ accounts and furthermore are inherently dependent on specific ‘second-person’ validations.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allison, T., Puce, Q., and McCarthy, G. (2000). “Social Perception from Visual Cues: Role of the STS region,Trends in Cognitive Science 4: 267–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bitbol, M. (2002). “Science as if Situation Mattered,Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 181224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braddock, G. (2001). “Beyond Reflection in Naturalized Phenomenology,Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(11): 316.Google Scholar
Breese, B. B. (1909). “Binocular Rivalry,” Psychological Review 16:410–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. J. (1995). “Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness,Journal of Consciousness Studies 2(3): 200–19.Google Scholar
Cosmelli, D., David, O., Lachaux, J.P., Garnero, L., Renault, B., and Varela, F. J. (2002). “Dynamic Neural Patterns Revealed by MEG/EEG during Visual Perception,“ Clinical Neurophysiology 113, Supplement 1: Abstracts of the 11th European Congress of Clinical Neurophysiology, Barcelona, Spain, August 24-28, 2002.Google Scholar
David, O., Garnero, L., Cosmelli, D., and F. J., Varela (2002). “Estimation of Neural Dynamics from MEG/EEG Cortical Current Density Maps: Application to the Reconstruction of Large-Scale Cortical Synchrony,IEEE Transactions Biomedical Engineering 49: 975–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2002). “A Third-person Approach to Consciousness.” Daewoo Lecture 1. Unpublished.Google Scholar
Depraz, N. (1995). Transcendance et incarnation. Le statut de l'intersubjectivite comme Altérité à soi chez Husserl. Paris: Vrin.Google Scholar
Depraz, N. (1999a). Ecrire en phénoménologue, Fougères, Encre Marine.Google Scholar
Depraz, N. (1999b). “The Phenomenological Reduction as Praxis.” In Varela, F. J. and Shear, J., eds., The View from Within, 95-110. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
N., Depraz (2001a). “The Husserlian Theory of Intersubjectivity as Alterology: Emergent Theories and Wisdom Traditions in the Light of Genetic Phenomenology.” In Thompson, E., ed., Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness, 169-78. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5-7): 169–78.Google Scholar
Depraz, N. (2001b). “La phénoménologie, une pratique concrete,” Magazine Littéraire, Novembre.Google Scholar
Depraz, N., and Varela, F. J. (2003). “Au cœur du temps I,” Intellektika, forthcoming.Google Scholar
Depraz, N., F. J., Varela, and Vermersch, P. (2003). On Becoming Aware: An Experiential Pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutour, E.-F. (1760). “Discussion d'une question d'optique,Academie des Sciences: Mémoires de Mathématique et de Physique Présentes par Divers Savants 3: 514–30.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (1999). “Interacting Minds-A Biological Basis,Science 286: 1692–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2001). “The Practice of Mind: Theory, Simulation or Primary Interaction.” In Thompson, E., ed., Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness, 83-108. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5-7): 83108.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., and Cole, J. (2002). “Gesture Following Deafferentation: A Phenomenologically Informed Experimental Study.” In: Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 4967.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., and A. I., Goldman (1998). “Mirror Neurons and the Simulation of Mind Reading,Trends in Cognitive Science 2: 493501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in Ethnomethodology. Prentice Hall, NJ: Englewood Cliffs.Google Scholar
Gendlin, E. (1997). Experiencing and the Creation of Meaning: A Philosophical and Psychological Approach to the Subjective. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (1989). “Interpretation Psychologized,Mind and Language 4: 161–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, K. (1995). The Organism. New York: Zone Books.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gordon, R. M. (1995). “Simulation without Introspection or Inference from Me to You.” In Davies, M. and Stone, T., eds., Mental Simulation: Evaluations and Applications. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gurwitsch, A. (1957). Théorie du champ de Ia conscience. Paris: Desclée de Brouwer.Google Scholar
Hanna, R. and E., Thompson (2004). “Neurophenomenology and the Spontaneity of Consciousness,” in Thompson, E. (ed.), The Problem of Consciousness: New Essays in Phenomenological Philosophy of Mind. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, Supplementary Volume.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1950a). Ideen zu einer reinen Phänomenologie I. Den Haag: M. Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1950b). Cartesianische Meditationen, Hua I. Den Haag: M. Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1954). Die Krisis der europäischen Wissenschaften und die transzendentale Phänomenologie, Hua VI. Den Haag: M. Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1973). Zur Intersubjektivität (1905-1920), Hua XIII. Den Haag: M. Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioannides, A. A., Kostopoulos, G. K., Lasarkis, N. A., Liu, L., Shibata, T., Schellens, M., Poghosyan, V., and Khurshudyan, A. (2002). “Timing and Connectivity in the Human Somatosensory Cortex from Single Trial Mass Electrical Activity,Human Brain Mapping 15: 231–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, H. (1966). The Phenomenon of Life: Toward a Philosophical Biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt Psychology. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1988). Science in Action: How to Follow Scientists and Engineers through Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lachaux, J.P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J., and Varela, F. J. (1999). “Measuring Phase Synchrony in Brain Signals,Human Brain Mapping 8(4): 194208.3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachaux, J.P., Lutz, A., Rudrauf, D., Cosmelli, D., Quyen, M. LeVan, Martinerie, J., and Varela, F. J. (2002). “Estimating the Time Course of Coherence between Single-Trial Brain Signals: An Introduction to Wavelet Coherence,Clinical Neurophysiology 32(3): 157–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A.M. (1991). “The Theory of Mind Impairment in Autism: Evidence for a Modular Mechanism of Development.” In A Whiten (ed.), Natural Theories of Mind: Evolution, Development and Simulation of Everyday Mindreading, 63-78. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Levine, J. (1983). “Materialism and Qualia: The Explanatory Gap.Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 64: 354–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutz, A. (2002). “Toward a Neurophenomenology as an Account of Generative Passages: A First Empirical Case Study,” Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1:133–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutz, A., J.-P., Lachaux, Martinerie, J., and F. J., Varela (2002). “Guiding the Study of Brain Dynamics Using First-Person Data: Synchrony Patterns Correlate with Ongoing Conscious States during a Simple Visual Task,Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99: 1586–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzhoff, A., and M. K., Moore (1977). “Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates,Science 198: 8399.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phénoménologie de Ia perception, Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1960). Signes. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). “What is it like to be a bat?Philosophical Review 83:43550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nam-in-Lee, (1998). “Edmund Husserl's Phenomenology of Mood.” In Depraz, N. and Zahavi, D., eds., Alterity and Facticity. New Perspectives on Husserl. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Necker, L.A. (1832). “Observations on some remarkable phaenomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phaenomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid,London and Edinburgh Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science 1: 329–37.Google Scholar
Nicolis, G., and Prigogine, I. (1989). Exploring Complexity: An Introduction. New York: W. H. Freeman and Co.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R., and Wilson, T. D. (1977). “Telling More than We Can Know: Verbal Reports on Mental Processes,” Psychological Review 84(3): 231–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petitmengin, C. (2001). L'expérience intuitive. Paris: L'harmattan.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, V., and Y.-M., Visetti (1999). “Sens et temps de Ia Gestalt,Intellectica 1: 147227.Google Scholar
Scheler, M. (1970). The Nature of Sympathy. Trans. P. Heath. Hamden, CO: Archon Books.Google Scholar
Schutz, A. (1967) The Phenomenology of the Social World. Trans. G. Walsh and F. Lehnert. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Spiegelberg, H. (1971). Doing Phenomenology. Essays on and in Phenomenology. The Hague: M. Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Spiegelberg, H. (1995). “Towards a Phenomenology of Imaginative Understanding of Others,” Proceedings of the 11th International Congress of Philosophy, Brussels, 1995, IV, 232–39.Google Scholar
Stengers, I. (1994). L'invention des sciences modernes. Paris: La Découverte.Google Scholar
Stumpf, C. (1883). Tonpsychologie, Leipzig: R. Hirzel, 1883.Google Scholar
Thompson, E., ed. (2001). Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., (1979). Principles of Biological Autonomy, New York: Elsevier North Holland.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J. (1991). “Organism: A Meshwork of Selfless Selves.” In Tauber, A., ed., Organism and the Origin of Self, 79-107. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., (1996). “Neurophenomenology: A Methodological Remedy for the Hard Problem,Journal of Consciousness Studies 3(4): 330–49.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., (1997). “The Naturalization of Phenomenology as the Transcendence of Nature,Alter 5: 355–81.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J. (1999). Ethical Know-how, Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., and Shear, J., eds. (1999). The View from Within. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., (2000). “The Specious Present: A Neuro-Phenomenology of TimeConsciousness.” In Petitot, J., Varela, F., Roy, J.-M., and Pachoud, B., eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., and Depraz, N. (2000). “At the Source of Time: Valence and the Constitutional Dynamics of Affect,” Arobase. Electronic Journal: http://www. arobase.toGoogle Scholar
Varela, F. J., and Depraz, N. (2003). “Imagining: Embodiment, Phenomenology and Transformation.” In Wallace, A., ed., Breaking the Ground: Essays on Tibetan Buddhism and the Natural Sciences. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Von Uexküll, J. (1956). Streifzüge durch die Umwelten von Tieren und Menschen - Bedeutungslehre, Hamburg: Rowohlt Verlag.Google Scholar
Schütz, A. (1962-66). Collected Papers. The Hague: M. Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Zahavi, D. (1996). Husserl und die transzendentale Intersubjektivitat. Eine Antwort auf die sprachpragmatische Kritik. Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahavi, D. (2001). “Beyond Empathy: Phenomenological Approaches to Intersubjectivity.” In Thompson, E., ed., Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic, 151-67. Published simultaneously as Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5-7): 151–67.Google Scholar