Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T06:38:43.488Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Improving Patient Compliance to Prophylactic Migraine Therapy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2014

Michel Aubé*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montréal, Québec
*
M. Aubé, 3801 University Street, Montréal, Québec, Canada H3A 2B4
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

As in many other chronic conditions, adherence to prophylactic treatment in migraine is probably poor. In chronic diseases, compliance at one year does not exceed 50%. That could explain the low therapeutic gain seen with migraine preventive medications. It also renders difficult the evaluation of clinical trials on migraine prophylaxis since in most of these trials compliance is not properly assessed. From the patients' perspective, there are several factors that could explain poor adherence to recommended treatments. Essentially, these factors are the expression of the patients'subjective perception of their disease and potential remedies in a context of a positive patient-physician relationship. When migraine prophylactic treatment is considered, patients should be informed of the natural history of their disease and a diagnosis of an accelerated form of migraine should be confirmed. Prophylactic treatment at best would reduce by 50% the frequency of migraine attacks. In most studies, however, the therapeutic gain is in the order of 30-40%. Treatment should be instituted for a minimum time of two to three months and if effective maintained for 6-12 months. The outcome of prophylaxis can rarely be determined in a prospective way. The choice of prophylactic regimens remains empirical, often based on the physician's experience and perception of the mechanism of migraine. A better adherence to prophylactic treatment of migraine could possibly improve outcomes but current methods of improving adherence for chronic health problems are mostly complex and not very effective.

Résumé:

RÉSUMÉ:

Comme il en est pour plusieurs maladies chroniques, la fidélité au traitement prophylactique de la migraine demeure probablement faible. On estime que, dans les conditions chroniques, la fidélité au traitement à un an ne dépasse pas 50%. Ceci expliquerait peut-être le faible gain thérapeutique observé avec les agents prophylactiques courants dans la migraine. Un faible taux de fidélité au traitement rendrait aussi difficile l'interprétation des résultats des essais cliniques des agents prophylactiques puisque dans la majorité de ces essais la fidélité au traitement n'est pas mesurée d'une façon adéquate. Du point de vue du patient, plusieurs facteurs peuvent expliquer un faible taux de fidélité. Ils reflètent essentiellement la perception que développe le patient de sa maladie et de son traitement dans un contexte de relation positive avec son médecin. Avant de considérer un traitement prophylactique, le patient doit être informé de l'histoire naturelle de sa condition et un diagnostique d'une forme accélérée de migraine doit être confirmé. Au mieux, le traitement prophylactique réduira de 50% la fréquence des attaques migraineuses. Dans la plupart des études cependant, le gain thérapeutique se limite à 30 à 40%. Pour juger de l'efficacité, le traitement doit être maintenu pour un minimum de 2 à 3 mois et, si efficace, continu pour 6 à 12 mois. Il n'existe que peu d'indices permettant de statuer d'une façon prospective sur l'efficacité d'un traitement. Le choix de l'agent prophylactique demeure empirique, basé sur l'expérience du clinicien et sur sa perception de la pathogenèse de la migraine. Une meilleure fidélité au traitement prophylactique pourrait à la rigueur en améliorer l'efficacité, mais les méthodes courantes visant à améliorer la fidélité des patients demeurent complexes et peu efficaces.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Canadian Journal of Neurological 2002

References

1. Haynes, RB. Introduction. In: Haynes, RB, Taylor, DW, Sackett, DL, (eds). Compliance in Health Care. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press 1979;17.Google Scholar
2. Eraker, SA, Kirscht, JP, Becker, MH. Understanding and improving patient compliance. Ann Intern Med 1984;100:258268.Google Scholar
3. Feinstein, AR. ‘Compliance bias’ and the interpretation of therapeutic trials. In: Haynes, RB, Taylor, DW, Sackett, DL, (eds). Compliance in Health Care Baltimore>: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1979:309322.:+Johns+Hopkins+University+Press;+1979:309–322.>Google Scholar
4. Nichol, MB, Venturini, F, Sung, JC. A critical evaluation of the methodology of literature on medication compliance. Ann Pharmacother 1999;33(5):531540.Google Scholar
5. Sackett, DL, Richardson, WS, Rosenberg, W, Haynes, RB, (eds). Evidence-based Medicine: How to Practice and Teach EBM. Churchill Livingstone 1998;175178.Google Scholar
6. Jones, I, Britten, N. Why do some patients not cash their prescription? Br J Gen Pract 1998;48(426):903905.Google ScholarPubMed
7. Martys, CR. Adverse reactions to drugs in general practice. Br Med J 1979;2:11941197.Google Scholar
8. Spierings, EL, Miree, LF. Noncompliance with follow-up and improvement after treatment at a headache center. Headache 1993;33(4):205209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9. Packard, RC. What does the headache patient want? Headache 1979;19:370374.Google Scholar
10. Bille, B. A 40-year follow-up of school children with migraine. Cephalalgia 1997;17:488491.Google Scholar
11. Rasmussen, BK, Olesen, J. Migraine with aura and migraine without aura: an epidemiological study. Cephalalgia 1992;12:221228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Zagami, AS, Rasmussen, BK. Symptomatology of migraine without aura. In: Olesen, J, Tfelt-Hansen, P, Welch, KMA. (Eds). The Headaches. 2nd ed. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins 2000:337343.Google Scholar
13. Blau, JN, Thavapalan, M. Preventing migraine: a study of precipitating factors. Headache 1988;28:481483.Google Scholar
14. Pryse-Phillips, WEM, Dodick, DW, Edmeads, JG, et al. Guidelines for the diagnosis and management of migraine in clinical practice. Can Med Assoc J 1997;156:12731287.Google Scholar
15. Silberstein, SD. Preventive treatment of migraine: an overview. Cephalalgia 1997;17:6772.Google Scholar
16. Wober, C, Wober, B, Koch, G, Wessely, P. Long-term results of migraine prophylaxis with flunarizine and beta-blockers. Cephalalgia 1991;11:251256.Google Scholar
17. Lucetti, C, Nuti, A, Pavese, N, et al. Flunarizine in migraine prophylaxis: predictive factors of a positive response. Cephalalgia 1998;18:349352.Google Scholar
18. Schoenen, J, Martens de Noordhout, A, Timsit-Berthier, M, Timsit, M. Contigent negative variation and efficacy of beta-blocking agents in migraine. Cephalalgia 1986;6:229233.Google Scholar
19. Ayajiki, K, Okamura, T, Toda, N. Flunarizine, an antimigraine agent, impairs nitroxidergic nerve function in cerebral arteries. Eur J Pharmacol 1997;329:4953.Google Scholar
20. Tfelt-Hansen, P, Welch, KMA. Prioritizing prophylactic treatment of migraine. In: Olesen, J, Tfelt-Hansen, P, Welch, KMA. (Eds). The Headaches. 2nd ed. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins 2000:499500.Google Scholar
21. Davidoff, RA. Migraine: Manifestations, Pathogenesis, and Management. Philadelphia: FA Davis Co. 1995.Google Scholar
22. Silberstein, SD. Preventive treatment of migraine: an overview. Cephalalgia 1997;17:6772.Google Scholar
23. Haynes, RB, Montague, P, Oliver, T, et al. Interventions for helping patients to follow prescriptions for medications. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2000;(2):CD000011.Google Scholar
24. Ferrari, MD. Migraine. Lancet 1998;351:10431051.Google Scholar