Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T16:41:33.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shana Poplack. 2018. Borrowing. Loanwords in the Speech Community and in the Grammar. New York: Oxford University Press. Pp. 272. $US $99 (hardcover).

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2021

Robert A. Papen*
Affiliation:
Université du Québec à Montréal
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Review/Compte rendu
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association/Association canadienne de linguistique 2021

Shana Poplack is, arguably, Canada's best known sociolinguist, enjoying a well-merited world-wide reputation due to the remarkable number of publications she and her numerous co-authors have produced on language mixing over the past 40 years. As Pieter Muysken points out in the foreword of the book, “I had been thinking that Shana Poplack, if she had the energy, should assemble her various writings on lexical borrowing into a single book. And, like rubbing Aladdin's lamp, there it was!”

The book is organized as follows. Following Muysken's foreword is a preface in which Poplack acknowledges the collaboration of her many colleagues and students as well as the numerous funding agencies which have allowed her to pursue her research. In Chapter 1, Poplack discusses the rationale of the work. The aim is not to provide new data but instead to synthesize, build on and reinterpret more than 40 years of research on language mixing strategies in the discourse of bilingual speakers, to characterize the phenomena of lexical borrowing in the speech community as well as in the grammar, both synchronically and diachronically. Poplack adds that the focus is not on the product of borrowing but on the process. For her, the process of borrowing is essentially one of integration into the structure of the recipient language. This integration must take into account the inherent variability in both donor languages (LD) and recipient languages (LR).

“Chapter 2 reviews the variationist perspective on language and outlines its specific applications to the study of language mixing” (p. 8). It discusses such issues as the primacy of the speech community, the optimal subjects for a variationist study of language mixing, namely the adult bilingual whose linguistic repertoire is stable, the type of data that best reflects the systematic form of language – the vernacular, the Principle of Accountability, the Comparative Sociolinguistic Method, and the Principle of Diagnosticity.

Chapter 3 briefly revisits the Canadian Ottawa-Hull region mega-corpus of French (2.5 million words, 120 carefully selected informants from five different communities, providing nearly 20,000 tokens of some 2,000 different English-origin types), the results of which are discussed in Chapter 4. The chapter also briefly describes two diachronic corpora, speech drawn from the Récits du français québécois d'autrefois, which are audio recordings of spontaneous language material produced by 44 elderly Québécois born between 1846 and 1895, and the Corpus du français en contexte : milieux scolaire et social, which consists of the oral recordings of 166 bilingual francophone teenagers from the Quebec community of Mont Bleu (Ottawa-Hull region), collected between 2005 and 2007.

As mentionned, Chapter 4 reviews the results obtained in the Ottawa-Hull region of Canada, originally described in Poplack et al. (Reference Poplack, Sankoff and Miller1988). This research, whose results are quite well-known, has undoubtedly set the standards for all ensuing large-scale investigations of language-mixing and, more specifically, borrowing – and the standards are very high indeed!

Chapter 5 discusses the problem of variability in borrowing and how to deal with it. Based on the hypotheses developed in Poplack and Meecham (Reference Poplack, Meecham, Poplack and Meecham1998), which state that if the constraints on variability of LD-origin forms are parallel to those constraining their LR counterparts, the LD-origin forms can only be borrowings and not code-switches.

Chapter 6 investigates the use of syntactic criteria to disambiguate ambiguous elements – specifically bare forms – in the analysis of intra-clausal mixing of French with Wolof and Fongbe, basically revisiting the results discussed earlier in Poplack and Meecham (Reference Poplack, Meecham, Milroy and Muysken1995) as well as English-Igbo data from Eze (Reference Eze1997, Reference Eze, Poplack and Meecham1998). Again, the results show that the very great majority of borrowed bare forms “display quantitative parallels to their relevant LR counterparts, specifically at conflict sites, parallels that are far too detailed to be due to chance” (p. 96).

Chapter 7 reviews a series of studies based on eight typologically distinct language pairs. Results again confirm that the overwhelming majority of language-mixing types are lone LD-origin items and that they systematically behave like LR items, that is, they are grammatically integrated to LR. They also show that true code-switching is exceedingly rare and represents only a minor strategy in most bilingual interactions.

Chapter 8 represents an innovation in that, for the first time, it traces “the diachronic trajectory of nonce forms in bilingual production over a real-time period of 61 years and nearly a century and a half in apparent time” (p. 11). Poplack compares data taken from three diachronically related spoken-language corpora of Quebec French: Récits du français québécois d'autrefois (Poplack and St. Amand Reference Poplack and St. Amand2007); a sub-sample of the Ottawa-Hull French corpus (48 speakers born between 1893 and 1965) and Le français en contexte : milieux scolaire et social (2005–2007) (Poplack and Bourdages Reference Poplack and Bourdages2005). Results show that the overwhelming majority of LD-origin material consist of lone items and that almost all of these have been attested in the LR lexicon. A very small proportion are unattested nonce LD items, which disappear after their first mention. The few nonce loans that do advance chronologically are integrated into LR morphology and syntax, this integration occurring abruptly, at the first mention of the item.

In Chapter 9, Poplack confronts the borrowing process with multiword code-switches produced by the same French-English bilinguals.

Chapter 10 addresses “the question of whether speakers marshal phonetic integration as a strategy to distinguish language-mixing types” (p. 12). Poplack convincingly shows that “individuals do not systematically integrate their LD words phonetically into LR”, this being the case whether the items are nonce borrowings or well-attested loanwords. “This confirms that phonetic and morphosyntactic integration are independent. Only the latter is a reliable metric for distinguishing language-mixing types” (p. 12).

Again, based on the Ottawa-Hull corpus, Chapter 11 assesses “the effects of age, gender, social class membership, level of education, individual bilingual proficiency, minority versus majority status, and neighborhood of residence on the adoption and distribution of borrowed material” (p. 13). Although various social and individual factors variably play a role, Poplack finds that community norms are the most important and that they crucially outweigh individual abilities. Chapter 12 concludes with some reflections on the implications of this work for the study of language mixing (p. 13).

This book is a very impressive piece of work. It neatly summarizes the results of many years of research on language-mixing strategies accomplished by Poplack and her colleagues, and convincingly argues in favour of the theoretical distinction between borrowing and code-switching, demonstrating the primacy of the former and relegating the latter to a minor language-mixing strategy. Throughout the book, the methodology employed to gather and analyze the various data is always well presented; the following argumentation is always clear, straightforward and easy to follow. Specialized technical linguistic terms are consistently defined and, in fact, kept to a minimum. Numerous examples, taken from the various corpora, are generously provided, allowing the reader to better understand the stakes at hand. Quantitative results are clearly presented in a variety of text, statistical or numerical tables, and line or bar graphs. References to relevant previously published material is amply provided throughout the text. Each chapter ends with a discussion reviewing the essential points made in the preceding pages.

As the French say: “Après les fleurs, le pot” (After the flowers, the pot). In her analysis of the Ottawa-Hull corpus, Poplack looks at the data according to part of speech and finds that, in accord with previous studies, the overwhelming majority of borrowed vocabulary falls in the category of nouns (64%), followed by verbs (14%), interjections and frozen expressions (12%), adjectives (8%) and conjunctions (1.5%) (p. 48). Unfortunately, Poplack fails to define or to give examples of what she considers to be a “frozen expression”. Flikeid (Reference Flikeid1989: 217), in her study of Nova Scotian Acadian French, points out that a great number of “frozen phrases” such as come to think of it, enough is enough, I suppose so, never mind, etc., occur regularly and repeatedly in the corpus. These can occur freely at various points of the sentence; others must occupy a specific slot, as in (1):

Some English sequences can even be part of an otherwise French expression, as in (2):

It is not totally evident whether Poplack would consider the above as “frozen expressions” or as bona fide code-switches, since they maintain LD structure. Since these sequences are often recurrent across speakers, Flikeid proposes that they be viewed as a special type of borrowing. A clarification on the part of Poplack on this issue would be most welcome.

On another note, one cannot help wonder why, in her study, Poplack fails to mention a particularly problematic but relatively well-known set of data of Louisiana French (LF). This data speaks to the borrowing of English participial and infinitival verb forms into LF, where they maintain neither their original LD structure nor integrate to LR structure. The following examples are taken from a study of LF by Dubois and Sankoff (Reference Dubois, Sankoff, Auger and Rose1997):

Particle verbs can also occur bare, as in the following example, taken from Rottet (Reference Rottetn.d.):

Dubois and Sankoff (Reference Dubois, Sankoff, Auger and Rose1997) state that 75% of verbs produced in their corpus are “bare forms”, as in the above examples. Rottet (Reference Rottet2018) confirms that many of these forms have been attested in dictionaries as early as 1936. In light of this, one cannot consider them as nonce loans, nor can they rightfully be considered as code-switches according to Poplack's own definition, since they do not maintain LD morphological structure. It is, of course, theoretically possible that LF-origin verbs variably delete infinitival or participial suffixes. If that were the case, then “bare forms” such as in the above examples must be considered borrowings, since they match LR variable structures. Indeed, Picone (Reference Picone, Beals, Denton, Knippen, Melnar, Suzuki and Zeinfeld1994: 273–74) admits that, in both Louisiana French (LF) and in Louisiana English (LE), there is some evidence of reduction or deletion of inflections for past participles, as in:

Unfortunately, I know of no quantitative study on either LF or LE verb form variation which could bolster arguments for bare form integration either into LF or into LE structure. Indeed, if both LR and LD verb structures were to prove to be variable, bare forms such as above might therefore correspond to either LR variation or to LD verb structure variation. The dilemma is thus far from being solved. Nevertheless, the fact that some of these bare verb forms are clearly historically attested show that they have the property of ‘listedness’ and should therefore, pace Poplack, be considered as borrowings, even if they do not respect LR morphological structure.

In spite of the above shortcomings, I consider this book to be well-researched, well-analyzed and well-discussed. I believe it to be a landmark in the history of research on linguistic borrowing, and it should be part of the personal library of anyone interested in (variationist) sociolinguistics in general and, more specifically, in language-mixing strategies.

References

Dubois, Sylvie, and Sankoff, David. 1997. L'absence de flexion sur les emprunts à l'anglais dans le français cadjin. In Explorations du lexique, ed. Auger, Julie and Rose, Yvan, 163176. Quebec : Centre interdisciplinaire de recherches sur les activités langagières (CIRAL).Google Scholar
Eze, Ejike. 1997. Aspects of language contact: A variationist perspective on code switching and borrowing in Igbo-English bilingual discourse. Doctoral dissertation, University of Ottawa.Google Scholar
Eze, Ejike. 1998. Lending credence to a borrowing analysis: Lone English-origin incorporations in Igbo discourse. In International Journal of Bilingualism 2(2): Instant loans, easy conditions: The productivity of bilingual borrowing, ed. Poplack, Shana and Meecham, Marjory, 183202.Google Scholar
Flikeid, Karin. 1989. “Moitié anglais, moitié français”? Emprunts et alternance de langues dans les communautés acadiennes de la Nouvelle-Écosse. Revue québécoise de linguistique théorique et appliquée 8(2) : 177227.Google Scholar
Picone, Michael. 1994. Code-intermediate phenomena in Louisiana French. In Cls 30-1: Papers from the 30th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Vol. 1: The main session, ed. Beals, Katherine, Denton, Jeannette, Knippen, Robert, Melnar, Lynette, Suzuki, Hisam and Zeinfeld, Erica, 320334. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Poplack, Shana, and Bourdages, Johanne. 2005. Corpus du français en contexte : milieux scolaire et social. University of Ottawa. SSHRC Research Grant #410-2005-2108.Google Scholar
Poplack, Shana, and Meecham, Marjory. 1995. Patterns of language mixture: Nominal structure in Wolof-French and Fongbe-French bilingual discourse. In One speaker, two languages: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on code-switching, ed. Milroy, Lesley and Muysken, Pieter, 199232. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poplack, Shana, and Meecham, Marjory. 1998. How languages fit together in code mixing. In International Journal of Bilingualism 2(2): Instant loans, easy conditions: The productivity of bilingual borrowing, ed. Poplack, Shana and Meecham, Marjory, 127138.Google Scholar
Poplack, Shana, Sankoff, David, and Miller, Christopher. 1988. The social correlates and linguistic processes of lexical borrowing and assimilation. Linguistics 26: 47104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poplack, Shana, and St. Amand, Anne. 2007. A real-time window on 19th century vernacular French: The Récits du français d'autrefois. Language in Society 36(5): 707734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rottet, Kevin. 2018. L'emprunt et l'alternance codique en français louisianais : perspectives lexicographiques. Paper presented at the International Colloquium Les français d'ici, Montreal.Google Scholar
Rottet, Kevin. n.d. L'anglicisme lexical en français louisianais. Indiana University.Google Scholar
Figure 0

(1)

Figure 1

(2)

Figure 2

(3)

Figure 3

(6)

Figure 4

(7)