No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Some remarks on deixis1
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 June 2016
Extract
This paper is concerned with so-called demonstratives. Languages use various devices to point out or indicate things; the demonstratives are but one of these devices. Much that has been written on this problem of deixis is confused and misleading because the criteria traditionally used in describing the different devices for pointing out or indicating have been for the most part notional or mentalistic; it behoves the theoretical linguist to seek for formal linguistic criteria to describe this feature, and these notes seek to outline some of the problems which are concerned with a satisfactory linguistic explanation of the grammatical category known as the “demonstratives.” In view of the title of this article, it might also be pointed out en passant (though the author has no wish to involve himself here in a terminological wrangle) that “deixis” may well be a more suitable term for this feature in view of the fact that languages possess other demonstrative devices or indicators in addition to those referred to traditionally as “demonstratives,” not to mention the quasi-linguistic feature of gesture.
- Type
- Research Article
- Information
- Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique , Volume 8 , Issue 2 , Spring 1963 , pp. 82 - 96
- Copyright
- Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association 1963
Footnotes
The author would like to express his thanks to his colleague, Professor J. O. St. Clair-Sobell of the University of British Columbia, who originally suggested this topic during his seminar on Comparative Slavonic Philology and for the many fruitful suggestions in the course of frequent informal discussions on the subject. He would also like to thank Professor C. E. Bazell of the School of Oriental and African Studies, London, who read an earlier draft of the article and made many valuable suggestions.
References
2 Collinson, W. E., “Indication: A Study of Demonstratives, Articles and other ‘Indicators’,” Language Monograph, no. 17 (1937), pp. 17–18.Google Scholar
3 Forchheimer, P., The Category of Person in Language (Berlin, 1953), pp. 7–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 Frei, H., Acta Linguistica IV (1944), pp. 111–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5 Firth, J. R., “A Synopsis of Linguistic Theory,” Studies in Linguistic Analysis (Oxford, 1957), p. 19.Google Scholar
6 Gray, L. H., Foundations of Language (New York, 1939), p. 173.Google Scholar
7 Brøndal, V., Essais de Linguistique Générale (Copenhagen, 1943), pp. 98–9.Google Scholar
8 V. Brøndal, op. cit., p. 103.
9 Brugmann, K., “Die Demonstrativpronomina der indo-germanischen Sprachen,” Sächs. Abh. XXII (1904), no. 6.Google Scholar
10 Jespersen, O., The Philosophy of Grammar (London, 1924), p. 212.Google Scholar
11 Grasserie, R. de la, “De la véritable nature du pronom,” Etudes de grammaire comparée (Louvain, 1888), p. 3.Google Scholar
12 Wundt, W., Völkerpsychologie, vol. II (Leipzig, 1911), p. 141.Google Scholar
13 V. Brøndal, op. cit., p. 101.
14 Bühler, K., Sprachtheorie (Jena, 1934), p. 79.Google Scholar
15 Ginneken, J. van, Principes de linguistique psychologique (Paris, 1907), pp. 209–10.Google Scholar
16 In Armenian, the demonstrative pronouns indicate not only temporal nearness or farness but also a relationship between one individual and another. Mention of this is made to support Firth’s thesis (cf. n. 5), i.e., that this is a fact of the Armenian deictic system which, though tri-personal, is however discrete from all other tri-personal systems.
17 Hirt, H., Indogermanische Granmmatik, vol. III (Heidelberg, 1927), p. 26.Google Scholar
18 When the third member is lost, it can be argued that the demonstratives lose the sense of relation to person but represent the particular versus the general, and are thus bi-dimensional in space or time.
19 Lampach, S., “La relation des genres dans le système des pronoms de la 3e personne en français,” Word 12 (1956), pp. 51–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20 Benveniste, E., “Structure des relations de personne dans le verbe,” BSLP 43 (1946), pp. 1–12.Google Scholar
21 Boas, F., Kwakiutl Grammar (Philadelphia, 1947).Google Scholar
22 Sapir, E., Language (New York, 1921).Google ScholarPubMed
23 Bloomfield, L., Language (New York, 1933), p. 259.Google ScholarPubMed
24 Ibid., p. 147.
25 Trager, G. L., “The Field of Linguistics,” Studies in Linguistics: Occasional Papers, no. 1 (Norman, 1949).Google Scholar
26 Whorf, B. L., Four articles on metalinguistics (Washington, D.C., 1949).Google Scholar
27 J. R. Firth, op. cit., p. 17.
28 Simon, H. F., “Two Substantival Complexes in Standard Chinese,” BSOAS XV (1953), pp. 327–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29 Bursill-Hall, G. L., “The Linguistic Theories of J. R. Firth,” Thought (Toronto, 1960), p. 243.Google Scholar
30 J. R. Firth, op. cit., p. 19.
31 E. Sapir, op. cit., pp. 118-9.
32 Bazell, C. E., Linguistic Form (Istanbul, 1953), p. 78.Google Scholar