Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T12:56:03.243Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive load theory as a framework for simulation-based, ultrasound-guided internal jugular catheterization training: Once is not enough, but we must measure it first

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 May 2019

Rene de la Fuente
Affiliation:
División de Anestesiología, Escuela de Medicina, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Marcoleta 367, 8330024 Santiago, Chile
Fernando R Altermatt
Affiliation:
División de Anestesiología, Escuela de Medicina, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Marcoleta 367, 8330024 Santiago, Chile

Abstract

Type
Letter
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Association of Emergency Physicians 2019 

February 2019

We have read with great interest the article entitled: “Cognitive load theory as a framework for simulation-based, ultrasound-guided internal jugular catheterization training: Once is not enough, but we must measure it first,” by McGraw et al.Reference McGraw, Chaplin, Rocca, Rang, Jaeger, Holden, Keri and Fichtinger1

The study reports the instructional design of simulation-based central venous access training, consisting of three sessions of progressive part practice. The entire procedure sequence is decomposed into part tasks that were incorporated into practice in a progressive fashion.

The authors made an important point of emphasizing the necessity for including principles of cognitive load theory in the planning and execution of procedural training sessions. The main aim is distributing the cognitive load and therefore to avoid overwhelming the working memory of the participants during the training process.

However, cognitive load theory principles go beyond the simple fact of segmenting the procedure into several steps with certain logical sequence. It is also important to define the reason that these specific steps (and no others) could and should be divided. The most important point on that decision must be to determine the cognitive load that each one of these steps has, and adjust it when the load surpasses the working memory of the practitioners. Unfortunately, the study does not provide any details on this issue.

There are at least three methods to measure cognitive loadReference Haji, Rojas, Childs, de Ribaupierre and Dubrowski2: subjective ratings, psychophysiological methods, and a secondary-task performance analysis. Each one of them has its own pros and cons. Subjective ratings, such as the one developed by Paas et al., continue to be the most used, due to their simplicity and reliability.

Does the distribution of the cognitive load into part tasks have an impact upon the learning process and ultimately on the performance of the students? Based on the results of the study, it seems that after just one 2-hour session, no difference is observed. It is only after three sessions that a significant improvement in performance is reached.

Is this the result of the distribution of the cognitive load during the sessions or the effect of distributed deliberate practice?Reference Mackay, Morgan, Datta, Chang and Darzi3 The latter has abundant evidence of effectiveness as a learning strategy in improving performance.Reference Spruit, Band, Hamming and Ridderinkhof4,Reference Mitchell, Lee, Sevdalis, Partsafas, Landry, Liem and Moneta5 Unless any kind of method to quantify the cognitive load is used, there is no way to answer this question, based on the results of the present study.

There is no doubt that cognitive load theory has a role in the simulation arena. Strategies aiming to quantify its impact upon a simulated learning environment are required; otherwise, it is impossible to modulate its effect on instructional designs. Further studies are warranted to address this important issue.

References

REFERENCES

1.McGraw, R, Chaplin, T, Rocca, N, Rang, L, Jaeger, M, Holden, M, Keri, Z, Fichtinger, G. Cognitive load theory as a framework for simulation-based, ultrasound-guided internal jugular catheterization training: Once is not enough, but we must measure it first. CJEM 2019;21(1):141–8.Google Scholar
2.Haji, FA, Rojas, D, Childs, R, de Ribaupierre, S, Dubrowski, A. Measuring cognitive load: performance, mental effort and simulation task complexity. Med Educ 2015;49(8):815–27.Google Scholar
3.Mackay, S, Morgan, P, Datta, V, Chang, A, Darzi, A. Practice distribution in procedural skills training: a randomized controlled trial. Surg Endosc 2002;16(6):957–61.10.1007/s00464-001-9132-4Google Scholar
4.Spruit, EN, Band, GP, Hamming, JF, Ridderinkhof, KR. Optimal training design for procedural motor skills: a review and application to laparoscopic surgery. Psychol Res 2014;78(6):878–91.Google Scholar
5.Mitchell, EL, Lee, DY, Sevdalis, N, Partsafas, AW, Landry, GJ, Liem, TK, Moneta, GL. Evaluation of distributed practice schedules on retention of a newly acquired surgical skill: a randomized trial. Am J Surg 2011;201(1):31–9.10.1016/j.amjsurg.2010.07.040Google Scholar