Article contents
The New Ethics of Neuroethics
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 September 2018
Abstract:
According to a familiar distinction, neuroethics incorporates the neuroscience of ethics and the ethics of neuroscience. Within neuroethics, these two parts have provoked distinct and separate lines of inquiry, and there has been little discussion of how the two parts overlap. In the present article, I try to draw a connection between these two parts by considering the implications that are raised for ethics by scientific findings about the way we make moral decisions. The main argument of the article is that although neuroscience is “stretching” ethics by revealing the empirical basis of our moral decisions and, thereby, challenging our present understanding of the dominant ethical theories, substantial further questions remain regarding the impact that neuroscience will have on ethics more broadly.
- Type
- Symposium: Competing Identities of Neuroethics
- Information
- Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics , Volume 27 , Special Issue 4: Clinical Neuroethics , October 2018 , pp. 558 - 565
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018
References
Notes
1. Australian Brain Alliance, BRAIN Initiative, Human Brain Project, Canada Brain Research Fund, China Brain Project, Cuban Human Brain Mapping Project (CHBMP), Israel Brain Technologies, Latin American Brain Mapping Network (LABMAN), Brain Mapping by Integrated Neurotechnologies for Disease Studies (Brain/MINDS), Korean Brain Initiative, and Blue Brain Project.Google Scholar
2. Rolston, H III. Environmental ethics: Values in and duties to the natural world. In: Bormann, FH, Kellert, ST, eds. The Broken Circle: Ecology, Economics and Ethics. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1991:228–47.Google Scholar
3. Merchant, C. Ecofeminism and feminist theory. In: Diamond, I, Orenstein, G, eds. Reweaving the World: The Emergence of Ecofeminism. San Francisco: Sierra Club Books; 1990:77–83.Google Scholar
4. Warren, KJ. The power and promise of ecological feminism. Environmental Ethics 1990:125–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5. Flanagan, O. Varieties of Moral Personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1993, at 32.Google Scholar
6. Green, JD. Beyond point-and-shoot morality: Why cognitive (neuro)science matter for ethics. In: Liao, SM, ed. Moral Brains: The Neuroscience of Morality. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016, at 119.Google Scholar
7. Greene, JD. The secret joke of Kant’s soul. In: Sinnott-Armstrong, W, ed. Moral Psychology, Volume III. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2008:35–81.Google Scholar
8. See note 6, Greene 2016.
9. Greene, JD. From neural ‘is’ to moral ‘ought’: What are the implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2003;4:847–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Haidt, J. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 2001;8(4):814–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11. See note 7, Greene 2008, at 41–2.
12. See note 9, Greene 2003, at 849.
13. Berker, S. The normative insignificance of neuroscience. Philosophy and Public Affairs 2009;37(4):293–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14. Smart, JJC, Williams, B. Utilitarianism For and Against. Cambridge: Cambridge University Pres; 1973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15. Douglas, T. Moral enhancement. Journal of Applied Philosophy 2008;25(3):228–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. See note 15, Douglas 2008, at 229.
17. Savulescu, J, Persson, I. Moral enhancement, freedom, and the God machine. The Monist 2012:95(3):399–421, at 406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Douglas, T. Moral enhancement via direct emotion modulation: A reply to John Harris. Bioethics 2013;160–8, at 161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. See note 17, Savulescu, Persson 2012, at 406.
20. See note 18, Douglas 2013, at 161.
- 7
- Cited by