Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T02:05:57.814Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Origins of Speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 December 2008

Extract

Spoken language is one of the defining human characteristics — the crucial accomplishment which makes us human and separates us from other species. Naturally enough, the origins of this accomplishment — which must lie somewhere back in the Palaeolithic — have been the subject of lively and often heated debate, not least since speech leaves no direct material residues. Many people have sought to resolve the question by careful analysis of the material remains of early hominids. But do patterns of tool-making or evidence of sophisticated subsistence strategies really provide an adequate base from which to deduce the presence of linguistic ability? Furthermore, is language inextricably bound up with the ability to vocalize and to speak? Are studies of the vocal tract of Neanderthals or Homo erectus really relevant to the question of language origins?

The wide diversity of view on the antiquity of human spoken language is very clear from the brief contributions which make up this feature. On the one hand is the evidence for the presence of Broca's and Wernicke's areas in the brain of Homo habilis around 2 million years ago. Does this provide grounds for believing that Homo habilis could speak? Did the use of tools as icons by Homo erectus play a key role in the development of human spoken language? Or should we instead go along with the growing consensus — supported by many linguists —that spoken language is a late addition to the range of human abilities, originating along with fully modern humans only within the last 200,000 years? And dare we go even further, and nominate Africa as the locus of language origin?

The time may come when we are able to specify not only when human spoken language first developed, but also where. For the present, however, the debate shows no sign of imminent resolution. In the pages which follow, we bring together the views of archaeologists from a number of different backgrounds; but we begin with a linguist's perspective, and seven propositions to set the scene for the archaeological enquiry.

Type
Viewpoint
Copyright
Copyright © The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aiello, L.C. & Wheeler, P., 1995. The expensive-tissue hypothesis. The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology 36(2), 199221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aitchison, J., 1996. The Seeds of Speech: Language Origin and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Andrews, P. & Stringer, C., 1993. The primates' progress, in The Book of Life, ed. Gould, S.J.. London: Ebury Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Arensburg, B., 1989. New skeletal evidence concerning the anatomy of Middle Palaeolithic populations in the Middle East: the Kebara skeleton, in Mellars & Stringer (eds.), 165–71.Google Scholar
Bailey, P., Von Bonin, G. & McCulloch, W.S., 1950. The Isocortex of the Chimpanzee. Urbana (IL): University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Bar-Yosef, O., 1994. The contribution of southwest Asia to the study of the origins of modern humans, in Origins of Anatomically Modern Humans, eds. Nitecki, M.T. & Nitecki, D.V.. New York (NY): Plenum Press, 2366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, R.H., 1993. Lateralization of neural control for vocalization by the frog (Rana pipiens). Psychobiology 21(3), 243–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begun, D. & Walker, A., 1993. The endocast, in Walker & Leakey (eds.), 326–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D., 1990. Language & Species. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D., 1995. Language and Human Behavior. Seattle (WA): University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., 1991. Teaching in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 41, 530–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, J. & Sartono, S. (eds.), 1996. Evolution and Ecology of Homo erectus. Leiden: Pithecanthropus Centennial Foundation.Google Scholar
Bowler, J.M. & Thome, A., 1976. Human remains from Lake Mungo: discovery and excavation of Lake Mungo III, in The Origin of the Australians, eds. Kirk, R.L. & Thorne, A.G.. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies, 127–38.Google Scholar
Brace, C.L., 1995. Biocultural interaction and the mechanism of mosaic evolution in the emergence of ‘modern’ morphology. American Anthropologist 97(4), 711–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradshaw, J.L. & Rogers, L.J., 1993. The Evolution of Lateral Asymmetries, Language, Tool Use, and Intellect. Sydney: Academic Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Brain, C.K., 1981. The Hunters or the Hunted? Chicago (IL) & London: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Burling, R., 1993. Primate calls, human language, and nonverbal communication. Current Anthropology 34(1), 2553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers, A.M., 1994. Symboling and the Middle-Upper Palaeolithic transition: a theoretical and methodological critique. Current Anthropology 35, 369–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers, A.M., forthcoming. Communication and material culture: Pleistocene tools as action cues. Cambridge Archaeological Journal.Google Scholar
Byrne, R.W., 1996. Relating brain size to intelligence in primates, in Mellars & Gibson (eds.), 4956.Google Scholar
Cachel, S. & Harris, J., 1996. Ranging patterns, land-use and subsistence in Homo erectus from the perspective of evolutionary ecology, in Bower & (eds.), 5166.Google Scholar
Chauvet, J.M., Brunet-Deschamps, E. & Hillaire, C., 1995. La grotte Chauvet à Vallon Pont-dArc. Paris: Seuil.Google Scholar
Cheney, D.L. & Seyfarth, R.M., 1990. How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D.L. & Seyfarth, R.M., 1996. Function and intention in the calls of non-human primates, in The Evolution of Social Behaviour Patterns in Primates and Man, eds. Runciman, W.G., Maynard Smith, J. & Dunbar, R.I.. (Proceedings of the British Academy 88.) Oxford: Oxford University Press, 5976.Google Scholar
Clark, J.D., 1992. African and Asian perspectives on the origins of modern humans, in The Origin of Modern Humans and the Impact of Chronometric Dating, eds. Aitken, M., Stringer, C.B. & Mellars, P.A.. (Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, series B, 337, no. 1280.) London: Royal Society, 201–16.Google Scholar
Clottes, J., 1996. Recent studies on Palaeolithic art. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 6 (2), 179–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connolly, C.J., 1950. External Morphology of the Primate Brain. Springfield (IL): Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Corvinus, G., 1976. Prehistoric exploration at Hadar, Ethiopia. Nature 261, 571–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corvinus, G. & Roche, H., 1976. La préhistoire dans la région d'Hadar (Bassin de l'Awash Afar, Éthiopie): prémiers resultats. L’Anthropologie 80, 315–24.Google Scholar
Corvinus, G. & Roche, H., 1980. Prehistoric exploration at Hadar in the Afar (Ethiopia) in 1973,1974 and 1976, in Proceedings of 8th Pan-African Congress of Prehistory and Quaternary Studies, Nairobi, 1977, eds. Leakey, R.E. & Ogot, B.A.. Nairobi: The International Louis Leakey Memorial Institute for African Prehistory, 168–88.Google Scholar
Cottrell, B. & Kamminga, J., 1987. The formation of flakes. American Antiquity 52(4), 675708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchley, M., 1953. The Parietal Lobes. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Dart, R.A., 1925. Australopithecus africanus: the man-ape of South Africa. Nature 115, 195–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dart, R.A., 1957. The Osteodontokeratic Culture of Australopithecus prometheus. (Memoir 10.) Pretoria: Transvaal Museum.Google Scholar
Davidson, I., 1990. Bilzingsleben and early marking. Rock Art Research 7, 52–6.Google Scholar
Davidson, I., 1991. The archaeology of language origins: a review. Antiquity 65, 39—48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, I. & Noble, W., 1989. The archaeology of perception: traces of depiction and language. Current Anthropology 30(2), 125–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, I. & Noble, W., 1992. Why the first colonisation of the Australian region is the earliest evidence of modern human behaviour. Arclweology in Oceania 27, 135–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, I. & Noble, W., 1993. Tools and language in human evolution, in Gibson & Ingold (eds.), 363–87.Google Scholar
Deacon, H., 1989. Late Pleistocene palaeoecology and archaeology in the southern Cape, South Africa, in Mellars & Stringer (eds.), 547–64.Google Scholar
Deacon, T., 1994. Interview, cited in Ape Man: the Story of Human Evolution, by R. Caird. London: Boxtree, 99101.Google Scholar
Defleur, A., 1993. Les Sépultures Moustériennes. Paris: CNRS.Google Scholar
Donald, M., 1991. Origins of the Modem Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R., 1996. Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Eccles, J.C., 1989. Evolution of the Brain: Creation of the Self. London & New York (NY): Routledge.Google Scholar
Eccles, J.C. & Creutzfeldt, O. (eds.), 1990. The principles of design and operation of the brain. Pontifical Academy of Sciences, Scripta Varia 78, 1589.Google Scholar
Falk, D., 1980. Language, handedness, and primate brains: did the Australopithecines sign? American Anthropologist 82, 72–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, D., 1983. Cerebral cortices of East African early hominids. Science 221, 1072–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, D., 1989. Ape-like endocast of ‘ape-man’ Taung. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 80, 335–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, D., 1992. Braindance. New York (NY): Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Falk, D., 1993. Sex differences in visuo-spatial skills: implications for hominid evolution, in Gibson & Ingold (eds.), 216–29.Google Scholar
Foley, R.A. & Lee, P.C., 1991. Ecology and energetics of encephalization in hominid evolution. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London, B 334, 223–32.Google ScholarPubMed
Gamble, C.S., 1993. Timewalkers: the Prehistory of Global Colonisation. Stroud, Gloucestershire: Alan Sutton.Google Scholar
Geschwind, N., 1965. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Part I. Brain 88, 237–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, K.R., 1990. New perspectives on instincts and intelligence: brain size and the emergence of hierarchical mental constructional skills, in Language and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes, eds. Parker, S. & Gibson, K.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 97128.Google Scholar
Gibson, K.R., 1996. The biocultural human brain, seasonal migrations, and the emergence of the Upper Palaeolithic, in Mellars & Gibson (eds.), 3346.Google Scholar
Gibson, K.R. & Ingold, T. (eds.), 1993. Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goodall, J., 1986. The Chimpanzees ofGombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge (MA) & London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S.J., 1981. The Mismeasure of Man. New York (NY): Norton.Google Scholar
Gould, S.J. & Vrba, E.S., 1982. Exaptation — a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8, 415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J., 1984a. Mental abilities of early Man: a look at some hard evidence, in Hominid Evolution and Community Ecology, ed. Foley, R.. London: Academic Press, 167–92.Google Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J., 1986. Culture and conceptualisation: the Oldowan-Acheulian gradient, in Stone Age Prehistory, eds. Bailey, G.N. & Callow, P.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 243–60.Google Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J., 1996. Mental abilities of early Homo: elements of constraint and choice in rule systems, in Mellars & Gibson (eds.), 191215.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P., 1991. Language, tools, and the brain: the development and evolution of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Evolution 14, 531.Google Scholar
Groves, C.P., 1989. A Theory of Human and Primate Evolution. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Griin, R. & Stringer, C.B., 1991. Electron spin resonance dating of the evolution of modern humans. Archaeometry 33, 153–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, J., 1986. Kraft und Aggression. Die Botschaft der Eiszeitkunst im Aurignacien Suddeutschlands? Tübingen: Institut für Urgeschichte der Universitat Tübingen.Google Scholar
Harpending, H.C., Sherry, S.T., Rogers, A.R. & Stoneking, M., 1993. The genetic structure of ancient human populations. Current Anthropology 34(4), 483–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J.W.K., 1983. Cultural beginnings: Plio-Pleistocene archaeological occurrences from the Afar, Ethiopia. The African Archaeological Review 1, 331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C.F., 1960. The human revolution. Scientific American 203(9), 8996.Google Scholar
Hodder, I., 1991. Reading the Past. (2nd edition.) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Holloway, R.L., 1969. Culture: a human domain. Current Anthropology 10, 395413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holloway, R.L., 1972. New australopithecine endocast, SK 1585, from Swartkrans, South Africa. American journal of Physical Anthropology 37,173–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holloway, R.L., 1981. Culture, symbols, and human brain evolution: a synthesis. Dialectical Anthropology 5, 287303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holloway, R.L., 1983a. Cerebral brain endocast pattern of Australopithecus afarensis hominid. Nature 303, 420–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holloway, R.L., 1983b. Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior. Human Neurobiology 2, 105–14.Google ScholarPubMed
Holloway, R.L., 1996. Evolutionary of the human brain, in Locke & Peters (eds.), 74108.Google Scholar
Houghton, P., 1993. Neandertal supralaryngeal vocal tract. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 90, 139–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaac, G.L., 1978. The archaeological evidence for the activities of early African hominids, in Early Hominids of Africa, ed. Jolly, C.J.. London: Duckworth, 219–54.Google Scholar
Jerison, H.J., 1973. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York (NY): Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jerison, H., 1977. Evolution of the brain, in The Human Brain, ed. Wittrock, M.C.. Englewood Cliffs (NY): Prentice Hall, 3962.Google Scholar
Jerison, H., 1991. Brain Size and the Evolution of Mind. James Arthur Lecture, American Museum of Natural History, New York City.Google Scholar
Jones, P., 1995. Results of experimental work in relation to the stone industries of Olduvai Gorge, in Olduvai Gorge, vol. 5: Excavation in Beds III-IV and the Masek Beds, 1968–1971, eds. Leakey, M. & Roe, D.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 254–98.Google Scholar
Joulian, F., 1996. Comparing chimpanzee and early hominid techniques, in Mellars & Gibson (eds.), 173–89.Google Scholar
Keeley, L.H. & Toth, N., 1981. Microwear polishes on early stone tools from Koobi Fora, Kenya. Nature 293, 464–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kempler, D., 1996. Disorders of language and tool use: neurological and cognitive links, in Gibson & Ingold (eds.), 193215.Google Scholar
Knight, C.D., 1991. Blood Relations: Menstruation and the Origins of Culture. New Haven (CT): Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Knight, C., Power, C. & Watts, I., 1995. The human symbolic revolution: a Darwinian account. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 5(1), 75114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krings, M., Stone, A., Schmitz, R.W., Krainitzki, H., Stoneking, M. & Pääbo, S., 1997. Neandertal DNA sequences and the origin of modern humans. Cell 90(1), 930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leakey, L.S.B., Tobias, P.V. & Napier, J.R., 1964. A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai Gorge. Nature 202, 79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leigh, S.R., 1992. Cranial capacity evolution in Homo ereclus and early Homo sapiens. American journal of Physical Anthropology 87, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis-Williams, D., 1993. The Rock Art of Southern Africa. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, D.E. & Shea, J.J., 1994. Behavioral differences between Archaic and Modern humans in the Levantine Mousterian. American Anthropologist 96(2), 300332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P., 1984. The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., 1989. The origins of some aspects of human language and cognition, in Mellars & Stringer (eds.), 391414.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., 1990. Uniquely Human: the Evolution of Speech, Thought, and Selfless Behavior. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., 1995. Manual versus speech motor control and the evolution of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18, 197–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P., Laitman, J.T., Reidenberg, J.S. & Gannon, P.J., 1992. The anatomy, physiology, acoustics and perception of speech: essential elements in analysis of the evolution of human speech. Journal of Human Evolution 23, 447–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, A. & Peters, C., 1996a. Editorial appendix II: Evolution of the human vocal apparatus, in Locke & Peters (eds.), 116–25.Google Scholar
Locke, A. & Peters, C. (eds.), 1996b. Handbook of Human Symbolic Evolution. Oxford: Oxford Science Publications.Google Scholar
Lynch, T.F., 1990. Glacial-age Man in South America? A critical review. American Antiquity 55(1), 1236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLarnon, A., 1993. The vertebral canal, in Walker & Leakey (eds.), 359–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshack, A., 1988. The Neandertals and the human capacity for symbolic thought: cognitive and problem-solving aspects of Mousterian symbol. Etudes et Recherches Archéologiques de VUniversité de Liège 32, 5791.Google Scholar
Mellars, P.A., 1989. Major issues in the emergence of modern humans. Current Anthropology 30, 349–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellars, P.A., 1991. Cognitive changes and the emergence of modern humans in Europe. Cambridge Archaeology Journal 1(1), 6376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellars, P.A., 1996a. The Neanderthal Legacy: an Archaeological Perspective from Western Europe. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellars, P.A., 1996b. Symbolism, language and the Neanderthal mind, in Mellars & Gibson (eds.), 1532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellars, P. A., 1996c. The emergence of biologically modern populations in Europe: a social and cognitive ‘revolution’?, in The Evolution of Social Behaviour Patterns in Primates and Man, eds. Runciman, W.G., Maynard Smith, J. & Dunbar, R.I.. (Proceedings of the British Academy 88.) Oxford: Oxford University Press, 179201.Google Scholar
Mellars, P.A., in press. Neanderthals, modern humans, and the archaeological evidence for language, in The Origins of Language, ed. Jablonski, N.. San Francisco (CA): California Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Mellars, P.A. & Gibson, K. (eds.), 1996. Modelling the Early Human Mind. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.Google Scholar
Mellars, P.A. & Stringer, C. (eds.), 1989. The Human Revolution: Behavioural and Biological Perspectives on the Origins of Modern Humans. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Morse, K., 1993. Shell beads from Mandu Mandu rockshelter, Cape Range Peninsula, Western Australia, before 30,000 bp. Antiquity 67, 877–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., 1991. The evolutionary emergence of modern human behaviour: Language and its archaeology. Man: Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 26, 223–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., 1996. Human Evolution, Language and Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., in press. Discovering the symbolic potential of communicative signs, in The Archaeology of Mind, ed. Nowell, A.. New York (NY): Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Parker, S.T. & Gibson, K.R., 1979. A developmental model for the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2, 367–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, S.T. & Gibson, K.R., 1990. ‘Language’ and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., 1994. The Language Instinct: the New Science of Language and Mind. London: Penguin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roche, H. & Tiercelin, J.J., 1977. Découverte d'une industrie lithique ancienne in situ dans la formation d'Hadar, Afar central, Ethiopie. C.R. Acad. Sci. Paris D284, 1871–4.Google Scholar
Rolland, N. & Dibble, H.L., 1990. A new synthesis of Middle Paleolithic variability. American Antiquity 55(3), 480–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schepers, G.W.H., 1946. The endocranial casts of the South African apemen, in The South African Fossil Apemen: the Australopithecinae, eds. Broom, R. & Schepers, G.W.H.. (Memoir 2.) Pretoria: Transvaal Museum, 153272.Google Scholar
Searle, J., 1969. Speech Acts: an Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, R. & Wymer, J., 1982. The Middle Stone Age at Klasies River Mouth in South Africa. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Skelton, R.R., McHenry, H.M. & Drawhorn, G.M., 1986. Phylogenetic analysis of early hominids. Current Anthropology 27, 2143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G.E., 1907. A new topographical survey of the human cerebral cortex, being an account of the anatomically distinct cortical areas and their relationship to the cerebral sulci. Journal of Anatomy 41, 237–54.Google Scholar
Stent, G.S., 1972. Prematurity and uniqueness in scientific discovery. Scientific American 227, 8493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stringer, C. & McKie, R., 1996. African Exodus: the Origins of Modern Humanity. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1965. Australopithecus, Homo habilis, tool-using and tool-making. South African Archaeological Bulletin 20, 167–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1967. Cultural hominization among the earliest African Pleistocene hominids. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 33, 367–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1971. The Brain in Hominid Evolution. New York (NY): Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1975. Brain evolution in the Hominoidea, in Primate Functional Morphology and Evolution, ed. Tuttle, R.. The Hague: Mouton, 353–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1979. Men, minds and hands: cultural awakenings over two million years of humanity. South African Archaeological Bulletin 34, 8592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1980. L'évolution du cerveau humain. La Recherche 11, 282–92.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1981a. The Evolution of the Human Brain, Intellect and Spirit. Adelaide: University of Adelaide, 170.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1981b. The emergence of man in Africa and beyond. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London 292, 4356.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1981c. From palaeo-anatomy to culture: the 10th International Congress of Prehistoric and Protohistoric Sciences, Mexico City, 19–24 October 1981. Adas, 120–48.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1982. The antiquity of man: human evolution, in Progress in Clinical and Biological Research, part A: Human Genetics, ed. Bonne-Tamir, B.. New York (NY): Alan R. Liss, Inc., 195214.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1983a. Recent advances in the evolution of the hominids with especial reference to brain and speech. Pontifical Academy of Sciences, Scripta Varia 50, 85140.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1983b. Hominid evolution in Africa. Canadian Journal of Anthropology 3, 163–90.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1987. The brain of Homo habilis: a new level of organization in cerebral evolution. Journal of Human Evolution 16, 741–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1990. Some critical steps in the evolution of the hominid brain. Pontifical Academy of Sciences, Scripta Varia 78, 116.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1991. Olduvai Gorge, vols. 4A & 4B: The Skulls, Endocasts and Teeth of Homo habilis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1992. The species Homo habilis: example of a premature discovery. Annales Zoologici Fennici 28, 371–80.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1995. The Communication of the Dead: Earliest Vestiges of the Origin of Articulate Language. (17th Kroon Lecture.) Amsterdam: Stichting Nederlands Museum Voor Anthropologie en Praehistorie.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1996a. Premature discoveries in science with especial reference to Australopithecus and Homo habilis. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 140, 4964.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V., 1996b. The dating of linguistic beginnings. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19, 789–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A.C. & Ratner, H.H., 1993. Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, 495552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toth, N., 1985a. The Oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artefacts. Journal of Archaeological Science 12, 101–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toth, N., 1985b. Archaeological evidence for preferential right-handedness in the Lower and Middle Pleistocene. Journal of Human Evolution 14, 607–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toth, N. & Schick, K., 1993. Early stone industries and inferences regarding language and cognition, in Gibson & Ingold (eds.), 346–62.Google Scholar
Vandermeersch, B., 1970. Une sepulture mousterien avec offrandes decouverte dans la grotte de Qafzeh. Comptes Rendus de I’Academie des Sciences de Paris 270, 298301.Google Scholar
Walker, A.C., 1993. Perspectives on the Nariokotome discovery, in Walker & Leakey (eds.), 411–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, A.C. & Leakey, R. (eds.), 1993. The Nariokotome Homo erectus Skeleton. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenban-Smith, F., 1989. The use of canonical variates for determination of biface manufacturing technology at Boxgrove Lower Palaeolithic site and the behavioral implications of this technology. Journal of Archaeological Science 16, 1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whallon, R., 1989. Elements of cultural change in the later Palaeolithic, in Mellars & Stringer (eds.), 433–54.Google Scholar
Wheeler, P.E., 1985. The loss of functional body hair in man: the influence of thermal environment, body form and bipedality. Journal of Human Evolution 14, 23–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitcombe, E., 1996. The anatomical foundations of cognition: suggestions for a reinterpretation, in Mellars & Gibson (eds.), 81–8.Google Scholar
White, R., 1989. Production complexity and standardisation in early Aurignacian bead and pendant manufacture: evolutionary implications, in Mellars & Stringer (eds.), 366–90.Google Scholar
Wilkins, W.K. & Wakefield, J., 1995. Brain evolution and neurolinguistic preconditions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18, 161226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, R.V.S. (ed.), 1971. Archaeology of the Gallus Site, Koonalda Cave. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.Google Scholar
Wynn, T., 1979. The intelligence of later Acheulean hominids. Man: Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 14, 371–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., 1991. Tools, grammar, and the archaeology of cognition. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 1(2), 191— 206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., 1993. Two developments in the mind of early Homo. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 12, 299322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., 1995. Handaxe enigmas. World Archaeology 27, 1024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., 1996. What artefacts reveal about the mind of Homo erectus, in Bower & Sartono (eds.), 263–74.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. & McGrew, W.C., 1989. An ape's eye view of the Oldowan. Man: Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 24, 383–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar