Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T09:22:01.713Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Oviposition in the onion fly Delia antiqua (Diptera: Anthomyiidae) is socially facilitated by visual cues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2020

Sugihiko Hoshizaki*
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Applied Entomology, Graduate School of Agricultural and Life Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
Noriaki Koshikawa
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Applied Entomology, Graduate School of Agricultural and Life Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
Takuya Toyoda
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Applied Entomology, Graduate School of Agricultural and Life Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
Yukio Ishikawa
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Applied Entomology, Graduate School of Agricultural and Life Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
*
Author for correspondence: Sugihiko Hoshizaki, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Ovipositional decisions in herbivorous insects may be affected by social information from conspecifics. Social facilitation of oviposition has been suggested for the onion fly Delia antiqua. In the current study, we found that D. antiqua oviposition was unequal between paired oviposition stations of equal quality and that more eggs were laid on an oviposition station baited with decoy flies than on the control. The increased oviposition toward the decoys continued over time >8 h. When decoys were placed upside down, the number of eggs laid did not differ between the decoy and control sides of oviposition stations, suggesting that social facilitation of oviposition is mediated by visual cues. Based on these findings, mechanisms of social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua were discussed.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

When laying eggs, female herbivorous insects need to find their host plants, choose among potential host plants, and then decide how many eggs to lay there. Such decision may be affected by social information such as oviposition by conspecifics (Danchin et al., Reference Danchin, Giraldeau, Valone and Wagner2004; Wagner and Danchin, Reference Wagner and Danchin2010). Oviposition by early-arriving females may deter oviposition by late-arriving conspecifics (e.g., Roitberg and Prokopy, Reference Roitberg and Prokopy1987; Li and Ishikawa, Reference Li and Ishikawa2005; Tanaka and Sugahara, Reference Tanaka and Sugahara2017) or may facilitate oviposition by late arrivers (social facilitation of oviposition; e.g., Browne et al., Reference Browne, Bartell and Shorey1969; Prokopy and Duan, Reference Prokopy and Duan1998; Prokopy and Roitberg, Reference Prokopy and Roitberg2001; Pasqualone and Davis, Reference Pasqualone and Davis2011; see also Otake and Dobata, Reference Otake and Dobata2018).

Maggots of the onion fly Delia antiqua (Diptera: Anthomyiidae) damage the bulb of onions and related plants in Europe, Asia, and North America (Ning et al., Reference Ning, Wei and Feng2017b); their life history is adapted to a wide range of climatic conditions (e.g., Ishikawa et al., Reference Ishikawa, Tsukada and Matsumoto1987; Nomura and Ishikawa, Reference Nomura and Ishikawa2001). The onion fly is a model for studying host selection in herbivorous insects (Visser, Reference Visser1986; Renwick, Reference Renwick1989; Romeis et al., Reference Romeis, Ebbinghaus and Scherkenbeck2003; Gouinguené and Städler, Reference Gouinguené and Städler2005; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Birch, Gregory and Murray2006). Female flies of D. antiqua find hosts, such as onions, using visual and olfactory cues (Matsumoto and Thorsteinson, Reference Matsumoto and Thorsteinson1968; Ishikawa et al., Reference Ishikawa, Ikeshoji and Matsumoto1978, Reference Ishikawa, Tanaka and Matsumoto1985; Harris and Miller, Reference Harris and Miller1991; Degen and Städler, Reference Degen and Städler1996; Gouinguené et al., Reference Gouinguené, Buser and Städler2005; Ning et al., Reference Ning, Yang, Fan and Feng2017a). Several studies have suggested the social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua (Vernon, Reference Vernon1979; Harris and Miller, Reference Harris and Miller1983); for example, a group of D. antiqua females often aggressively lay eggs on the cage floor when several hundreds of flies are confined without particular oviposition substrates (personal observation by the authors). Judd and Borden (Reference Judd and Borden1992) presented experimental evidence suggesting that oviposition of D. antiqua is facilitated to a weak degree by stimuli associated with ovipositing females and newly laid eggs. As the social facilitation of oviposition may confound laboratory bioassays of ovipositional stimuli in insects (Judd and Borden, Reference Judd and Borden1992), a better understanding of the mechanisms of social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua is indispensable for the accurate assessment of oviposition stimulants.

The social information received by late-arriving females may induce different responses depending on the degree and timing of previous exploitation of oviposition sites (Lam et al., Reference Lam, Babor, Duthie, Babor, Moore and Gries2007; Wasserberg et al., Reference Wasserberg, Bailes, Davis and Yeoman2014; Stephan et al., Reference Stephan, Stenberg and Bjӧrkman2015). For example, in the housefly, olfactory cues from eggs laid on the larval food change from induction to inhibition of oviposition over time in late-arriving females (Lam et al., Reference Lam, Babor, Duthie, Babor, Moore and Gries2007). In D. antiqua, it remains unclear whether social information facilitating oviposition by late-arriving females continues over time or is independent of increases in the degree of oviposition site exploitation.

In D. antiqua, while the use of chemical cues in the social facilitation of oviposition has been suggested (Judd and Borden, Reference Judd and Borden1992), involvement of visual cues has not been examined. Indeed, the use of visual information emitted by conspecifics was found in the damsel fly (Byers and Eason, Reference Byers and Eason2009).

The current study first aimed to verify the social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua by examining whether the number of eggs laid is unequal between paired oviposition stations of equal quality. Then, based on the finding that D. antiqua oviposition is biased to an oviposition station baited with decoy flies, we tested two specific hypotheses: (1) social information facilitating oviposition in D. antiqua continues over time and (2) socially facilitated oviposition in D. antiqua is mediated by visual information from conspecifics.

Materials and methods

Study insects

A laboratory population of D. antiqua originating from Hokkaido, Japan (Kayukawa et al., Reference Kayukawa, Chen, Hoshizaki and Ishikawa2007) has been maintained for >15 years according to the methods described by Ishikawa et al. (Reference Ishikawa, Mochizuki, Ikeshoji and Matsumoto1983) and Kayukawa et al. (Reference Kayukawa, Chen, Hoshizaki and Ishikawa2007). Newly eclosed adults were reared in mesh cages (25 cm × 25 cm × 25 cm), and supplied with water, sugar cubes, and dried yeast at 23°C for 12–15 days until sexual maturation and mating (Ishikawa, Reference Ishikawa1979; Spencer and Miller, Reference Spencer and Miller2002).

Oviposition station

A plastic cup (9 cm in diameter; 4.5 cm in height) was filled with fine gravel, and 10 ml of water was poured into the cup. At the center of the top of the gravel field, a piece of green onion leaf (‘Ban-nou-negi’; 10 cm in length) was vertically positioned with the core of a piece of steel wire (a standard oviposition station; Supplementary fig. S1). Eggs were laid in the gravel near the stand of the green onion in oviposition stations. A pair of standard oviposition stations was prepared using a single green onion plant, immediately before each trial.

Decoys

Gravid females of D. antiqua were chosen from 12 to 15 day-old flies based on the red color on the ventral side of abdomen (Ishikawa, Reference Ishikawa1979; Spencer and Miller, Reference Spencer and Miller2002). They were anesthetized by using CO2 and then kept under −20°C for 1 h immediately before experiments using decoys. In each trial of experiments 2–4, five decoys were placed around the green onion stand in one of the paired standard oviposition stations (decoy side).

Experimental conditions

Two oviposition stations were placed on the floor of a mesh cage (25 cm × 25 cm × 25 cm) together with a water bottle, a sugar cube, and a small dish of dried yeast (Supplementary fig. S1). In each trial, 20 gravid female D. antiqua flies were released into the test cage immediately before the experiments started. The test cage was surrounded by a sheet (30 cm in height) of black paper. Experiments were conducted at 23°C and 15 L:9 D; the light phase in the room started at 8:00 and terminated at 23:00, and all experiments were started between 12:00 and 17:00. Thus, the test flies were allowed to start oviposition in the late afternoon during the photoperiod; D. antiqua females lay eggs most actively in the evening (Havukkala and Miller, Reference Havukkala and Miller1987).

Experiment 1

Two standard oviposition stations, representing oviposition sites of equal quality, were presented for 24 h. Eggs were recovered from each oviposition station and counted. Four test cages were used with ten trials per cage.

Experiment 2

Dead bodies of D. antiqua were placed on their legs, on one of the two standard oviposition stations. Eggs were recovered from each oviposition station and counted 24 h after the release of test flies. A total of 16 trials were conducted in four test cages. The decoy placement within cages was rotated among trials to avoid bias toward one side of the cage.

Experiment 3

Decoys were used as in experiment 2. Eggs were recovered from each oviposition station and counted 0.25, 1, 4, 8, or 24 h after the release of test flies. For 0.25-h oviposition, a total of 32 trials were conducted in five cages. In every cage, the decoy placement was rotated among trials. For 1-, 4-, 8-, or 24-h oviposition, the experiment was conducted in a total of 16 trials in the same manner as experiment 2.

Experiment 4

Decoys were placed on their back, on one of the two standard oviposition stations. Eggs were recovered from each oviposition station and counted 24 h after the release of test flies. The experiment was conducted in a total of 16 trials in the same manner as experiment 2.

Statistical analyses

For experiment 1, the probability of egg presence in one oviposition station in each trial was assessed against 0.5 by the binomial test. Trials with no significant differences from 0.5 were considered ties. Then, the potential oviposition preference toward either oviposition-station place over the other place in the cage was evaluated. The number of trials in favor of one place and that in favor of the other place were compared by the bilateral sign test, disregarding the tied trials, for each cage.

For experiments 2–4, the oviposition preference toward a particular side was examined in two approaches with different null hypotheses. The first approach adopted the null hypothesis that the number of trials in favor of the decoy side was equal to that in favor of control side. For each trial, the probability of egg presence on the decoy side was assessed against 0.5 by the binomial test. Trials with no significant differences from 0.5 were considered ties. Then, the numbers of trials in favor of the decoy and control sides were compared by the sign test, disregarding the tied trials.

The second approach adopted the null hypothesis that the numbers of eggs on the decoy and control sides were equal. Generalized linear mixed effect models (GLMM) were used to test this hypothesis. The dependent variables used were the numbers of eggs on the decoy and control sides. The treatment was used as the fixed factor, and the trial was used as the random factor for the slope and intercept. Poisson distributions were used as the error distribution.

In the statistical analyses, the two approaches were adopted to complement each other. In the first approach, the numbers of trials in favor of the decoy and control sides were compared. In this approach, the statistical power would not be significantly affected by the degree of bias in oviposition toward a particular side of stations. On the other hand, the statistical power decreases as the number of tied trials increases, and hence may differ between experiments. The second approach does not disregard tied trials in the analysis; however, when trials in favor of the decoy and control sides occurred within an experiment, the distribution of egg number for a particular side of oviposition stations may not follow the assumed one.

R version 3.3.2 (R Core Team, 2016) was used for the statistical analyses. The glmer() function in the R package ‘lme4’ was employed for the GLMM analyses.

Results

Experiment 1

D. antiqua females were allowed to lay eggs at a pair of oviposition stations of equal quality for 24 h. In this and the other experiments, no or few eggs were laid outside the oviposition stations (personal observations by N. K., T. T., and S. H.). The even distribution of eggs between the oviposition stations tested in the pair was rejected in 36 of 40 trials (table 1). In every cage, the number of trials in favor of a particular side in the cage was not significantly larger than that in favor of the other side (table 1). The median ratio of egg number at the oviposition station with more eggs to the total eggs was 0.59 (fig. 1).

Figure 1. Distribution of eggs over two oviposition stations of equal quality (experiment 1). The egg ratio indicates the number of eggs on the side with more eggs divided by the total number of eggs. Circles and crosses represent trials rejecting (binomial test, P < 0.01) and supporting, respectively, the null hypothesis of the even distribution of eggs. The box plot indicates the median, and the first and third quartiles.

Table 1. Experiment 1: the egg distribution between two oviposition stations of equal quality in four cages

For each trial, the probability of egg presence at one oviposition station was assessed against 0.5 by the binomial test with the criterion of P < 0.05. Trials with P > 0.05 were considered ties. The number of trials with more eggs on one side of oviposition stations and that with more eggs on the other side were compared by the bilateral sign test.

Thus, the egg distribution was mostly unequal between the paired oviposition stations of equal quality. Attention should be paid when interpreting the results of the following experiments, as the egg distribution between the paired oviposition stations may be unequal even if bait treatment is not effective.

Experiment 2

D. antiqua females were allowed to lay eggs at paired oviposition stations, one baited with decoys placed on their legs and the other not baited with decoys, for 24 h. The number of trials with more eggs on the decoy side was significantly larger than that with more eggs on the control side (table 2). In the GLMM analysis, the fixed effect of decoys was significant (z = 2.87, P = 0.004; fig. 2). Note that the data variance was large in experiment 2 and the others in the current study; we dealt with that using mixed models. The median ratio of egg number on the decoy side to the total number was 0.59. Thus, females laid more eggs in those oviposition stations where dead bodies were present.

Figure 2. The GLMM analysis for experiments 2 (left) and 4 (right). Pairs of circles connected by lines represent single trials of the experiment. The thick line indicates the model prediction.

Table 2. Experiments 2 and 4: effects of decoys on oviposition facilitation

Decoys were placed on their legs or back. For each trial, the probability of egg presence on the decoy side was assessed against 0.5 by the binomial test with the criterion of P < 0.05. Trials with P > 0.05 were considered ties. The number of trials with more eggs on the decoy side and that with more eggs on the control side were compared by the bilateral sign test.

Experiment 3

D. antiqua females were allowed to lay eggs at paired oviposition stations as in experiment 2, but for different lengths of time. After 0.25-h oviposition, more eggs tended to be laid on the decoy side, but the numbers of trials with more eggs on the decoy and control sides were not significantly different (table 3). In the GLMM analysis, the fixed effect of decoys was not significant (z = 1.21, P = 0.226; fig. 3). The median ratio of eggs on the decoy side to total eggs after 0.25-h oviposition was 0.69, and was similar to the median egg probabilities observed in experiments 1 and 2.

Figure 3. The GLMM analysis for experiment 3. Pairs of circles connected by black lines represent single trials of the experiment. The thick line indicates the model prediction.

Table 3. Experiment 3: oviposition facilitation for varying durations of oviposition when decoys were placed on their legs

For each trial, the probability of egg presence on the decoy side was assessed against 0.5 by the binomial test with the criterion of P < 0.05. Trials with P > 0.05 were considered ties. The number of trials with more eggs on the decoy side and that with more eggs on the control side were compared by the bilateral sign test.

After 1-, 4-, and 24-h oviposition, the number of trials with more eggs on the decoy side was significantly larger than that with more eggs on the control side; however, no significant difference was found for 8-h oviposition (table 3). In GLMM analyses, the fixed effect of decoys was significant after all of the oviposition periods examined (1-h oviposition: z = 2.79, P = 0.005; 4-h oviposition: z = 2.05, P = 0.04; 8-h oviposition: z = 2.2, P = 0.028; 24-h oviposition: z = 3.66, P = 0.00025; fig. 3). The median probability of egg presence on the decoy side after 1, 4, 8, or 24 h was approximately 0.6 (0.58–0.66).

The ratio of egg number on the decoy side to the total number of eggs was not correlated with the oviposition duration (Kendall's τ = −0.0545, z = −0.706, P = 0.48). The total egg number continued to increase from 0.25 to 24 h (Kendall's τ = 0.833, z = 10.799, P < 0.001; fig. 4), demonstrating that oviposition continued.

Figure 4. Total egg number at different times (experiment 3). One circle represents one trial. For both panels, the box plot indicates the median, and first and third quartiles.

Experiment 4

Decoys were used as in experiment 2, but they were placed upside down. Oviposition was allowed for 24 h. There were no significant differences between the number of trials with excess of eggs on the decoy side and that on the control side (table 2). In the GLMM analysis, the fixed effect of decoys was not significant (z = 0.20, P = 0.84; fig. 2). The median ratio of egg on the decoy side to total eggs was 0.46.

Discussion

D. antiqua females laid eggs unequally between two oviposition stations of equal quality over 24 h (experiment 1). This indicates that the oviposition was socially facilitated by already laid eggs, some traces of oviposition, and/or egg-laying females themselves. This finding is consistent with the previous study by Judd and Borden (Reference Judd and Borden1992).

When one of the two oviposition stations was baited with dead D. antiqua bodies placed on their legs, oviposition was mostly concentrated to the decoy side in almost all trials (experiment 2). This confirms the occurrence of social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua and indicates that the oviposition was facilitated by a cue(s) other than already laid eggs or some traces of oviposition. The degree of egg aggregation was weak (median probability of egg presence of 0.59 on the decoy side), as reported by Judd and Borden (Reference Judd and Borden1992).

The results of experiment 1 are particularly significant in that D. antiqua oviposition was socially facilitated without presenting extrinsic stimuli. Previous assays of social facilitation of D. antiqua oviposition (Judd and Borden, Reference Judd and Borden1992) used extrinsic stimuli such as ten females, 300 eggs, or ovipositor extracts of D. antiqua. Such experimental designs represent a situation in which eggs are already concentrated at an oviposition site to a certain degree, and not an initial phase of the oviposition facilitation process.

In several insects, the degree of egg congestion at an oviposition site or the time since previous exploitation of an oviposition site affects the message of social cues; the message to late-arriving females changes from induction to inhibition of oviposition when an oviposition site becomes overcrowded or a prolonged period has passed since the previous oviposition (Lam et al., Reference Lam, Babor, Duthie, Babor, Moore and Gries2007; Wasserberg et al., Reference Wasserberg, Bailes, Davis and Yeoman2014; Stephan et al., Reference Stephan, Stenberg and Bjӧrkman2015). In contrast, in D. antiqua, the degree of egg concentration to decoys did not significantly change with time (experiment 3 in the current study), suggesting that social facilitation continues even after the oviposition stations become crowded with eggs. This by itself may not be a notable finding; however, we did not find other reports of the continuous facilitation of oviposition via social information.

The results of experiment 3 suggest that decoys placed on their legs continued to facilitate conspecific oviposition for >8 h. This raises a question about the possibility of social facilitation of oviposition during the scotophase; however, this is not likely because D. antiqua females are known to rarely lay eggs during the scotophase (Havukkala and Miller, Reference Havukkala and Miller1987).

Decoys placed on their legs facilitated oviposition by conspecifics (experiment 2). However, such facilitation was not observed when the decoys were placed on their back (experiment 4). This suggests that the social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua involves visual cues. We speculate that the decoys placed on their legs and back were recognized as living and dead flies, respectively, because flies died naturally tend to lie on their back on the ground.

The social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua was previously explained by olfactory cues, i.e., egg and female factors (Judd and Borden, Reference Judd and Borden1992). As the egg factor, Judd and Borden (Reference Judd and Borden1992) reported that D. antiqua eggs in contact with onion tissues facilitate oviposition by late-arriving females; facilitation effects were not observed when eggs were laid away from the onion. In the current study, D. antiqua females were allowed to lay eggs not in onion tissues, but in gravel; therefore, the egg factor may not have been an influence. Regarding the female factor, Judd and Borden (Reference Judd and Borden1992) reported that the female ovipositor can attract conspecific females to oviposit. Ovipositors of decoy flies were not exposed in the current study (experiments 2, 3, and 4); therefore, the supposed female factor may not have influenced the decoy-using experiments. Given these findings, we interpret the results in experiments 2 and 3 as follows: visual cues from decoys likely facilitated oviposition by pioneer females, and then visual cues from the decoys, and visual and olfactory cues from late-arriving females may have further facilitated oviposition.

Social effects on oviposition have also been studied in species related to D. antiqua. The eggs of D. radicum stimulated oviposition by late-arriving conspecifics by means of chemical cues (de Jong and Städler, Reference De Jong and Städler2001; Gouinguené et al., Reference Gouinguené, Poiger and Städler2006). In another anthomyiid species, ovipositing females were suggested to deposit oviposition-deterring pheromone (Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman1979, Reference Zimmerman1980, Reference Zimmerman1982). Thus, there seems to be diversity among anthomyiid flies in the social effect on oviposition.

Several limitations in the current study should be noted. First, we used green onion leaves to prepare oviposition stations. There may be some difference in the quality of leaves used for a pair of oviposition stations. Since the leaf color, shape, and chemicals serve as ovipositional cues in the onion fly (reviewed by Gouinguené and Städler, Reference Gouinguené and Städler2005; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Birch, Gregory and Murray2006), differences in these factors may have affected the oviposition. Although we minimized this possibility by using leaves of the same plant for single trials, experiments by using artificial oviposition stimulants rather than onion leaves would further confirm the social facilitation of oviposition via visual cues in D. antiqua. Second, group oviposition behavior often occurs in mass-rearing cages of D. antiqua (Supplementary fig. 1; personal observation by the authors). This may have influenced the social facilitation of oviposition observed in the current study. Third, D. antiqua females may choose an oviposition site by social facilitation cues, or lay more eggs at a site with social facilitation cues than at that without them. Last, it is unknown why the degree of social facilitation of oviposition is weak in D. antiqua (the current study; Judd and Borden, Reference Judd and Borden1992). It is possible that oviposition by each female fly is weakly facilitated by social cues or that oviposition by some females in the test cage is moderately/strongly facilitated. To examine these hypotheses, oviposition behavior in response to conspecific females and eggs should be directly observed for D. antiqua.

In conclusion, the current study revealed that the oviposition in D. antiqua is socially facilitated by visual cues, and that the facilitation continues over time. The social facilitation of oviposition in D. antiqua may play a role in improving the development of larvae in onion bulbs. Ovipositing D. antiqua females prefer damaged plants (Ikeshoji et al., Reference Ikeshoji, Ishikawa and Matsumoto1980), and this preference is consistent with newly hatched D. antiqua larvae being able to better colonize onion bulbs damaged by conspecific larvae than healthy ones (Hausmann and Miller, Reference Hausmann and Miller1989). Similarly, the aggregation of D. antiqua eggs due to social facilitation of oviposition may be adaptive for late-arriving females because their newly hatched larvae can more easily colonize onion bulbs that are pre-conditioned by larvae from pioneer females. Furthermore, larvae of D. antiqua usually aggregate on the larval diet in laboratory conditions (personal observation by Y.I. and S.H.), and larval aggregation may be promoted by the social facilitation of oviposition. It is possible that the success of larval colonization of onion bulbs increases with aggregation. These hypotheses follow the ‘mother knows best’ principle of the preference–performance relationships in host selection in herbivorous insects (Valladares and Lawton, Reference Valladares and Lawton1991; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Birch, Gregory and Murray2006; Garcia-Roberdo and Horvitz, Reference Garcia-Roberdo and Horvitz2012).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007485320000152.

Acknowledgements

We thank Drs Keisuke Nagamine and Ayumi Kudo for their helpful discussions during the course of this study.

Author contributions

S. H. and Y. I. conceived the study. S. H. and N. K. designed the experiments. N. K. conducted the preliminary work. T. T. collected data. S. H. analyzed the data with the aid of N. K. and T. T. N. K., T. T., and S. H. wrote the first draft, and Y. I. edited it. Y. I. prepared the reagents and equipment.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally to this study.

References

Browne, LB, Bartell, RJ and Shorey, HH (1969) Pheromone-mediated behaviour leading to group oviposition in the blowfly Lucilia cuprina. Journal of Insect Physiology 15, 10031014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers, CJ and Eason, PK (2009) Conspecifics and their posture influence site choice and oviposition in the damselfly Argia moesta. Ethology 115, 721730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danchin, É, Giraldeau, L-A, Valone, TJ and Wagner, RH (2004) Public information: from noisy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305, 487491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degen, T and Städler, E (1996) Influence of natural leaf shapes on oviposition in three phytophagous flies: a comparative study. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 80, 97100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Jong, R and Städler, E (2001) Complex host marking in the cabbage root fly. Chemoecology 11, 8588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia-Roberdo, C and Horvitz, CC (2012) Parent-offspring conflicts, ‘optimal bad motherhood’ and the ‘mother knows best’ principles in insect herbivores colonizing novel host plants. Ecology and Evolution 2, 14461457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouinguené, SPD and Städler, E (2005) Comparison of the sensitivity of four Delia species to host and non-host plant compounds. Physiological Entomology 30, 6274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouinguené, SP, Buser, H-R and Städler, E (2005) Host-plant leaf surface compounds influencing oviposition in Delia antiqua. Chemoecology 15, 243249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouinguené, SPD, Poiger, T and Städler, E (2006) Eggs of cabbage root fly stimulate conspecific oviposition: evaluation of the activity and determination of an egg-associated compound. Chemoecology 16, 107113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, MO and Miller, JR (1983) Color stimuli and oviposition behavior of the onion fly, Delia antiqua (Meigen) (Diptera: Anthomyiidae). Annals of Entomological Society of America 76, 766771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, MO and Miller, JR (1991) Quantitative analysis of ovipositional behaviour – effects of a host plant chemical on the onion fly (Diptera, Anthomyiidae). Journal of Insect Behavior 4, 773792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausmann, SM and Miller, JR (1989) Ovipositional preference and larval survival of the onion maggot (Diptera: Anthomyiidae) as influenced by previous maggot feeding. Journal of Economic Entomology 82, 426429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Havukkala, IJ and Miller, JR (1987) Daily periodicity in the ovipositional behavior of the onion fly, Delia antiqua (Diptera, Anthomyiidae). Environmental Entomology 16, 4144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ikeshoji, T, Ishikawa, Y and Matsumoto, Y (1980) Attractants against the onion maggots and flies, Hylemya antiqua, in onions inoculated with bacteria. Journal of Pesticide Science 5, 343350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishikawa, Y (1979) Oviposition-stimulating factors for the onion maggot Hylemya antiqua MEIGEN (Master thesis). Faculty of Agriculture, University of Tokyo (in Japanese).Google Scholar
Ishikawa, Y, Ikeshoji, T and Matsumoto, Y (1978) A propylthio moiety essential to the oviposition attractant and stimulant of the onion fly, Hylemya antiqua MEIGEN. Applied Entomology and Zoology 13, 115122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishikawa, Y, Mochizuki, A, Ikeshoji, T and Matsumoto, Y (1983) Mass-rearing of the onion and seed-corn flies, Hylemya antiqua and H. platura (Diptera: Anthomyiidae), on an artificial diet with antibiotics. Applied Entomology and Zoology 18, 6269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishikawa, Y, Tanaka, S and Matsumoto, Y (1985) Color preference of the onion fly, Hylemya antiqua MEIGEN (Diptera: Anthomyiidae) with reference to ultraviolet reflection. Applied Entomology and Zoology 20, 2026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishikawa, Y, Tsukada, S and Matsumoto, Y (1987) Effect of temperature and photoperiod on the larval development and diapause induction in the onion fly, Hylemya antiqua MEIGEN (Diptera: Anthomyiidae). Applied Entomology and Zoology 22, 610617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, SN, Birch, ANE, Gregory, PJ and Murray, PJ (2006) The ‘mother knows best’ principle: should soil insects be included in the preference-performance debate? Ecological Entomology 31, 395401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Judd, GJR and Borden, JH (1992) Aggregated oviposition in Delia antiqua (MEIGEN): a case for mediation by semiochemicals. Journal of Chemical Ecology 18, 621635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kayukawa, T, Chen, B, Hoshizaki, S and Ishikawa, Y (2007) Upregulation of a desaturase is associated with the enhancement of cold hardiness in the onion maggot, Delia antiqua. Insect Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 37, 11601167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lam, K, Babor, D, Duthie, B, Babor, E-M, Moore, M and Gries, G (2007) Proliferating bacterial symbionts on house fly eggs affect oviposition behaviour of adult flies. Animal Behaviour 74, 8192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, G and Ishikawa, Y (2005) Oviposition deterrents from the egg masses of the adzuki bean borer, Ostrinia scapulalis and Asian corn borer, O. furnacalis. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 115, 401407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, Y and Thorsteinson, (1968) Effect of organic sulfur compounds on oviposition in onion maggot, Hylemya antiqua Meigen (Diptera: Anthomyiidae). Applied Entomology and Zoology 3, 512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ning, SY, Yang, HY, Fan, DS and Feng, JN (2017 a) Influence of larval experience on preference of a subterranean insect Delia antiqua on Allium hosts. Journal of Applied Entomology 142, 263271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ning, SY, Wei, JF and Feng, JN (2017 b) Predicting the current potential and future worldwide distribution of the onion maggot, Delia antiqua using maximum entropy ecological niche modelling. PLoS ONE 12, e0171190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nomura, M and Ishikawa, Y (2001) Dynamic changes in cold hardiness, high-temperature tolerance and trehalose content in the onion maggot, Delia antiqua (Diptera: Anthomyiidae), associated with the summer and winter diapause. Applied Entomology and Zoology 36, 443449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otake, R and Dobata, S (2018) Copy if dissatisfied, innovative if not: contrasting egg-laying decision making in an insect. Animal Cognition 21, 805812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pasqualone, AA and Davis, JM (2011) The use of conspecific phenotypic states as information during reproductive decisions. Animal Behaviour 82, 281284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prokopy, RJ and Duan, JJ (1998) Socially facilitated egglaying behavior in Mediterranean fruit flies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 42, 117122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prokopy, RJ and Roitberg, BD (2001) Joining and avoidance behavior in nonsocial insects. Annual Review of Entomology 46, 631665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2016) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Renwick, JAA (1989) Chemical ecology of oviposition in phytophagous insects. Experientia 45, 223228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roitberg, BD and Prokopy, RJ (1987) Insects that mark host plants. BioScience 37, 400406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romeis, J, Ebbinghaus, D and Scherkenbeck, J (2003) Factors accounting for the variability in the behavioral response to the onion fly (Delia antiqua) to n-dipropyl disulfide. Journal of Chemical Ecology 29, 21312142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, JL and Miller, JR (2002) Lifetime ovipositional patterns of mated and virgin onion flies, Delia antiqua (Diptera: Anthomyiidae). Journal of Insect Physiology 48, 171180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, JG, Stenberg, JA and Bjӧrkman, C (2015) How far away is the next basket of eggs? Spatial memory and perceived cues shape aggregation patterns in a leaf beetle. Ecology 96, 908914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanaka, S and Sugahara, R (2017) Desert locusts Schistocerca gregaria (Acrididae: Orthoptera) do not lay eggs in old sand: why? Applied Entomology and Zoology 52, 635642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valladares, G and Lawton, JH (1991) Host-plant selection in the holly leaf-miner: does mother know best? Journal of Animal Ecology 60, 227240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vernon, RS (1979) Visual and olfactory aspects of food and oviposition host selection in Hylemya antiqua (Meigen) (Diptera: Anthomyiidae) (PhD thesis). Simon Fraser University.Google Scholar
Visser, JH (1986) Host odor perception in phytophagous insects. Annual Review of Entomology 31, 121144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, RH and Danchin, É (2010) A taxonomy of biological information. Oikos 119, 203209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasserberg, G, Bailes, N, Davis, C and Yeoman, K (2014) Hump-shaped density-dependent regulation of mosquito oviposition site-selection by conspecific immature stages: theory, field test with Aedes albopictus, and a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 9, e92658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, M (1979) Oviposition behavior and the existence of an oviposition-deterring pheromone in Hylemya (Diptera, Anthomyiidae). Environmental Entomology 8, 277279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, M (1980) Selective deposition of an oviposition-deterring pheromone by Hylemya (Diptera, Anthomyiidae). Environmental Entomology 9, 321324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, M (1982) Facultative deposition of an oviposition-deterring pheromone by Hylemya (Diptera, Anthomyiidae). Environmental Entomology 11, 519522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Distribution of eggs over two oviposition stations of equal quality (experiment 1). The egg ratio indicates the number of eggs on the side with more eggs divided by the total number of eggs. Circles and crosses represent trials rejecting (binomial test, P < 0.01) and supporting, respectively, the null hypothesis of the even distribution of eggs. The box plot indicates the median, and the first and third quartiles.

Figure 1

Table 1. Experiment 1: the egg distribution between two oviposition stations of equal quality in four cages

Figure 2

Figure 2. The GLMM analysis for experiments 2 (left) and 4 (right). Pairs of circles connected by lines represent single trials of the experiment. The thick line indicates the model prediction.

Figure 3

Table 2. Experiments 2 and 4: effects of decoys on oviposition facilitation

Figure 4

Figure 3. The GLMM analysis for experiment 3. Pairs of circles connected by black lines represent single trials of the experiment. The thick line indicates the model prediction.

Figure 5

Table 3. Experiment 3: oviposition facilitation for varying durations of oviposition when decoys were placed on their legs

Figure 6

Figure 4. Total egg number at different times (experiment 3). One circle represents one trial. For both panels, the box plot indicates the median, and first and third quartiles.

Supplementary material: File

Hoshizaki et al. supplementary material

Hoshizaki et al. supplementary material

Download Hoshizaki et al. supplementary material(File)
File 352.9 KB