Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T11:00:49.729Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why Politicians Won't Apologize: Communication Effects in the Aftermath of Sex Scandals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 November 2024

Bence Hamrak
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Central European University, Vienna, Austria
Gabor Simonovits*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Central European University, Vienna, Austria Rajk College for Advanced Studies, Budapest, Hungary Centre for Social Sciences, Institute for Political Science, Budapest, Hungary
Alex Rusnak
Affiliation:
Political Science Department, Comenius University, Bratislava, Slovakia
Ferenc Szucs
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
*
Corresponding author: Gabor Simonovits; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A startling feature of the countless recent sex scandals involving politicians has been the almost complete lack of public apologies. This note explores the electoral incentives politicians face when crafting communication strategies in the aftermath of sex scandals. We focus on two communication strategies – denials and apologies – and assess their impact on incumbent support across a wide range of scandals that vary in terms of the seriousness of the charges as well as the availability of evidence. Using data from a series of survey experiments, including over 10,000 respondents we find that citizens punish incumbents who apologize, even in the case of accusations that appear the least serious in the eyes of voters. Moreover, apologies fail to generate political support compared to denials, even in cases when voters are exposed to evidence. This suggests that in most cases apologies are simply not politically viable communication strategies.

Type
Letter
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In the last few years, high-profile politicians have been accused of sexual misconduct of varying levels of transgressions, with different political ramifications. Politicians overwhelmingly deny charges, even in the face of seemingly strong evidence. Most notably, former presidents Donald Trump and Bill Clinton were able to do so without losing political support, as shown in our analysis in Supplementary Material S5.

By contrast, the few incumbents – such as Anthony Weiner and Mark Sanford – who apologized for their behaviour appeared to pay significant political costs. Such patterns present a normative problem beyond the transgressions themselves. First, the absence of public apologies is problematic because they prevent reconciliation and forgiveness (Fehr, Gelfand, and Nag Reference Fehr, Gelfand and Nag2010) that could be facilitated through the perpetrator's remorse (Hornsey et al. Reference Hornsey2019). Moreover, denials fuel uncertainty (Schumann and Dragotta Reference Schumann and Dragotta2020), provoke divisive responses reflecting pre-existing dispositions (Fischle Reference Fischle2000), and therefore, increase polarization.

The goal of this note is to explore the possible role of voter preferences in this lack of such apologies and to consider the empirical attributes of public scandals that limit or enhance electoral incentives for honest communication. To measure the public incentives for apologies, our study compares their impact to denials.Footnote 1 These strategies have alternative rationales when managing the impressions of voters and, therefore, offer exclusive choices for the politician in their response strategy. On the one hand, denial creates doubt about the veracity of the accusations at the cost of creating the impression of hypocrisy (Scott and Lyman Reference Scott and Lyman1968). On the other hand, apologies maintain the honest image of the accused at the cost of revealing the guilt. We ask under what circumstances voters prefer apologies compared to denials when it comes to the incumbent's support in sex scandals.

To assess the causal effect of elite communication on voters' evaluation, we rely on a series of survey experiments in which we exposed over 10,000 American adults to fictional sex scandals.Footnote 2 The experiments varied (1) the severity of the accusations, going from a consensual extramarital affair to rape, (2) the partisanship of the incumbent (co-/out-partisan, or non-partisan), (3) the communication used by the politician (denial or apology), and (4) the availability of evidence.

Across our studies, voters punished incumbents for their apologies even in the case of scandals that they did not perceive as particularly serious. In addition, denials increased support for politicians to a greater degree in the case of more severe accusations. This suggests that voters place much greater emphasis on innocence rather than honesty. Furthermore, we demonstrate that these communication effects persist, even in circumstances we thought would favour honest communication, when the politician is a co-partisan and when the evidence is revealed to voters. First, while accused politicians garner more support from their co-partisans on the baseline, elite responses impact public opinion similarly across partisan aisles; therefore, apologies fail to sway even the likely voters of accused incumbents. Second, apologies proved ineffective even when we provided respondents with additional evidence corroborating the accusation. Compared to voters who only received the evidence, the additional, ex-ante, or ex-post apology did not increase voter support.

Our study demonstrates that, while political pundits often lament the lack of honest apologies, especially in the context of sex scandals, it seems that public opinion simply doesn't offer sufficient incentives for politicians to apologize instead of denying in any of the circumstances we study – even considering the possibility that evidence can refute these denials, and therefore, the politician is caught lying. Based on our findings, we conclude that politician apologies are unlikely because (1) scandals that become public are perceived as serious, (2) conclusive evidence for the allegations are rare, and (3) even in the availability of evidence, voters would not punish dishonesty.

Political Scandals

Political scandals are mediated events that feature transgressions of universally accepted norms by political actors leading to public outrage (Esser and Hartung Reference Esser and Hartung2004; Thompson Reference Thompson2000). The study of political scandals is inherently multidisciplinary. Sociologists and anthropologists have focused on scandals' cultural embeddedness, political scientists and psychologists have explored their political impact, and communication scholars have examined the role of mass media. After a short review of the relevant literature, we introduce our theoretical account of how politicians’ responses influence voters' reactions to scandals.Footnote 3

A large body of the political science literature has established the damaging impact of scandals on election outcomes and the favourability of the politicians involved (Banducci and Karp Reference Banducci and Karp1994; Fernandez-Vazquez, Barbera, and Rivero Reference Fernandez-Vazquez, Barbera and Rivero2016; Hirano and Snyder Reference Hirano and Snyder2012; Welch and Hibbing Reference Welch and Hibbing1997; Winters and Weitz-Shapiro Reference Winters and Weitz-Shapiro2016). However, studies also found a great deal of heterogeneity in voters' responses depending on the type (Carlson, Ganiel, and Hyde Reference Carlson, Ganiel and Hyde2000; Doherty, Dowling, and Miller Reference Doherty, Dowling and Miller2011); the context of the scandal (Carlin, Love, and Martinez-Gallardo Reference Carlin, Love and Martinez-Gallardo2015); the traits of the accused politicians, like gender or ethnicity (Berinsky et al. Reference Berinsky2011; Bhatti, Hansen, and Leth Olsen Reference Bhatti, Hansen and Leth Olsen2013; Smith, Powers, and Suarez Reference Smith, Powers and Suarez2005); the predispositions of the voters such as partisanship (Costa et al. Reference Costa2020; Klar and McCoy Reference Klar and McCoy2021; Sikorski, Heiss, and Matthes Reference Sikorski, Heiss and Matthes2020); or sexism (Masuoka, Grose, and Junn Reference Masuoka, Grose and Junn2021).

A necessary condition for a transgression to become a scandal is communication or mediation (Esser and Hartung Reference Esser and Hartung2004; Thompson Reference Thompson2000). Past research emphasized the central role of the mass media in covering scandals (Costas-Perez, Sole-Olle, and Sorribas-Navarro Reference Costas-Perez, Sole-Olle and Sorribas-Navarro2012; Nyhan Reference Nyhan2015; Sikorski Reference Sikorski2017), but much less is known about how politician responses influence voters' opinion about scandals (Sikorski Reference Sikorski2018, 3,120).

Motivated by theories on political image restoration and blame management (Benoit Reference Benoit2014), we argue for the central role played by the politicians' communication on voters' reactions to scandals. We know that politicians can avoid blame by using strategic communication (Grose, Malhotra, and Van Houweling Reference Grose, Malhotra and Van Houweling2015; McGraw Reference McGraw1991). This might be especially true for scandals regarding sexual misconduct, in which case competing frames (Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007) might allow for more strategic discretion on the side of the politician.

Apologies and Denials

While some experimental studies have explored the impact of political communication in the aftermath of sex scandals, their results have been mixed (Sigal et al. Reference Sigal1988; Smith, Powers, and Suarez Reference Smith, Powers and Suarez2005). Specifically, the relative impact of two key, but fundamentally different communications – public apologies and denials – is unclear, possibly because researchers have tested these responses across different types of sex scandals that may moderate the communications' relative effects (Schumann and Dragotta Reference Schumann and Dragotta2020). In the following, we develop a theory about the conditions under which denials or apologies might be more effective in avoiding negative electoral consequences.

Building on existing work, we propose that two factors drive voter preferences during a scandal. First, voters consider the available evidence and elite messages to gauge whether the accused politician is guilty of the charges. Second, voters also consider the degree to which the politician's communication regarding the scandal is honest. While the question of why voters care about guilt is trivial, honesty might require more explanation. We argue that besides its moral value – facilitating reconciliation and repair (Fehr, Gelfand, and Nag Reference Fehr, Gelfand and Nag2010; Hornsey et al. Reference Hornsey2019; Schumann Reference Schumann2014; Schumann and Dragotta Reference Schumann and Dragotta2020) – honesty also matters for voters because it signals the politician's performance in representing them in different domains concerning other democratic outcomes.

Voters' rationale presents a dilemma for the politician: denying can elicit doubts about the accusation, but also create an impression of hypocrisy (Scott and Lyman Reference Scott and Lyman1968). Conversely, apologies maximize the perceptions of honesty and necessarily reveal guilt as well. We argue that the choice will depend on the severity of the accusation and the likelihood of evidence. First, the more severe the charges are, the more importance voters might attach to innocence compared to honesty, leading incumbents to deny the charges. Second, when the politician anticipates that the wrongdoing will be likely proven, we expect them to apologize, given the chances of getting caught and appearing both guilty and dishonest.

Taken together, we view these factors – scandal severity and the difficulty of collecting evidence – as crucial, though not comprehensive dimensions of sex scandal communication. Other, individual-level factors such as voters' partisan predispositions could also influence the relative effect of apologies and denials: co-partisans could perceive transgressions as less serious, the accusations or evidence less credible, and the politicians' denial more honest, in line with motivated reasoning (Leeper and Slothuus Reference Leeper and Slothuus2014). This could further complicate the optimal response strategy in different scandal conditions. Mapping the impact of two communication strategies across these dimensions will thus provide rich evidence regarding the type of scandals where apologies can be expected in the real world. In what follows, we explain how we operationalized these dimensions and how we tested the impact of elite communication.

Experimental Design

We designed a series of three experiments to study the effect of denials and apologies in the aftermath of political scandals. The surveys were fielded through Lucid (Coppock and McClellan Reference Coppock and McClellan2019), a marketplace for online survey panels.Footnote 4 In each experiment, subjects were asked to read a vignette about a fictional sex scandal featuring a sitting member of the US Congress, John Miller, in the form of a news story. They were then randomly assigned to read one of two communication strategies: either an apology in which Miller admitted the accusations and apologized for his behaviour or a denial where Miller denied all the accusations and called the news story ridiculous.

Finally, at various points in the experiment, they were asked their opinion about (1) the seriousness of the accusation, (2) their belief in the allegations, and (3) whether they thought Miller should resign.Footnote 5 Across the three experiments, however, we executed some modifications in the design to address some conditions under which the communication effects are observed. We offer more details about the experimental design and procedure in Supplementary Material S2–S4.

Experiment 1

The goal of the first experiment (n = 7,880) was to compare the impact of these communication strategies across a broad range of sex scandals with varying severity.Footnote 6 In the ‘least serious’ condition, Miller was accused of having a long extramarital affair, while in the most serious one, Miller was accused of rape. The in-between conditions included harassment, sexting, and sexual assault, resulting in five ‘seriousness’ conditions.Footnote 7 We also randomly assign subjects to one of the two response strategies by Miller (apology or denial).

Finally, we randomized Miller's partisanship to either match respondents' party affiliation (measured pre-treatment), describe him as an out-partisan, or leave his party unspecified. On the one hand, this makes our vignettes more realistic and provides a more conservative test of the effect of communications, as partisans could simply disregard the politicians' communication and express preferences in line with partisanship (Broockman and Butler Reference Broockman and Butler2017). On the other hand, this feature of the design also allows us to test if the possible effect of communication varies across partisan sub-groups.

Experiment 2

The second experiment (n = 998) served as a replication of our first one with two important modifications. First, we utilized a repeated measures design in which the respondents' support for Miller, as well as their belief in the allegation, was elicited both before and after the incumbent's response. We use this first measure as a control group so that we can compare subjects' attitudes after each of the two kinds of responses to a baseline, which allows us to observe their absolute effects on resignation support. Second, in this experiment, we only included a subset of the scandal types.Footnote 8 This decision was driven by a consideration of statistical power due to the lower sample size.

Experiment 3

In the third experiment (n = 1,244), we provided respondents with new information in addition to the scandal allegation and Miller's communication – a piece of evidence supporting the charges. The purpose of this modification was to tease out the effect of apologies and denials in a setting where voters are quite sure that the accusations are true, and thus should not update their beliefs about the incumbent's guiltiness based on the apology or denial. Subjects were randomly assigned to receive evidence of the accusation,Footnote 9 elicited either before or after Miller's apology or denial.

In this experiment, we did not vary the scandal's seriousness.Footnote 10 Moreover, we used a repeated measures design and asked each dependent variable after each of the three information stages: one after the accusation, one after the communication (or evidence), and one after the evidence (or communication). This yields comparisons of the measures under an allegation, evidence, communications, and evidence and communications (in a permuted order) conditions.

Results

The Relative Impact of Apology vs. Denial

Figure 1 presents our findings from the first experiment. As for the manipulation checks, the left panel shows that we successfully induced variation in the perceived seriousness of the scandal (55 to 75, on a 100-point scale). Second, we show that compared to their prior beliefs – elicited before the politician's response – subjects' posterior beliefs vary according to the response types.Footnote 11 Across each of the five scenarios, denials reduced while an apology increased voters' beliefs that the charges were true, though compared to denial, apologies' absolute effect on beliefs was much larger. This suggests that apologies are much more indicative of guilt, while denials are less effective in building uncertainty about the accusations – especially in the case of the most serious accusations.

Figure 1. Scandal type, incumbent response and citizen attitudes (Study 1).

Note: The left panel shows the change in the mean perceived seriousness of the allegations by scandal type, obtained from the non-partisan group responses (measured prior to the politician response). The middle panel shows respondents' mean reported belief that the allegations are true, by scandal type. Prior beliefs were asked before the politician's communication. Apology and denial beliefs were asked after the communication. The right panel shows the mean resignation support for each scandal type and by response strategies. Error bars denote 95 per cent CIs. The regression results are in Supplementary Material S6.

We present our main finding in the right panel of Fig. 1, which plots average support for the accused incumbent as a function of scandal severity and the type of response (apology vs. denial). We observe a clear trend: the relative effectiveness of denial increases dramatically for allegations seen as more serious. For instance, the relative effect of denial is −5.5 points in the case of an extra-marital affair but −18.6 points in the case of rape. This pattern suggests that the relative weight that voters place on honesty and innocence varies as a function of the seriousness of the scandal. The cost of lying is comparable to the cost of the wrongdoing in the case of ‘light’ scandals; however, the returns of the strategies diverge dramatically in more serious scandals. At the same time, even for the scandals that are seen as less serious by our respondents, apologies fare worse. Therefore, honesty is not valued higher than innocence by voters. This suggests that politicians are always better off denying it, independent of the seriousness of the accusations.

The Absolute Impact of Incumbent Communication

While our findings are quite clear regarding the relative effectiveness of the two communication strategies, the question remains whether apologies are just less effective modes of incumbent communication, or whether they hurt politicians in the eyes of voters. To explore this question, we conducted a replication experiment in which we measured the outcomes (that is belief in the allegation and incumbent support) for a subset of allegation conditions both before and after the response. The pre-communication measures serve as a baseline to which we can compare post-communication levels of disapproval. The second experiment's findings demonstrate that denials are steadily effective compared to the pre-communication baseline, while apologies are not rewarded in low-seriousness conditions; furthermore, they hurt incumbents as the severity of the scandal increases (see Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Scandal type, incumbent response and citizen attitudes (Study 2).

Note: The zero line depicts the mean resignation support for subjects who only received the allegation, but no politician response. The left panel shows the coefficients for the mean reported belief in the allegation by response types and by scandal type compared to the reference group. The right panel shows the coefficient for the mean resignation support by response types and scandal types compared to the reference group. Ninety-five per cent of CIs are adjusted for the repeated measures by respondent IDs. The regression results are in Supplementary Material S9.

The Role of Further Evidence

The other limitation of our first experiment is that it could not parse out two potentially counter-veiling effects of apologies: revealing guilt and demonstrating remorse and honesty. By incorporating a phase in our design in which subjects receive additional evidence supporting the accusations, we can test the impact of communication in a setting where voters have a high degree of confidence that the accusations are indeed true, or, in other words, we can adjust for the ‘guilt revealing effect’ of apologies. As a result, we can see the potential electoral reward for honesty without it being masked by the effect of guilt. In addition, we can also consider the possibility that voters might punish a denial if lying is revealed, and this could change the relative electoral effectiveness of apologies and denials in the aftermath of scandals and, therefore, politicians’ behaviour.

We first estimate the impact of apologies compared to a baseline where the subjects are exposed to additional evidence confirming the charges, but no communication (observed from the respondents who received the evidence before the incumbent message). As Fig. 3 shows (right panel), an apology has a modest and statistically insignificant mitigating effect. Voters do not seem to appreciate apologies either before or after evidence is revealed. These results are surprising as the information environment making the accusations likely creates the most favourable conditions for apologies, according to our account.

Figure 3. Incumbent response, evidence and citizen attitudes (Study 3).

Note: The zero line depicts resignation support for subjects who only received evidence of the accusation, but no communication. Left panel: Mean reported belief in the allegation by treatment groups. Right panel: Mean resignation support by treatment groups. Ninety-five per cent of CIs are adjusted for the repeated measures by respondent IDs. The regression results are in Supplementary Material S10.

Our last analyses assess the impact of denial in a context where subjects read the evidence after a denial and the politician is caught lying. Voters do not seem to punish this when compared to a simple revelation of guilt. As Fig. 3 shows (right panel), voters who received a denial and then the evidence, only support the resignation of the politician approx. 1 percentage point more than voters who only received the evidence. Furthermore, the left panel of Fig. 3 shows that this is not the case because, even with evidence arising later, denial still keeps the mean reported belief in the allegation lower compared to the evidence-only group. The difference is effectively zero between these groups' reported outcomes.

Even more devastating are the effects we observed for the group for whom the denial followed the evidence. Denial can reduce resignation support by approx. 4 percentage points compared to evidence only, and this comes from the denials' ability to fuel uncertainty (the mean reported belief in the allegation is 7 percentage points lower for this group than the evidence-only group). Altogether, apologies fail to garner support compared to denials even in the ‘ideal case’ – when further evidence is revealed.

One explanation for this result is that the additional evidence failed to fully convince respondents that the incumbent was guilty.Footnote 12 Beyond a seeming limitation of our experimental design, these patterns may also reflect a key feature of mediatized scandals where the media or even authorities are seen as controversial and political. In such cases, denials can capitalize on the lingering doubts about the evidence. In any case, even if evidence was uncontroversial, voters might still not reward apologies because of what they signal about the politician's character, as we attempt to explain in more detail in the conclusion.

The Role of Partisanship

Finally, how do partisan contexts change these findings? Prior to communication, co-partisan respondents perceived the allegations as less serious and less credible than out-partisan respondents. Consequently, co-partisan politicians, on average, were less likely to be sanctioned for the scandals than out-partisan politicians. Notwithstanding these absolute differences, the relative impact of denial compared to apology was not significantly different for co-partisans compared to out-partisans. On the one hand, denials resulted in lower post-treatment beliefs in the accusations, and with similar magnitude across the partisan contexts.Footnote 13 On the other hand, it appears that co-partisans did not reward apologies vis-a-vis denials more compared to out-partisans, not even for less serious transgressions (see Supplementary Material S7).

Conclusion

Our results on voters' reactions to scandals and communications demonstrate how politicians involved in sex scandals can garner support by denying charges rather than apologizing. While both victims and voters at large would benefit from apologies, public opinion seems to generate incentives for denials instead, even in the case of wrongdoings the public itself does not view too serious, minimizing the problem of guilt, and even in an ideal world where guilty politicians denying charges would be caught lying. All in all, our study paints a dark picture of accountability in the era of the #MeToo movement: even increasing media scrutiny and public outrage fail to force incumbents to take responsibility for their actions.

There are two limitations of our study that future research should address. First, while our experimental approach addresses challenges that would make it difficult to study the same question relying on observational data (see Supplementary Material S5), studying voters' reactions in hypothetical situations could deviate from real-life effects because partisan-motivated reasoning is expected to be stronger towards well-known politicians or in cases with actual stakes.

Second, while our study offers some hints as to why voters do not appreciate apologies, it does not produce direct evidence on the matter. Perhaps, the politician's response strategy also provides an indirect signal about their leadership style. ‘Masculine defaults’ in politics (Lombard, Azpeitia, and Cheryan Reference Lombard, Azpeitia and Cheryan2021) and misogyny in political communication (Boatright and Sperling Reference Boatright and Sperling2019) might provide an advantage for male politicians' denials over apologies in sex scandals, as the earlier meets gender-normative expectations more about strength, boldness, and aggression, while apologies could be connected to notions of weakness.Footnote 14 On the other side of the debate, voters might think apologies are fabricated (Lepore Reference Lepore2022; Schumann and Dragotta Reference Schumann and Dragotta2020) and only used pragmatically for political survival.

Given these limitations, future research could combine the approach presented here with both descriptive and causal studies of elite communication in the aftermath of real scandals using observational data. Moreover, future research should probe the impact of incumbent responses across a broader set of communication strategies, in other types of scandals such as corruption cases and test theories on how apologies influence voters' evaluation of the candidate in other important aspects.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123424000292.

Data availability statement

Replication Data for this article can be found in Harvard Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/M5RJ0T.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Luca Varadi, Levente Littvay, and multiple anonymous reviewers for the helpful feedback on the earlier versions of the article.

Financial support

None.

Competing Interest

None.

Ethical standards

The research was approved by the Ethical Research Committee of the Central European University.

Footnotes

1 While we acknowledge that other strategies – such as justification (Benoit Reference Benoit2014) – are used as well, these are less germane to the context that we study and they share the underlying rationales with denials.

2 Given that we told respondents that they were reading about a fictional scandal, the project did not deceive respondents. Moreover, at the beginning of the study subjects were informed about the goal of the study and were told that they were free to withdraw from the study anytime they wanted to. All in all, we think that the experiment presented little impact and no foreseeable harm to subjects.

3 For a more thorough synthesis of the existing literature see a meta-analysis (Sikorski Reference Sikorski2018).

4 The summary statistics for each study can be found in Supplementary Material S1.

5 All outcomes were measured on a 0–100 scale.

6 The first experiment was pre-registered.

7 For the exact wording of the communications, news story items, and outcomes see Supplementary Material S4.

8 We selected affair and assault as a low and a high severity scandal condition. The two conditions were substantially different in the subjects' perception of seriousness based on the results from Study 1.

9 See Supplementary Material S4: Evidence.

10 We selected the sexting vignette from Study 1 – a condition with ‘moderate’ perceived seriousness.

11 Note that we asked respondents' beliefs randomly before and after the response so that we have pre-communication and post-communication values as well for beliefs in the allegation.

12 The mean level of belief in the allegation for the group who received evidence only as treatment was 80.5 per cent.

13 An exception to this was the ‘sexting’ scandal condition, where out-partisan denials failed to generate doubts in the accusation.

14 We thank an anonymous reviewer for raising that point.

References

Banducci, SA and Karp, JA (1994) Electoral consequences of scandal and reapportionment in the 1992 house elections. American Politics Quarterly 22(1), 326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benoit, WL (2014) Accounts, Excuses, and Apologies, Second Edition: Image Repair Theory and Research. New York: SUNY Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, AJ et al. (2011) Sex and race: Are black candidates more likely to be disadvantaged by sex scandals? Political Behavior 33(2), 179202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhatti, Y, Hansen, KM and Leth Olsen, A (2013) Political hypocrisy: The effect of political scandals on candidate evaluations. Acta Politica 48(4), 408428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boatright, RG and Sperling, V (2019) Trumping Politics as Usual: Masculinity, Misogyny, and the 2016 Elections. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broockman, DE and Butler, DM (2017) The causal effects of elite position-taking on voter attitudes: Field experiments with elite communication. American Journal of Political Science 61(1), 208221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlin, RE, Love, GJ and Martinez-Gallardo, C (2015) Cushioning the fall: Scandals, economic conditions, and executive approval. Political Behavior 37(1), 109130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, J, Ganiel, G and Hyde, MS (2000) Scandal and political candidate image. Southeastern Political Review 28(4), 747757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, D and Druckman, JN (2007) Framing public opinion in competitive democracies. American Political Science Review 101(4), 637655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, A and McClellan, OA (2019) Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents. Research & Politics 6(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costas-Perez, E, Sole-Olle, A and Sorribas-Navarro, P (2012) Corruption scandals, voter information, and accountability. European Journal of Political Economy 28(4), 469484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, M et al. (2020) How partisanship and sexism influence voters’ reactions to political #MeToo scandals. Research & Politics 7(3).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, D, Dowling, CM and Miller, MG (2011) Are financial or moral scandals worse? It Depends. PS: Political Science & Politics 44(4), 749757.Google Scholar
Esser, F and Hartung, U (2004) Nazis, pollution, and no sex: political scandals as a reflection of political culture in Germany. American Behavioral Scientist 47(8), 1040-1071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, R, Gelfand, MJ and Nag, M (2010) The road to forgiveness: A meta-analytic synthesis of its situational and dispositional correlates. Psychological Bulletin 136(5), 894914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Vazquez, P, Barbera, P and Rivero, G (2016) Rooting out corruption or rooting for corruption? The Heterogeneous Electoral Consequences of Scandals. Political Science Research and Methods 4(2), 379397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischle, M (2000) Mass response to the Lewinsky Scandal: Motivated reasoning or Bayesian updating? Political Psychology 21(1), 135159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grose, C, Malhotra, N and Van Houweling, R (2015) Explaining explanations: How legislators explain their policy positions and how citizens react. American Journal of Political Science 59(3), 724743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamrak, B et al. (2024) Replication Data: Why Politicians Won't Apologize: Communication Effects in the Aftermath of Sex Scandals. Harvard Dataverse V1. Available from https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/M5RJ0T. eprint: UNF:6:3toT6hyrmKdVCVWB5CIfcA==.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirano, S and Snyder, JM (2012) What happens to incumbents in scandals? Quarterly Journal of Political Science 7(4), 447456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornsey, M et al. (2019) Embodied remorse: Physical displays of remorse increase positive responses to public apologies, but have negligible effects on forgiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 119(2), 367389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klar, S and McCoy, A (2021) Partisan-motivated evaluations of sexual misconduct and the mitigating role of the #MeToo movement. American Journal of Political Science 65(4), 777789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeper, TJ and Slothuus, R (2014) Political parties, motivated reasoning, and public opinion formation. Political Psychology 35(1), 129156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lepore, J (2022) The Case Against the Twitter Apology: Our twenty-first-century culture of performed remorse has become a sorry spectacle. Available from https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2022/11/14/the-case-against-the-twitter-apology-matthewichihashi-potts-forgiveness-danya-ruttenberg-on-repentance-and-repair (accessed 20 March 2024).Google Scholar
Lombard, EJ, Azpeitia, J and Cheryan, S (2021) Built on uneven ground: How masculine defaults disadvantage women in political leadership. Psychological Inquiry 32(2), 107116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuoka, N, Grose, C and Junn, J (2021) Sexual harassment and candidate evaluation: Gender and partisanship interact to affect voter responses to candidates accused of harassment. Political Behavior 45(1), 12851307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGraw, KM (1991) Managing blame: An experimental test of the effects of political accounts. The American Political Science Review 85(4), 11331157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyhan, B (2015) Scandal potential: how political context and news congestion affect the president's vulnerability to Media scandal. British Journal of Political Science 45(2), 435466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schumann, K (2014) An affirmed self and a better apology: The effect of self-affirmation on transgressors’ responses to victims. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 54(1), 8996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schumann, K and Dragotta, A (2020) Is moral redemption possible? The effectiveness of public apologies for sexual misconduct. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 90(1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, MB and Lyman, SM (1968) Accounts. American Sociological Review 33(1), 4662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigal, J et al. (1988) Factors affecting perceptions of political candidates accused of sexual and financial misconduct. Political Psychology 9(2), 273280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sikorski, C (2017) Politische Skandalberichterstattung: Ein Forschungsuberblick und Systematisierungsversuch. Publizistik 62(3), 299323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sikorski, C (2018) Political scandals as a democratic challenge – the aftermath of political scandals: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Communication 12(1).Google Scholar
Sikorski, C, Heiss, R and Matthes, J (2020) How political scandals affect the electorate: Tracing the eroding and spillover effects of scandals with a panel study. Political Psychology 41(3), 549568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, ES, Powers, A and Suarez, GA (2005) If Bill Clinton were a woman: The effectiveness of male and female politicians account strategies following alleged transgressions. Political Psychology 26(1), 115134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, JB (2000) Political Scandal: Power and Visability in the Media Age. Malden: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Welch, S and Hibbing, JR (1997) The effects of charges of corruption on voting behavior in Congressional elections, 1982–1990. The Journal of Politics 59(1), 226239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winters, MS and Weitz-Shapiro, R (2016) Who is in charge here? Direct and indirect accusations and voter punishment of corruption. Political Research Quarterly 69(2), 207219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Scandal type, incumbent response and citizen attitudes (Study 1).Note: The left panel shows the change in the mean perceived seriousness of the allegations by scandal type, obtained from the non-partisan group responses (measured prior to the politician response). The middle panel shows respondents' mean reported belief that the allegations are true, by scandal type. Prior beliefs were asked before the politician's communication. Apology and denial beliefs were asked after the communication. The right panel shows the mean resignation support for each scandal type and by response strategies. Error bars denote 95 per cent CIs. The regression results are in Supplementary Material S6.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Scandal type, incumbent response and citizen attitudes (Study 2).Note: The zero line depicts the mean resignation support for subjects who only received the allegation, but no politician response. The left panel shows the coefficients for the mean reported belief in the allegation by response types and by scandal type compared to the reference group. The right panel shows the coefficient for the mean resignation support by response types and scandal types compared to the reference group. Ninety-five per cent of CIs are adjusted for the repeated measures by respondent IDs. The regression results are in Supplementary Material S9.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Incumbent response, evidence and citizen attitudes (Study 3).Note: The zero line depicts resignation support for subjects who only received evidence of the accusation, but no communication. Left panel: Mean reported belief in the allegation by treatment groups. Right panel: Mean resignation support by treatment groups. Ninety-five per cent of CIs are adjusted for the repeated measures by respondent IDs. The regression results are in Supplementary Material S10.

Supplementary material: File

Hamrak et al. supplementary material

Hamrak et al. supplementary material
Download Hamrak et al. supplementary material(File)
File 3.1 MB
Supplementary material: Link

Hamrak et al. Dataset

Link