Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T00:31:58.294Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship between eating behaviours and food and drink consumption in healthy postmenopausal women in a real-life context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2008

Julie Goulet
Affiliation:
Institute of Nutraceuticals and Functional Foods, Laval University, 2440 Hochelaga Blvd, Québec G1K 7P4, Canada
Véronique Provencher
Affiliation:
Institute of Nutraceuticals and Functional Foods, Laval University, 2440 Hochelaga Blvd, Québec G1K 7P4, Canada
Marie-Ève Piché
Affiliation:
Institute of Nutraceuticals and Functional Foods, Laval University, 2440 Hochelaga Blvd, Québec G1K 7P4, Canada
Annie Lapointe
Affiliation:
Institute of Nutraceuticals and Functional Foods, Laval University, 2440 Hochelaga Blvd, Québec G1K 7P4, Canada
S. John Weisnagel
Affiliation:
Endocrinology and Diabetes Research Unit, Québec, Canada
André Nadeau
Affiliation:
Endocrinology and Diabetes Research Unit, Québec, Canada
Jean Bergeron
Affiliation:
Lipid Research Center, CHUQ, Québec, Canada
Simone Lemieux*
Affiliation:
Institute of Nutraceuticals and Functional Foods, Laval University, 2440 Hochelaga Blvd, Québec G1K 7P4, Canada
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Simone Lemieux, fax +1 418 656 5877, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Associations between eating behaviours and dietary variables have not been thoroughly investigated in healthy postmenopausal women in a real-life uncontrolled context. To investigate how eating behaviours (cognitive dietary restraint, disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger) were associated with food and drink consumption, energy density and meal pattern in 112 healthy postmenopausal women (age 56·8 (sd 4·4) years) not on hormonal therapy. Women completed a 3 d weighed food record and filled out the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire. The sample was divided according to the median of the distribution of cognitive dietary restraint and disinhibition (9 and 6 respectively). Both subgroups of women with high restraint level (presenting either high or low disinhibition) consumed a diet with a lower energy density than subgroups of women with lower restraint level. Women with high restraint–low disinhibition had a lower consumption of red meat and processed meat and a lower consumption of diet soft drinks than women with low restraint–high disinhibition. They were also characterised by a higher intake of whole grains than women with high restraint–high disinhibition and than women with lower restraint level (with either high or low disinhibition). Women with high restraint–high disinhibition levels showed differences in dietary variables when compared with subgroups of women with lower restraint level, namely for refined grains and diet soft drinks. We conclude that in healthy postmenopausal women, dietary consumption of specific food and drink may be related to particular eating behaviours. Women with high restraint and low disinhibition levels generally showed the most healthy dietary pattern.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2008

The prevalence of obesity in Western countries has steadily increased in the last decade, and has became a major public health problem(Reference Ledoux1Reference Tjepkema and Shields3). Obesity, and more specifically abdominal obesity, has been shown to increase the risk of CVD and type 2 diabetes(4, Reference Kissebah5) and in women, CVD remains the first cause of mortality after menopause(6Reference Mosca, Manson, Sutherland, Langer, Manolio and Barrett-Connor8). While dietary treatment is the basic therapeutic approach for obesity, its long-term impact on weight management is disappointing(Reference Wing and Phelan9). In fact, approximately 20–30 % of overweight or obese individuals succeed in maintaining their weight loss after 1–3 years following a hypoenergetic diet, and a lower percentage succeed after 3–5 years(Reference Wing and Phelan9). On the other hand, dieting in order to control weight is very prevalent in our modern society, especially among women. In fact, results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2001–2 underline that restraining food intake is the most prevalent weight-control practice, either in women wanting to lose weight or trying not to gain weight(Reference Weiss, Galuska, Khan and Serdula10).

The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire is one of the most widely used scales in behavioural research, developed in order to measure cognitive dietary restraint, disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger(Reference Stunkard and Messick11). Even if each method to assess eating behaviours has its strengths and weaknesses, results from a study comparing four questionnaires of dietary restraint measurement has shown that the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire cognitive dietary restraint scale was the most valid measure of the intent to diet and of actual energy restriction(Reference Williamson, Martin, York-Crowe, Anton, Redman and Han12).

The associations between eating behaviours and body-weight management have been studied extensively but some inconsistency remains. More precisely, some intervention studies underlined that dietary restraint was a predictor of weight loss(Reference Drummond, Dixon, Griffin and De Looy13). On the other hand, other studies found no association between cognitive dietary restraint and BMI(Reference Dykes, Brunner, Martikainen and Wardle14, Reference Provencher, Drapeau, Tremblay, Despres and Lemieux15). Higher scores of disinhibition, which is defined as a loss of control over eating in response to different stimuli that leads to an overconsumption of food(Reference Stunkard and Messick11), have been associated more systematically to higher BMI(Reference Provencher, Drapeau, Tremblay, Despres and Lemieux15Reference Hays, Bathalon, McCrory, Roubenoff, Lipman and Roberts19). The inconsistent associations between restraint and BMI could be partly explained by the fact that in some studies, increased restriction has been associated with increased disinhibition(Reference Westenhoefer, Stunkard and Pudel20, Reference McLean, Barr and Prior21).

Since change in food choices is the cornerstone of obesity management and because eating behaviours are related to obesity, better knowledge with regard to eating behaviours and their relationship with food and drink consumption and meal pattern could be helpful to improve dietary treatment of obesity. Some studies evaluated the relationship between eating behaviours and food consumption(Reference Borg, Fogelholm and Kukkonen-Harjula22Reference Contento, Zybert and Williams27). Accordingly, cognitive dietary restraint has been previously associated with frequent use of reduced-energy and reduced-fat foods(Reference Rideout, McLean and Barr26, Reference Kirkley, Burge and Ammerman28). Furthermore, French et al. demonstrated that subjects who self-reported that they were dieting to lose weight (which is related to increased dietary restraint)(Reference French, Jeffery and Forster29) had lower self-reported intake of several high-fat/high-energy foods, including sweets, meat, soft drinks, French fries and dairy products(Reference French, Jeffery and Forster29).

In most studies, associations between eating behaviours, BMI and food intakes have been examined in premenopausal women(Reference Tuschl, Platte, Laessle, Stichler and Pirke23, Reference Moreira, de Almeida and Sampaio25Reference Contento, Zybert and Williams27, Reference Westerterp-Plantenga, Wijckmans-Duijsens, Verboeket-van de Venne, de Graaf, het Hof and Weststrate30Reference Beiseigel and Nickols-Richardson32) and studies have generally not considered a wide set of dietary variables and rather focused on energy intake, macronutrient composition of the diet and only a few food groups. Fewer studies have been performed in healthy postmenopausal women not under treatment, under ‘real-life settings’. Also, to our knowledge, no study has yet investigated whether the presence of disinhibition could modulate the association between restraint and the pattern of food consumption among postmenopausal women. Therefore, the present cross-sectional study aimed at examining the relationship between eating behaviours (cognitive dietary restraint, disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger) and habitual food and drink consumption as well as energy and nutrient intake, energy density and meal pattern. We also wanted to verify the hypothesis that the presence of disinhibition could modulate the association between restraint and dietary variables under study.

Research design and methods

Subjects

The present study was conducted in a sample of 112 postmenopausal women (aged between 46 and 68 years) in a real-life uncontrolled context recruited through the media in the Quebec City metropolitan area. Those reporting that they did not had menses for at least 1 year were considered as postmenopausal and were included in the study. A measure of the follicle-stimulating hormone was used to confirm the menopausal status (follicle-stimulating hormone value between 28 and 127 IU/l). All women included in the present study were free from metabolic disorders, were not using any type of hormonal therapy and were not under treatment for CHD, diabetes, dyslipidaemias or endocrine disorders (except stable thyroid disease). Five women included in the present study were smokers. None of the participants had received a diagnosis of type 2 diabetes before the study. All participants signed an informed consent document before entering the study, which was approved by the Laval University Medical Centre and the Laval University Research Ethics Committees.

Dietary profile

Food intake was assessed by a 3 d weighed food record, which was completed during two weekdays and one weekend day. The food record was explained and reviewed by the study registered dietitian. Copies of food record examples were also provided to each subject. In addition, participants were encouraged to consume the usual amount of typical foods and drinks. The food record included a section for recording information about recipes. Women were asked to weigh foods with a scale provided by the dietitian. Evaluation of nutrient intake derived from the 3 d food record was performed using the Nutrition Data System for Research software (version 4.03, developed by the Nutrition Coordination Center, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, Food and Nutrient Database 31, released in November 2000)(Reference Schakel, Sievert and Buzzard33). Each 3 d food record was reviewed to determine daily portions for 121 categories of food items that were further regrouped in twenty-eight food groups. Food categories used were adapted from the food list derived from a FFQ developed by Goulet et al. (Reference Goulet, Nadeau, Lapointe, Lamarche and Lemieux34) (see Appendix 1 for more details).

Each subject also had to identify in the 3 d food record the time of day and location for each meal consumed. Total energy intake for each day was divided by the total weight of the foods and beverages reported to determine daily energy density values(Reference Ledikwe, Blanck, Khan, Serdula, Seymour, Tohill and Rolls35). Similarly, energy density was also calculated for each meal.

Anthropometric measurements

Height (cm), body weight (kg) and BMI (kg/m2) were determined following the procedures recommended at the Airlie Conference(Reference Lohman, Roche, Martorell, Lohman, Roche and Martorell36). Anthropometric measurements were performed after the completion of the weighed food record.

Questionnaire about history of weight

A questionnaire about weight and diet history was administered by a registered dietitian. Participants had to indicate whether they had previously been on a diet to lose weight and when was their last dieting attempt. They also had to indicate their body weight at age 20 years.

Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire

Postmenopausal women filled out a French version at home of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire. The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire is a fifty-one-item questionnaire developed by Stunkard & Messick in 1985(Reference Stunkard and Messick11). The purpose of this questionnaire is to assess three factors related to cognitions and behaviours associated with eating. These factors are cognitive dietary restraint, disinhibition, and susceptibility to hunger. More precisely, cognitive dietary restraint is a conscious control over food intake with concerns about shape and weight (twenty-one items, score ranging from 0 to 21). Disinhibition is an overconsumption of food in response to a variety of stimuli, such as emotional stress, associated with a loss of control on food intake (sixteen items, score ranging from 0 to 16). Finally, susceptibility to hunger refers to food intake in response to feelings and perceptions of hunger (fourteen items, score ranging from 0 to 14)(Reference Stunkard and Messick11). This questionnaire has been validated, and all three of these scales have good test–retest reliability(Reference Stunkard and Messick11, Reference Laessle, Tuschl, Kotthaus and Pirke37, Reference Lluch38).

Statistical analyses

Pearson correlation analyses were performed to assess how eating behaviours were associated with BMI, energy intake and energy density. The whole sample of women was divided according to the median of the distribution of cognitive dietary restraint (score of 9) and of disinhibition (score of 6), resulting in the formation of four subgroups (low restraint–low disinhibition; low restraint–high disinhibition; high restraint–low disinhibition; high restraint–high disinhibition). Comparisons between groups were performed by ANOVA. In the presence of significant effects, Duncan's multiple comparison test was used to determine precisely the location of significant differences. The χ2 frequency procedure was used to compare the frequency of dieters in the four groups described above. Moreover, participants were also divided in two subgroups using the median of the distribution of susceptibility to hunger score (4 units). Student's t tests were then computed to evaluate differences between these two subgroups.

Finally, ordinary least squares regression was performed to determine the independent contribution of cognitive dietary restraint, disinhibition and their interaction to the determination of dietary variables studied. All analyses were performed with the SAS statistical package version 8.02 (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

Results

Postmenopausal women had a mean age of 56·8 (sd 4·4) years, a mean menopause duration of 8·3 (sd 6·9) years and a mean BMI of 28·5 (sd 5·9) kg/m2. BMI was significantly correlated with dietary restraint (r − 0·33; P = 0·0003), disinhibition (r 0·49; P < 0·0001) and susceptibility to hunger (r 0·39; P < 0·0001). Neither eating behaviours nor BMI were significantly correlated with energy intake. However, cognitive dietary restraint and disinhibition were both significantly associated with daily energy density of the diet (r − 0·30, P = 0·002; r 0·21, P = 0·04, respectively). Because both cognitive dietary restraint and disinhibition were significantly associated with energy density, regression analysis was also performed to determine the contribution of restraint, disinhibition and the interaction between restraint and disinhibition to the variance in energy density. Results showed that only cognitive dietary restraint tended (P = 0·06) to be an independent predictor of energy density. Neither disinhibition (P = 0·97) nor the interaction between restraint and disinhibition (P = 0·51) was a predictor of energy density.

The whole sample of women was separated according to median values of the distribution of restraint and disinhibition levels. Accordingly, four subgroups were compared: low restraint–low disinhibition; low restraint–high disinhibition; high restraint–low disinhibition; high restraint–high disinhibition. It is shown in Table 1 that women with low restraint and low disinhibition scores were characterised by a higher BMI than women from the other three groups (P < 0·05). Also, women with low restraint and high disinhibition gained significantly more weight since the age of 20 years than women characterised by low restraint and low disinhibition and than both subgroups of women with a higher restraint. Women with low restraint and high disinhibition also had a higher BMI at the age of 20 years than women with low restraint and low disinhibition (P < 0·05). It is also shown in Table 1 that both groups of women with high restraint level had a lower energy density than the two groups of women with lower restraint level. Although all women were weight stable at the time of investigation, some of them had already experienced dieting in the past. It was found that women with low restraint and low disinhibition scores were less susceptible to have been dieting in the past than women from the other three groups.

Table 1 Dietary characteristics according to the level of dietary restraint and disinhibition in 112 postmenopausal women

(Mean values and standard deviations)

a,b Values with unlike superscript letters are significantly different (P < 0·05).

Table 2 presents differences in consumption of food and drinks according to restraint and disinhibition levels. Women with high restraint and low disinhibition were different from women with lower restraint for some dietary variables. In fact, they had a lower consumption of red meat and processed meat than women with low restraint and high disinhibition. They also presented higher intakes of whole grains than both groups of women with low restraint (i.e. low restraint–low disinhibition and low restraint–high disinhibition). In addition, women with high restraint and low disinhibition consumed fewer diet soft drinks than women with low restraint and high disinhibition.

Table 2 Differences in consumption of food and drinks according to restraint and disinhibition levels (n 112)

(Mean values and standard deviations)

a,b Mean values with unlike superscript letters are significantly different (P < 0·05).

Women with high restraint and high disinhibition also showed some differences when compared with women with lower restraint level. In fact, they had lower consumption of refined grains than women with low restraint and low disinhibition and lower consumption of diet soft drinks than women with low restraint and high disinhibition. When the two groups of women with high restraint values were directly compared with each other it was found that only the consumption of whole grains differed between the two groups (women with high restraint and low disinhibition having a higher consumption).

Low restraint combined with either high or low disinhibition differentially influenced dietary intake. Specifically, women with both low restraint and low disinhibition consumed less processed meat, less dessert and fewer soft drinks than women with low restraint and high disinhibition.

Subjects were also compared according to their susceptibility to hunger score (high v. low). Women characterised by a low susceptibility to hunger score had significantly lower total energy intakes than women with a higher score (7695 (sd 1685) v. 8648 (sd 1982) kJ respectively; P = 0·007) (Table 3). No significant differences were observed for macronutrient partition, energy density and number of meals per d. However, energy partition at breakfast and lunch tended to be significantly different between the two groups of women (breakfast: 32·6 (sd 26·9) % of daily energy for low v. 24·8 (sd 19·1) % for high susceptibility to hunger, P = 0·08; lunch: 23·2 (sd 11·7) % of daily energy for low v. 27·1 (sd 9·8) % for high susceptibility to hunger, P = 0·056).

Table 3 Dietary variables according to the level of susceptibility to hunger in postmenopausal women (n 112)

(Mean values and standard deviations)

* Mean value was significantly different from that of the group with low susceptibility to hunger (P ≤ 0·05).

Consumption of meat substitutes (i.e. legumes, tofu, nuts, seeds) (P = 0·04), desserts (P = 0·009) and diet soft drinks (P = 0·04) were significantly higher and consumption of fruit juices was significantly lower (P = 0·03) in subjects with a high susceptibility to hunger score than in women with lower susceptibility to hunger (Table 4).

Table 4 Differences in daily consumption of food and drinks according to the level of susceptibility to hunger in postmenopausal women (n 112)

(Mean values and standard deviations)

Discussion

The main purpose of the present study was to examine how eating behaviours were associated with dietary profile, meal pattern and energy density in healthy postmenopausal women. The present results showed that women characterised by a high restraint level (presenting either high or low disinhibition) had a diet with a lower energy density as compared with women with lower restraint level. Differences in the consumption of red meat and processed meat, whole grains, refined grains and diet soft drinks were also observed between women with high restraint compared with those with lower restraint. Contrary to our hypothesis, the presence of disinhibition in women with high restraint did not seem to have a major impact on the overall pattern of food and drink consumption.

Our data also revealed that there was no significant association between eating behaviours and energy intake. Although some studies have shown that highly restrained individuals are more susceptible to report a lower consumption of energy(Reference Moreira, de Almeida and Sampaio25, Reference Rideout, McLean and Barr26, Reference de Lauzon, Romon, Deschamps, Lafay, Borys, Karlsson, Ducimetière and Charles39Reference de Castro41), a lower energy intake has not been systematically reported in highly restrained eaters(Reference Tepper, Trail and Shaffer42). The lack of consistency in reported associations between dietary restraint and actual physiological energy restriction might be explained by the fact that dietary restraint is a heterogeneous concept and can also reflect a state of perceived deprivation (eating less than wanted without creating energy deficit)(Reference Lowe and Levine43). In addition, it is recognised that restraint could be imposed either by those who are currently dieting to lose weight or in those who have successfully lost weight and wish to maintain their weight status. Also some restrained eaters imposed restraint simply because they do not want to gain weight(Reference Weiss, Galuska, Khan and Serdula10). It is also possible that highly restrained subjects are more likely to underreport their dietary intake(Reference Bathalon, Tucker, Hays, Vinken, Greenberg, McCrory and Roberts44). However, the significant negative association between cognitive dietary restraint and BMI in the present study suggests that women with higher cognitive dietary restraint have an energy intake which is more accurately adapted to actual energy needs.

The present results also showed that women with low restraint and high disinhibition had the highest BMI and the more important body-weight gain since the age of 20 years, which suggests persistent positive energy balance. From the present results, it is difficult to determine whether the gain in body weight was a factor responsible for the determination of eating behaviours or whether eating behaviours had an impact on body-weight variation. Furthermore, we can not assume that eating behaviours measured after menopause are representative of eating behaviours measured at the age of 20 years since previous studies have shown that eating behaviours are likely to change over time(Reference Drapeau, Provencher, Lemieux, Despres, Bouchard and Tremblay31, Reference Rizvi, Stice and Agras45).

Although eating behaviours were not significantly associated with energy intake, the present results showed that cognitive dietary restraint was negatively and disinhibition was positively associated with energy density of the diet. Furthermore, regression analyses revealed that only cognitive dietary restraint tended to be a significant predictor of energy density. This is concordant with the fact that women with high restraint, irrespective of disinhibition level, had a diet with lower energy density than women with lower restraint level. The lower energy density of the diet of women with high restraint might be one of the factors explaining the significant association between higher cognitive dietary restraint and lower BMI. In fact, dietary energy density has been suggested as a determinant of energy intake and of body weight(Reference Yao and Roberts46Reference Kant and Graubard48).

With regard to food and drink consumption, we found some differences between women with high v. low cognitive dietary restraint. The lower consumption of red meat and processed meat and the higher consumption of whole grains in women with high restraint and low disinhibition when compared with women with lower restraint level are suggestive of a healthier dietary pattern. In fact, lower consumption of red meat and increased consumption of whole grains are components of the Alternate Healthy Eating Index that has been previously described as a dietary pattern associated with lower risk of major chronic diseases(Reference McCullough, Feskanich, Stampfer, Giovannucci, Rimm, Hu, Spiegelman, Hunter, Colditz and Willett49). However, the lower consumption of diet soft drinks in women with high cognitive restraint is somehow discordant with previous studies suggesting that restrained eaters are more likely to avoid high-energy food items(Reference Laessle, Tuschl, Kotthaus and Pirke50) and to prefer food generally labelled as ‘low-calorie’(Reference Laessle, Tuschl, Kotthaus and Pirke50). On the other hand, the lower consumption of diet soft drinks might be an additional indicator of an overall healthier diet. In fact, it has been recently shown by Dhingra et al. (Reference Dhingra, Sullivan, Jacques, Wang, Fox, Meigs, Di'Agostino, Gaziano and Vasan51) that the incidence of the metabolic syndrome was increased in middle-aged adults consuming at least one soft drink per d, regardless of whether it was regular or diet.

Contrary to our hypothesis, the presence of disinhibition did not seem to influence to a large extent the association between cognitive dietary restraint and food and drink consumption. In fact, the only difference between women with high restraint–low disinhibition and women with high restraint–high disinhibition was a higher whole grain intake in women with low disinhibition. Therefore, it appears that the presence of cognitive dietary restraint might be a more important determinant of food and drink consumption than disinhibition. This is concordant with regression analysis showing that cognitive dietary restraint tended to be a significant predictor of energy density whereas neither disinhibition nor the interaction between restraint and disinhibition predicted energy density. However, from the present results it can be postulated that the impact of disinhibition on food pattern might vary according to cognitive dietary restraint. In fact, among women with lower restraint, the presence of disinhibition was associated with the consumption of more processed meat, more desserts and more diet soft drinks which can overall explain, at least partially, the increased BMI observed in women with low restraint and high disinhibition when compared with the other three groups of women tested.

The relationship between meal pattern and eating behaviours has not been thoroughly investigated so far. It has been reported that subjects with either high or low dietary restraint eat similar numbers of meals per d, but restrained eaters consumed more snacks(Reference Kirkley, Burge and Ammerman28). In the present study, no significant difference in variables related to meal pattern was noted between groups of women separated on the basis of eating behaviour scores. However, women characterised by a low susceptibility to hunger consumed less energy and tended to consume a larger proportion of energy at breakfast than women with high susceptibility to hunger. It has been previously reported that the proportion of intake in the morning was negatively correlated with total daily intake(Reference de Castro52). De Castro also reported that human subjects demonstrated less satiety from a given amount of food later in the day than earlier(Reference de Castro53), suggesting that a larger proportion of total intake in the morning is related to a smaller total intake. According to the present results, women with low susceptibility to hunger score consumed less energy on a daily basis and had a lower body weight. Therefore, we could hypothesise that women with high susceptibility to hunger could benefit from eating a larger amount in the morning when the satiating value of food is higher and, thus, this may contribute to regulate their energy intake and probably their body weight. However, we agree that changes in daily meal partition may be difficult to achieve and maintain in a real-life context. Further studies will be needed to understand the interrelationship between susceptibility to hunger, meal pattern and body-weight regulation.

Conclusion

The present results suggest associations between eating behaviours, energy and nutrient intakes, dietary food choices and meal pattern in healthy postmenopausal women. In the present study, postmenopausal women with higher cognitive dietary restraint appear to have a healthier dietary profile while the presence of disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger were rather associated with a less healthy dietary profile. Although nutritional advice per se is a key component in weight management, results from the present study suggest that intervention aiming at modifying eating behaviours may also influence food choices that will in turn impact on the regulation of energy balance and body weight.

Acknowledgements

J. G. is a recipient of a studentship from the Fonds de la Recherche en Santé du Québec (FRSQ). V. P. is a recipient of a studentship from FRSQ and A. L. is a recipient of a studentship from the Canadian Institute of Health Research and the FRSQ. The present study was supported by the Canada Institutes of Health Research (MOP-37957) and by the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada.

The authors would like to express their gratitude to the subjects for their excellent collaboration and to the staff of the Lipid Research Centre, the Physical Activity Sciences Laboratory, and the Diabetes Research Unit for their contribution to the present study. We especially want to thank Louise Corneau MSc RD, Nancy Gilbert MSc RD, Rolande Couture, Danielle Aubin, Fanny Therrien, Marie Tremblay and George Cousineau for their help in the collection and analysis of the data.

None of the authors had a personal or professional conflict of interest.

J. G. performed data analysis and drafted the manuscript. M. E. P. and V. P. participated in data collection. A. L. participated in data analysis. S. J. W., A. N., J. B. and S. L. conceived the study, and participated in its design and coordination.

Appendix 1

Food list

  • Vegetables. Cruciferous vegetables, orange vegetables, dark green vegetables, tomatoes, other vegetables, garlic, potatoes.

  • Fruits. Citrus fruits, other raw fruits, fruit sauce, dried fruits.

  • Fruit juices. 100 % fruit juices.

  • Legumes. Legumes, tofu.

  • Nuts. Peanut butter (hydrogenated or not), other nut butter, nuts and seeds.

  • Whole-grain bread and cereals. Bread, crackers, bagels, English muffins, pita bread, homemade muffins, pasta, brown rice, whole-grain couscous, breakfast cereals, hot cereals, granola bars, other (linseeds, wheat germ), croissants, waffles, pancakes.

  • Refined bread and cereals. Bread, crackers, bagels, English muffins, pita bread, homemade muffins, pasta, rice, couscous, breakfast cereals, hot cereals, granola bars, other (linseeds, wheat germ), croissants, waffles, pancakes.

  • Skimmed and partly skimmed dairy products. Milk (1 or 2 % fat), soya milk, sour cream (less than 5 % fat), cottage cheese (1 or 2 % fat), yoghurt (skimmed, 1 or 2 % fat), frozen milk or yoghurt (less than 5 % fat), milk dessert (skimmed, 1 or 2 % fat).

  • Whole dairy products. Milk (3·25 % fat), yoghurt (2 % fat or more), regular cottage or ricotta cheese, cheese, cream, sour cream (5 % fat or more), ice cream.

  • Processed meat. Delicatessen, sausages, headcheese (brawn), bacon.

  • Red meat. Beef, pork, veal, lamb, hamburgers, other red meat.

  • Organ meat. Liver, kidney, liver pâté, etc.

  • Poultry. Chicken, turkey, duck, other white meat not mentioned above.

  • Fish and seafoods. Fresh, frozen and canned fish, shrimps, scallops, etc.

  • Other food categories. Fries.

  • Eggs. Eggs, recipes (quiche, omelette).

  • Saturated and trans fat. Butter, lard, shortening, hydrogenated margarine.

  • Unhydrogenated fat. Unhydrogenated margarine, olive oil, rapeseed oil, other oils, olives, mayonnaise, salad dressing (regular or light).

  • Fast food. Poutine (French fries, cheese curds and gravy), chicken (nuggets or fried), fish (nuggets or fried), meat pie.

  • Pizza. All dressed, cheese, vegetarian.

  • Snacks. Popcorn, salted crackers, pretzels, chips (crisps).

  • High-energy drinks. Regular soft drinks, fruit punch.

  • Low-energy drinks. Diet soft drinks, other drinks low in energy (diet fruit drinks), excluding water.

  • Alcohol. Beer, wine, spirits.

  • Other beverages. Water, coffee, tea, herbal tea.

  • Soup. Soup, cream.

  • Dessert, sweets. Chocolate, candy, cookies, pie, cake, doughnuts, bars.

  • Side dishes. Sauce (ketchup, mustard, etc), jam, syrup, honey, sugar.

Notes

  • For dairy products we have established that a portion was equivalent to one cup of milk or enriched soya beverages, 50 g cheese or 175 g yoghurt.

  • For red meat and processed meat, poultry, or fish, one portion was equivalent to 1 ounce (about 30 g).

  • For the meat substitutes component we have established that a portion was equivalent to half a cup of legumes, quarter a cup of nuts or seeds or 100 g tofu.

  • For grains products we have established that a portion was equivalent to one slice of bread, half a cup of pasta, rice or couscous, 30 g cereal.

  • For fruits and vegetables we have established that a portion was equivalent to half a cup or one medium fruit or vegetable (fruit and vegetable juices included).

  • For added fat a portion was equivalent to one teaspoon.

  • For sweets, we have established that a portion was equivalent, for example, to 1/12 of cake, 1/6 of pie or one regular chocolate bar.

  • For soft drinks a portion was equivalent to a cup.

  • For snacks a portion was equivalent to one cup of pop maize or ten saltine crackers, pretzels, chips (crisps) or nachos.

References

1Ledoux, M (1998) Poids corporel. In Equête Social et de Santé 1998 (Body Weight: Social and Health Questions), chapter 8. Québec: Institut de la Statistique du Québec.Google Scholar
2Flegal, KM, Carroll, MD, Ogden, CL & Johnson, CL (2002) Prevalence and trends in obesity among US adults, 1999–2000. JAMA 288, 17231727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Tjepkema, M & Shields, M (2005) Nutrition Findings from Canadian Community Health Survey Adult Obesity in Canada: Measured Height and Weight. Ottawa: Statistics Canada.Google Scholar
4World Health Organization (1997) Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic. Report on a WHO Consultation on Obesity. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
5Kissebah, AH (1996) Intra-abdominal fat: is it a major factor in developing diabetes and coronary artery disease? Diabetes Res Clin Pract 30, Suppl., 2530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6Fondation des Maladies du Coeur du Canada (2005) Les maladies cardiovasculaires et accidents vasculaires cérébraux au Canada (Cardiovascular illnesses and cerebral vascular accidents in Canada). 4-7-2005. Ottawa, Canada: Fondation des Maladies du Coeur du Canada. www.fmcoeur.ca.Google Scholar
7Eaker, E, Chesebro, JH, Sacks, FM, Wenger, NK, Whisnant, JP & Winston, M (1994) Special report: cardiovascular disease in women. Special writing group. Heart Dis Stroke 3, 114119.Google ScholarPubMed
8Mosca, L, Manson, JE, Sutherland, SE, Langer, RD, Manolio, T, Barrett-Connor, E & Writing Group (1997) Cardiovascular disease in women: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association. Circulation 96, 24682482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Wing, RR & Phelan, S (2005) Long-term weight loss maintenance. Am J Clin Nutr 82, Suppl. 1, 222S225S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Weiss, EC, Galuska, DA, Khan, LK & Serdula, MK (2006) Weight-control practices among U.S. adults, 2001–2002. Am J Prev Med 31, 1824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Stunkard, AJ & Messick, S (1985) The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 29, 7183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Williamson, DA, Martin, CK, York-Crowe, E, Anton, SD, Redman, LM & Han, H (2007) Measurement of dietary restraint: validity tests of four questionnaires. Appetite 48, 183192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Drummond, S, Dixon, K, Griffin, J & De Looy, A (2004) Weight loss on an energy-restricted, low-fat, sugar-containing diet in overweight sedentary men. Int J Food Sci Nutr 55, 279290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14Dykes, J, Brunner, EJ, Martikainen, PT & Wardle, J (2004) Socioeconomic gradient in body size and obesity among women: the role of dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger in the Whitehall II study. Int J Obes 28, 262268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15Provencher, V, Drapeau, V, Tremblay, A, Despres, JP & Lemieux, S (2003) Eating behaviors and indexes of body composition in men and women from the Quebec family study. Obes Res 11, 783792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16Lindroos, AK, Lissner, L, Mathiassen, ME, Karlsson, J, Sullivan, M, Bengtsson, C & Sjöström, L (1997) Dietary intake in relation to restrained eating, disinhibition, and hunger in obese and nonobese Swedish women. Obes Res 5, 175182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Carmody, TP, Brunner, RL & St Jeor, ST (1995) Dietary helplessness and disinhibition in weight cyclers and maintainers. Int J Eat Disord 18, 247256.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18Lawson, OJ, Williamson, DA, Champagne, CM, DeLany, JP, Brooks, ER, Howat, PM, Wozniak, PJ, Bray, GA & Ryan, DH (1995) The association of body weight, dietary intake, and energy expenditure with dietary restraint and disinhibition. Obes Res 3, 153161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19Hays, NP, Bathalon, GP, McCrory, MA, Roubenoff, R, Lipman, R & Roberts, SB (2002) Eating behavior correlates of adult weight gain and obesity in healthy women aged 55–65 y. Am J Clin Nutr 75, 476483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20Westenhoefer, J, Stunkard, AJ & Pudel, V (1999) Validation of the flexible and rigid control dimensions of dietary restraint. Int J Eat Disord 26, 5364.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21McLean, JA, Barr, SI & Prior, JC (2001) Cognitive dietary restraint is associated with higher urinary cortisol excretion in healthy premenopausal women. Appetite 73, 712.Google ScholarPubMed
22Borg, P, Fogelholm, M & Kukkonen-Harjula, K (2004) Food selection and eating behaviour during weight maintenance intervention and 2-y follow-up in obese men. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 28, 15481554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Tuschl, RJ, Platte, P, Laessle, RG, Stichler, W & Pirke, KM (1990) Energy expenditure and everyday eating behavior in healthy young women. Am J Clin Nutr 52, 8186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24French, SA, Jeffery, RW & Wing, RR (1994) Food intake and physical activity: a comparison of three measures of dieting. Addict Behav 19, 401409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25Moreira, P, de Almeida, MD & Sampaio, D (2005) Cognitive restraint is associated with higher intake of vegetables in a sample of university students. Eat Behav 6, 229237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26Rideout, CA, McLean, JA & Barr, SI (2004) Women with high scores for cognitive dietary restraint choose foods lower in fat and energy. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 11541157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Contento, IR, Zybert, P & Williams, SS (2005) Relationship of cognitive restraint of eating and disinhibition to the quality of food choices of Latina women and their young children. Prev Med 40, 326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28Kirkley, BG, Burge, JC & Ammerman, A (1988) Dietary restraint, binge eating and dietary behavior patterns. Int J Eat Disord 7, 771778.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29French, SA, Jeffery, RW & Forster, JL (1994) Dieting status and its relationship to weight, dietary intake, and physical activity changes over two years in a working population. Obes Res 2, 135144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30Westerterp-Plantenga, MS, Wijckmans-Duijsens, NE, Verboeket-van de Venne, WP, de Graaf, K, het Hof, KH & Weststrate, JA (1998) Energy intake and body weight effects of six months reduced or full fat diets, as a function of dietary restraint. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 22, 1422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31Drapeau, V, Provencher, V, Lemieux, S, Despres, JP, Bouchard, C & Tremblay, A (2003) Do 6-y changes in eating behaviors predict changes in body weight? Results from the Quebec Family Study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 27, 808814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32Beiseigel, JM & Nickols-Richardson, SM (2004) Cognitive eating restraint scores are associated with body fatness but not with other measures of dieting in women. Appetite 43, 4753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
33Schakel, SF, Sievert, YA & Buzzard, IM (1988) Sources of data for developing and maintaining a nutrient database. J Am Diet Assoc 88, 12681271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34Goulet, J, Nadeau, G, Lapointe, A, Lamarche, B & Lemieux, S (2004) Validity and reproducibility of an interviewer-administered food frequency questionnaire for healthy French-Canadian men and women. Nutr J 3, 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35Ledikwe, JH, Blanck, HM, Khan, LK, Serdula, MK, Seymour, JD, Tohill, BC & Rolls, BJ (2005) Dietary energy density determined by eight calculation methods in a nationally representative United States population. J Nutr 135, 273278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36Lohman, TG, Roche, AF & Martorell, R (1988) The Airlie (VA) consensus conference. In Anthropometric Standardization Reference Manual, pp. 3980 [Lohman, TG, Roche, AF and Martorell, R, editors]. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.Google Scholar
37Laessle, RG, Tuschl, RJ, Kotthaus, BC & Pirke, KM (1989) A comparison of the validity of three scales for the assessment of dietary restraint. J Abnorm Psychol 98, 504507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38Lluch, A (1995) Identification des Conduites Alimentaire par Approches Nutritionnelles et Psychométriques: Implications Thérapeutiques et Préventives dans l'Obésité Humaines (Identification of Eating Patterns by Nutritional and Psychometric Approaches: Implications for the Prevention and Treatment of Human Obesity). PhD Thesis, University of Nancy 1, France.Google Scholar
39de Lauzon, B, Romon, M, Deschamps, V, Lafay, L, Borys, JM, Karlsson, J, Ducimetière, P, Charles, MA & Fleurbaix Laventie Ville Sante Study Group (2004) The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire-R18 is able to distinguish among different eating patterns in a general population. J Nutr 134, 23722380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40Klesges, RC, Isbell, TR & Klesges, LM (1992) Relationship between dietary restraint, energy intake, physical activity, and body weight: a prospective analysis. J Abnorm Psychol 101, 668674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41de Castro, JM (1995) The relationship of cognitive restraint to the spontaneous food and fluid intake of free-living humans. Physiol Behav 57, 287295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42Tepper, BJ, Trail, AC & Shaffer, SE (1996) Diet and physical activity in restrained eaters. Appetite 27, 5164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43Lowe, MR & Levine, AS (2005) Eating motives and the controversy over dieting: eating less than needed versus less than wanted. Obes Res 13, 797806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44Bathalon, GP, Tucker, KL, Hays, NP, Vinken, AG, Greenberg, AS, McCrory, MA & Roberts, SB (2000) Psychological measures of eating behavior and the accuracy of 3 common dietary assessment methods in healthy postmenopausal women. Am J Clin Nutr 71, 739745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45Rizvi, SL, Stice, E & Agras, WS (1999) Natural history of disordered eating attitudes and behaviors over a 6-year period. Int J Eat Disord 26, 406413.3.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
46Yao, M & Roberts, SB (2001) Dietary energy density and weight regulation. Nutr Rev 59, 247258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47Poppitt, SD & Prentice, AM (1996) Energy density and its role in the control of food intake: evidence from metabolic and community studies. Appetite 26, 153174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48Kant, AK & Graubard, BI (2005) Energy density of diets reported by American adults: association with food group intake, nutrient intake, and body weight. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 29, 950956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49McCullough, ML, Feskanich, D, Stampfer, MJ, Giovannucci, EL, Rimm, EB, Hu, FB, Spiegelman, D, Hunter, DJ, Colditz, GA & Willett, WC (2002) Diet quality and major chronic disease risk in men and women: moving toward improved dietary guidance. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 12611271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50Laessle, RG, Tuschl, RJ, Kotthaus, BC & Pirke, KM (1989) Behavioral and biological correlates of dietary restraint in normal life. Appetite 12, 8394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51Dhingra, R, Sullivan, L, Jacques, PF, Wang, TJ, Fox, CS, Meigs, JB, Di'Agostino, RB, Gaziano, JM & Vasan, RS (2007) Soft drink consumption and risk of developing cardiometabolic risk factors and the metabolic syndrome in middle-aged adults in the community. Circulation 116, 480488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52de Castro, JM (2004) The time of day of food intake influences overall intake in humans. J Nutr 134, 104111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53de Castro, JM (1987) Circadian rhythms of the spontaneous meal pattern, macronutrient intake, and mood of humans. Physiol Behav 40, 437446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Dietary characteristics according to the level of dietary restraint and disinhibition in 112 postmenopausal women(Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 1

Table 2 Differences in consumption of food and drinks according to restraint and disinhibition levels (n 112)(Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 2

Table 3 Dietary variables according to the level of susceptibility to hunger in postmenopausal women (n 112)(Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 3

Table 4 Differences in daily consumption of food and drinks according to the level of susceptibility to hunger in postmenopausal women (n 112)(Mean values and standard deviations)