Infants and young children are at particular risk of Fe deficiency, which can lead to microcytosis and anaemia. Term infants are born with Fe reserves that last for about 6 months; however after this the infant relies on dietary intakes( Reference Agostoni, Decsi and Fewtrell 1 ). In addition to inadequate dietary Fe intakes, other dietary practices including an increased consumption of unmodified cows’ milk and reduced consumption of Fe-fortified products can also result in Fe deficiency in infancy and early childhood( Reference McCarthy, Ní Chaoimh and Hourihane 2 , Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson 3 ). Globally, it is estimated that 25 % of preschool-age children have Fe deficiency anaemia( Reference McLean, Cogswell and Egli 4 ), while in Europe, the prevalence of Fe deficiency ranges from 3 to 48 %, with the majority of Fe deficiency anaemia rates reported as below 5 %( Reference Eussen, Alles and Uijterschout 5 ). However, the lack of consensus and therefore, the wide variability in diagnostic criteria, including both biomarkers and thresholds used for Fe deficiency and Fe deficiency anaemia in infants and young children has challenged the interpretation of Fe status worldwide.
Fe is essential for a number of processes in the developing brain; altered neurotransmitter functioning, brain morphology and reduced myelination have all been observed in Fe deficient rodent models( Reference Lozoff and Georgieff 6 , Reference Lozoff 7 ). Studies in humans have consistently reported that Fe deficiency with and without anaemia in infancy and early childhood is associated with poorer cognitive, motor and behavioural outcomes( Reference Lozoff, Beard and Connor 8 , Reference Grantham-McGregor and Ani 9 ), with long-lasting effects into adulthood( Reference Lozoff, Smith and Kaciroti 10 ).
Most studies exploring associations between Fe and neurodevelopment have focused on 6–24-month-olds in low-resource settings or in low socioeconomic subgroups; few have explored associations in healthy young children in low risk, high-resource settings. Therefore, the aim of this study was to explore associations between Fe status, haematological indices and neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 years in an apparently healthy, low risk population recruited through a prospective birth cohort in Ireland.
Methods
Study design and participants
This study is a secondary analysis of a nested case–control subgroup of participants that were recruited as part of the Cork Babies after Screening for Pregnancy Endpoints (SCOPE): Evaluating the Longitudinal Impact using Neurological and Nutritional Endpoints (BASELINE) Birth Cohort Study between March 2008 and January 2011. The BASELINE Study was established as a follow-on to the SCOPE Ireland pregnancy cohort study where 1768 low risk, nulliparous women were recruited before 15 weeks’ gestation from Cork University Maternity Hospital, as part of an international, multicentre study aimed at investigating early indicators of pregnancy complications( Reference Kenny, Black and Poston 11 ). At 15 weeks’ gestation, research midwives collected information on maternal socioeconomic status, occupation, education, relationship status and a complete medical history. Information on nutritional supplement use, recreational activity, cigarette and alcohol use were recorded for the 3-month period before conception and during the first trimester. Maternal anthropometric and clinical measurements were also collected.
Women in the SCOPE Ireland study provided written informed consent to the BASELINE Study for their infants at 15 weeks’ gestation (n 1537). Infants were followed prospectively from birth, with assessments at day 2 and at 2, 6, 12 and 24 months. Assessments at 5 years of age were completed in December 2016. Detailed information on early life environment, diet, lifestyle, health, growth and development was gathered by interviewer-led questionnaires and clinical assessments performed by trained researchers in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, with further information on study design and procedures reported previously( Reference O’Donovan, Murray and Hourihane 12 ). Ethical approval was granted by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee of the Cork teaching hospitals (ECM 5(9) 01/07/2008) and the study is registered with the US Institutes of Health Clinical Trials Registry (www.clinicaltrials.gov NCT01498965).
Developmental assessment
Within the BASELINE Study cohort, a nested case–control subgroup (n 460) of firstborn children was selected at birth for developmental assessment to examine the effect of fetal growth on developmental outcomes( Reference O’Neill, Hannon and Khashan 13 ). A statistician identified the subgroup using the following criteria to select cases: (1) children born small-for-gestational age, defined as a customised birth weight centile (adjusted for maternal height, booking weight, ethnicity, infant sex and gestational age( Reference McCowan, Thompson and Taylor 14 )) less than the 10th centile (n 80) and (2) children with a body fat percentage (measured by air-displacement plethysmography) at 2 d less than the 10th centile for gestation and sex (n 81). The control group consisted of appropriate-for-gestational age (AGA) infants comprised of a stratified random sample taken from across the customised birth weight centiles above the 10th centile (n 299).
Participants identified as eligible for the nested subgroup at birth were approached at their 24-month assessment to partake in a detailed developmental assessment. The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd edition (BSID-III)( Reference Bayley 15 ) was administered by a research psychologist trained in administration and scoring of the BSID-III who was blinded to the status of the participants. The assessment consists of five scales: cognition, receptive communication, expressive communication, fine motor and gross motor, with receptive and expressive communication combined to form a language composite score and fine and gross motor combined to form a motor composite score. Published normative scores for the composite scales of the BSID-Ш are a mean of 100 with a sd of 15; developmental delay was defined as a score <85.
For the secondary, cross sectional analysis of the nested case–control subgroup in this study, we only included the healthy, AGA children from the control group to ensure exclusion of those with fetal growth restriction, due to its confounding impact on both Fe status and neurodevelopment( Reference Savchev, Sanz-Cortes and Cruz-Martinez 16 , Reference Rao and Georgieff 17 ). We also excluded children with acute infections, chronic disease/illness or born premature (<37 weeks’ gestation).
Biological samples
Venous blood was collected from participants at the 24-month assessment, in only those with parental consent. Whole blood was sent to the Haematology Laboratory of Cork University Hospital for determination of Hb and mean corpuscular volume (MCV) on the Sysmex XE 2100 Automated Hematology System (Sysmex America Inc.). Serum ferritin and high sensitivity C-reactive protein (CRP) were analysed in the laboratory of the Cork Centre for Vitamin D and Nutrition Research, University College Cork, by immunoturbidimetric assay on the RX Monaco Clinical Chemistry Analyser (Randox Laboratories Ltd, Co.). Children with potential infections/inflammation as indicated by an elevated CRP (>5 mg/l) were excluded from analyses.
Statistical analysis
Data were analysed using IBM SPSS® for Windows™ version 21 (IBM Corp.). The distribution of data was examined by Q–Q plots and histograms and descriptive statistics were generated; medians and interquartile ranges (IQR) were used for not normally distributed data, whereas means and standard deviations were provided for the BSID-III scores due to their normal distribution. Significant differences between groups in categorical variables were evaluated using χ 2 tests and independent t tests or Mann–Whitney U test for continuous variables. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d effect size calculation. Given the low numbers with Fe deficiency or Fe deficiency anaemia, associations with individual haematological indices, first as continuous variables and then as categorical variables (assigned using commonly used thresholds) were explored. Univariate and multivariate adjusted linear regression models were developed to estimate the effect of each haematological index on BSID-III scores. Factors explored in the univariate analysis included maternal age, education, occupation, income, relationship status, Fe status, smoking status, BMI, mode of delivery and infant sex, gestational age, birth anthropometric measures and early feeding methods. Potential confounding factors that were retained in the final multivariate models were identified if they were significant at the 10 % (P<0·1) level in the univariate analysis. The residuals of the final model were normally distributed and were also assessed for linearity, homoscedasticity, independence and for the presence of any outliers; associations were expressed as adjusted estimates and 95 % CI.
In addition, receiver operating characteristic curve analysis was used to estimate the ferritin concentration cut-point to best predict a MCV <74 fl. The AUC, sensitivity and specificity were calculated and the best possible cut-point for ferritin concentrations was defined at the highest Youden Index ((sensitivity+specificity)−1).
Results
From the 460 children selected in the nested case–control subgroup, 366 had a complete BSID-Ш assessment at 2 years (forty-two had an incomplete assessment and fifty-two were unable to attend the assessment). Only the children born AGA (control group) were included in this secondary analysis (n 240). The measurement of haematological indices at 2 years was a prerequisite for this study, therefore children with no blood sample provided at 2 years (n 144) were excluded. Those with infection/inflammation at 2 years (n 3) or born premature (n 6) were also excluded to provide a final sample size of eighty-seven children (see Fig. 1). The principal characteristics of those included in this study and their comparisons to the rest of the study cohort that attended the 24-month assessment are presented in Table 1.
* Maternal data collected at 15 weeks’ gestation unless otherwise stated.
† P values are comparisons between groups with χ 2 or Mann–Whitney U tests.
‡ Haematological indices not available in all participants that were not eligible for this study.
Median Hb, MCV and serum ferritin concentrations at 2 years of those included in this study are outlined in Table 1. Using WHO definitions, Fe deficiency (serum ferritin<12 µg/l) was observed in 6 % (n 5) and no child had Fe deficiency anaemia (serum ferritin<12 µg/l+Hb<110 g/l). Five children were anaemic (Hb<110 g/l) and 15 % (n 13) had a MCV <74 fl, a threshold indicative of microcytosis( Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson 3 , Reference Thorisdottir, Thorsdottir and Palsson 18 , Reference Vendt, Grunberg and Leedo 19 ).
The median age of completion for the BSID-Ш assessment was 27·2 (IQR 26·0, 28·6) months. The distribution of subscale and composite scores of the BSID-Ш are presented in Table 2. There was a low prevalence of scores indicative of developmental delay; 6 % (n 5), 6 % (n 5) and 3 % (n 2) had scores <85 in the cognition, language and motor composite scales, respectively.
Hb concentrations at 2 years were not associated with BSID-Ш subscale or composite scores at 2 years. From the univariate regression analysis, MCV was positively associated with BSID-Ш cognitive composite (estimate 0·71; 95 % CI 0·03, 1·40), P=0·041) scores, however, this association was no longer significant in the final model adjusted for potential confounding factors (sex, birth weight, maternal education, occupation, household income). Children with microcytosis (MCV<74 fl) had significantly lower BSID-Ш cognitive composite scores than those without microcytosis (MCV≥74 fl), with a strong Cohen’s d effect size of 0·8 (Table 3). This association remained robust after adjustment for potential confounding factors (adjusted estimate −6·7; 95 % CI −12·9, −0·5, P=0·034, see online Supplementary Table S1 for full multivariate model). There were no other significant differences in study characteristics between those with and without microcytosis at 2 years.
* P values are comparisons between groups using independent t tests.
† P values from linear regression models adjusted for sex, birth weight, maternal education, occupation and household income.
‡ Cohen’s d effect size=0·8
Of the five participants with cognitive delay (scores <85), four had microcytosis. Only four children with microcytosis at 2 years had concurrent Fe deficiency, indicated by ferritin concentrations <12 µg/l. When the four children were excluded, those with microcytosis and ferritin concentrations ≥12 µg/l (n 9) continued to have lower mean cognitive composite (87·2 (sd 15·2) v. 96·9 (sd 7·7), P=0·069) scores at 2 years, although this association was weaker.
Serum ferritin concentrations were positively associated with BSID-Ш fine motor subscale scores (estimate 0·04; 95 % CI 0·01, 0·08, P=0·036) at 2 years, which was robust after adjustment for sex, birth weight, maternal education, occupation and household income (adjusted estimate 0·04; 95 % CI 0·01, 0·08, P=0·037). There were no significant associations between Fe deficiency or Fe deficiency anaemia and BSID-Ш scores at 2 years.
There was a strong positive association between MCV and ferritin concentrations at 2 years (Spearman’s r 0·52, P<0·0001). Children with microcytosis had significantly lower median ferritin concentrations compared with those without microcytosis (13·8 (IQR 11·6, 15·8) v. 21·1 (IQR 18·1, 39·3) µg/l, P<0·0001). Following this observation, receiver operating characteristic curve analysis was performed to estimate the ferritin concentration to best predict microcytosis in this subgroup of healthy children. The ferritin concentration which best predicted microcytosis was calculated to be 18·4 µg/l (AUC=0·87 (95 % CI 0·75, 0·98), P<0·0001, sensitivity 92 %, specificity 75 %). From this, a threshold of <18·5 µg/l was subsequently used to investigate associations with BSID-Ш scores. Children with ferritin concentrations <18·5 µg/l (n 28) had significantly lower mean BSID-Ш cognitive composite (92·3 (sd 10·5) v. 97·8 (sd 8·1), P=0·012, Cohen’s d effect size=0·6) scores than those with concentrations ≥18·5 µg/l. After adjustment for confounding factors, ferritin concentrations <18·5 µg/l remained inversely associated with cognitive composite (adjusted estimate −6·0 (95% CI −10·2, −1·8), P=0·006) scores at 2 years.
Discussion
This secondary analysis of a nested case–control subgroup of children from a low risk maternal-infant birth cohort has provided a much-needed exploration of associations between haematological indices and neurodevelopmental outcomes in a high-resource setting. In this apparently healthy cohort, microcytosis, indicated by a MCV <74 fl was associated with lower cognitive outcomes in 2-year-old children. We also observed lower cognitive scores in children with ferritin concentrations <18·5 µg/l, a threshold we estimated to predict microcytosis in this group of children, highlighting an association between currently defined normal or borderline low ferritin concentrations, as opposed to overt Fe deficiency, and adverse developmental outcomes in children in a high-resource setting.
Neither Fe deficiency nor Fe deficiency anaemia was associated with BSID-Ш scores at 2 years in this study, in contrast to findings from earlier studies in more deprived populations( Reference Grantham-McGregor and Ani 9 , Reference Lozoff, Brittenham and Wolf 20 ). However, our study was not adequately powered to accurately explore these associations given the low prevalence of Fe deficiency and Fe deficiency anaemia in our healthy cohort, which we previously attributed to a dietary pattern of increased Fe-fortified product consumption and low intakes of unmodified cows’ milk( Reference McCarthy, Ní Chaoimh and Hourihane 2 ). Our rates of Fe deficiency and Fe deficiency anaemia are much lower than contemporary data reported in many other European countries( Reference Eussen, Alles and Uijterschout 5 ) and the USA( Reference Brotanek, Gosz and Weitzman 21 ) and are in particular contrast to the last report in Ireland from the 1990’s, where 9 % of 2-year-olds had Fe deficiency anaemia( Reference Freeman, Mulder and van’t Hof 22 ). In addition, the cohort assessed in the current study differs from most observational studies in that the majority of previous studies have examined cohorts based in either low-resource settings or in subgroups of low socioeconomic status( Reference Lozoff, Brittenham and Wolf 20 , Reference Shafir, Angulo-Barroso and Jing 23 ). Only two studies in Iceland( Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson 24 , Reference Thorisdottir, Gunnarsdottir and Palsson 25 ) and an earlier study from the UK( Reference Sherriff, Emond and Bell 26 ) have investigated associations in young children in high-resource settings similar to the current study. In low-resource settings, the presence of confounding factors which may also impact neurodevelopment including nutritional deprivation and malnourishment can make it difficult to identify Fe deficiency as the sole cause of neurodevelopmental alterations in such populations.
Interestingly, microcytosis indicated by a low MCV at 2 years was associated with significantly lower BSID-Ш cognitive scores, even following adjustment for socioeconomic factors. Linear associations between MCV and behaviour and language/expression have been observed previously( Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson 24 , Reference Johnson, Winkleby and Boyce 27 , Reference Ruff, Markowitz and Bijur 28 ), however, to our knowledge, this is the first study to show an adverse effect of microcytosis on cognition. Microcytosis is characterised by erythrocytes that are smaller than normal. In healthy populations, this presentation is often due to a decreased availability and supply of Fe resulting in a decreased production of Hb, although other causes of microcytosis include anaemia of chronic disease, congenital thalassaemias and sideroblastic anaemias( Reference DeLoughery 29 ). However, the microcytosis observed in this study is almost certainly Fe deficiency related. Our cohort was a healthy birth cohort population, with normal CRP levels making chronic inflammation or disease unlikely. Our ethnic diversity is low, 99 % of our cohort were white Caucasian, with no children of African or Mediterranean decent, making thalassaemia an unlikely cause. Although we cannot fully rule out lead poisoning as a cause of acquired sideroblastic anaemia, no sideroblasts were reported on the manual blood films which accompanied our full blood count analysis.
The finding that microcytosis was associated with reduced cognitive outcomes in this low risk population is an important one. It suggests that a low MCV may be a more specific marker of functional Fe deficiency than very low serum ferritin concentrations, which are susceptible to much variation( Reference Domellof, Braegger and Campoy 30 ). Over two-thirds of those with microcytosis presented with normal serum ferritin concentrations (≥12 µg/l), therefore the use of ferritin alone as a diagnostic tool would have resulted in a number of children at risk of poorer cognitive outcomes being undetected. Furthermore, despite the small numbers with cognitive delay in this cohort, it is striking to note that the majority of those with delay had microcytosis. This finding emphasises the need for further consideration of the diagnostic criteria for Fe deficiency in young children, with a need to assess Fe status using multiple haematological indices to ensure no children at risk of physiological or developmental consequences are inadvertently undiagnosed.
In this exploratory analysis, we calculated that a ferritin concentration threshold of 18·4 µg/l was predictive of microcytosis in this cohort, albeit in a small group of children. We observed that children with ferritin concentrations below 18·5 µg/l had significantly lower BSID-Ш cognitive scores at 2 years, raising the possibility that the current commonly used WHO serum ferritin cut-off for Fe deficiency of 12 µg/l may need re-evaluation. Based on our preliminary findings, 18·5 µg/l may be considered a candidate threshold to identify those at risk of adverse clinical outcomes or suboptimal development, especially in high-resource settings, although, further research with adequately powered validation studies are urgently required to ascertain this. This is the first study to show that in a healthy population where very low ferritin concentrations (Fe deficiency) were uncommon, there was still a significant adverse consequence of being in the lower end of the currently defined normal range for ferritin concentrations. Longitudinal studies have previously shown a persistent effect of Fe deficiency at this stage( Reference Lozoff, Smith and Kaciroti 10 ), however, the long-term consequences of this borderline low Fe status at this crucial phase of brain development is yet to be elucidated.
In addition, ferritin concentrations at 2 years were positively associated with BSID-Ш fine motor scores, in accordance with previous studies( Reference Thorisdottir, Gunnarsdottir and Palsson 25 , Reference Shafir, Angulo-Barroso and Calatroni 31 ). A number of animal studies have suggested altered oligodendrocyte function due to Fe deficiency, resulting in impaired myelination of neurons, affecting motor function. Alterations in monoaminergic pathways, in particular in dopamine metabolism have also been suggested to impact motor development in early childhood( Reference Doom and Georgieff 32 ).
The obligate reliance on global developmental assessments at this age is a limitation in this area of research. Global developmental assessments, including the BSID-Ш, have been criticised for being too broad and not sensitive enough to identify specific cognitive processes that are affected by nutritional factors and do not correlate highly with later tests of intelligence in school-age children( Reference Wainwright and Colombo 33 , Reference Canals, Hernandez-Martinez and Esparo 34 ). More specific measures of cognitive tasks, such as tests of memory and learning at school-age may be of more importance to investigating the more subtle effects of Fe deficiency and borderline low Fe status( Reference Wainwright and Colombo 33 ).
A potential limitation of this study is that the analysis is cross-sectional in design, although it is important to note that prospectively collected maternal and early life data were used in the analysis, thereby enabling deeper interpretation of the data than would be possible with a cross-sectional survey. The prospective design of the Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study, with its multidisciplinary team, with both validated nutritional and neurodevelopmental assessments performed by experts in their field are strengths of this study. Our secondary analysis of the nested subgroup within the cohort is limited by the small sample size included, although the eighty-seven children included were representative of the entire BASELINE cohort (see Table 1) and this sample size is comparable to previous observational studies in this field( Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson 24 , Reference Thorisdottir, Gunnarsdottir and Palsson 25 ). However, adequately powered studies are required to further investigate and expand on our findings in this study with the aim to identify the best measures of Fe status in young children. The exploration of associations between Fe deficiency/Fe deficiency anaemia and developmental outcomes were somewhat limited by the small number of children with either condition, however this is reflective of the high-resource population studied and generalisable to other similar low risk, healthy populations. Despite the overall normal developmental profile of the group, we still found a significant functional effect of microcytosis and borderline normal/low Fe status.
Although the public health threat associated with Fe deficiency has diminished in many low risk, high-resource settings, the current study highlights that strategies aimed at preventing Fe deficiency, but also low or suboptimal Fe status in young children in such settings are still pertinent. Despite a low prevalence of Fe deficiency using current diagnostic criteria in this apparently healthy cohort, microcytosis was still associated with lower cognitive outcomes at 2 years. This study further emphasises the need for re-consideration and evaluation of the diagnostic criteria currently used to diagnose Fe deficiency and Fe deficiency anaemia in infants and young children. Further research is warranted to identify the most suitable biomarkers and thresholds to ensure no children at risk of adverse clinical or developmental consequences are inadvertently undiagnosed and untreated.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank the participants and their families for their continued support and both the SCOPE Ireland and Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study research teams.
A grant from the National Children’s Research Centre to D. M. M., J. O’B. H., A. D. I., L. C. K. and M. E. K. is the primary funding source for the Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study. Additional support came from grants from the UK Food Standards Agency to J. O’B. H. and A. D. I. and from Danone Nutricia Early Life Nutrition to M. E. K. and D. M. M. The SCOPE Ireland Study was funded by a grant from the Health Research Board of Ireland (CSA 02/2007) to L. C. K. The INFANT Research Centre is Science Foundation Ireland funded (grant no. 12/RC/2272). No funding agencies had any role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.
E. K. M. carried out data collection, database construction and data analysis and drafted the first and subsequent drafts of the manuscript. D. M. M. is the overall Principal Investigator (PI) of the Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study and planned the study design, aided in data analysis and manuscript preparation. M. E. K. is co-PI and specialist nutrition lead of the Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study and aided in study governance, design and manuscript preparation. G. H. and C. A. carried out data collection and database construction. L. C. K., J. O’B. H. and A. D. I. are co-PI of the Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study and aided in study governance and edited the final manuscript. L. C. K. is the PI of the SCOPE Ireland pregnancy study. All authors reviewed and approved the final submission.
The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114517001945