During the consumption of a meal, aroma molecules reach the olfactory epithelium retro-nasally (perceived as arising from the mouth)(Reference Murphy, Cain and Bartoshuk1, Reference Rozin2). The brain response, i.e. neural brain activation, to a retro-nasally sensed food odour signals the perception of food and is suggested to be related to satiation(Reference Small, Gerber and Mak3). The extent of retro-nasal aroma release during consumption depends on both product (i.e. the physical structure of a food, which has an influence on the breakdown and oral processing in the mouth(Reference Linforth, Baek and Taylor4–Reference van Ruth, de Witte and Uriarte7)) and interpersonal differences(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8). Interpersonal differences are factors that are likely to be uncontrolled by a person, e.g. saliva production, nasal anatomy and oral processing habits(Reference Brown, Dauchel and Wakeling9–Reference Pionnier, Chabanet and Mioche13). In addition to product and (uncontrolled) interpersonal features, bite size and duration of oral processing may also be of importance for the extent of retro-nasal aroma release.
Recently, Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu14, Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15) investigated the effect of bite size (i.e. either free or fixed (5 and 15 g)) and duration of oral processing (i.e. either free or fixed (3 and 9 s)) for a semi-liquid food product (i.e. dark chocolate-flavoured custard) on satiation/ad libitum food intake. In the fixed eating conditions, subjects consumed significantly less when bite sizes were small (5 g for both short and longer duration of oral processing) than when bite sizes were large (15 g for both short and longer duration of oral processing (bite size effect P < 0·05) and when duration of oral processing was 9 s compared with 3 s (oral processing time effect P < 0·05). Ad libitum consumption in the free eating condition (without any chewing protocol) was comparable to the ad libitum consumption with large bite sizes. According to Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15), the lower ad libitum food intake when consuming the custard in small bite sizes or with a longer duration of oral processing was probably explained by a longer oro-sensory exposure from the dark chocolate-flavoured custard to the sensory tactile and taste receptors in the oral cavity(Reference Zijlstra, Mars and de Wijk16). Longer oro-sensory exposure may accelerate sensory satiation, which in turn may lead to earlier meal termination(Reference Hetherington, Rolls and Burley17, Reference Hetherington and Boyland18).
In this context, differences in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release may also be (partly) responsible for this effect, stimulating olfactory receptors. We hypothesise that differences in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release during consumption may be one of the reasons that people vary in their satiation characteristics, due to differences in perceived intensity, duration or quality of retro-nasal aroma stimulation(Reference Ruijschop19). In a recent study, we were able to show that a retro-nasally delivered aroma stimulus was capable of inducing satiation(Reference Ruijschop, Boelrijk and de Ru20). Fig. 1 illustrates some of the features that are suggested to contributing to the extent of retro-nasal aroma stimulation and, subsequently, sensory satiation, from both a person and food product point of view(Reference Ruijschop, Boelrijk and de Graaf21).
Although the extent of retro-nasal aroma release appears to be subject specific, food product properties can be tailored in such a way that these can lead to a higher quality and/or quantity of retro-nasal aroma stimulation. This in turn provokes enhanced feelings of satiation and, ultimately, may contribute to a decrease in food intake.
The aim of the present study was, therefore, to provide an additional, possibly complementary, explanation for the ad libitum intake results obtained by Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15) from a retro-nasal aroma release perspective. In addition, the results from the free eating condition served to determine whether interpersonal differences in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release can be linked to interpersonal differences in ad libitum food intake, as measured by Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15) and as previously reported(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8).
Subjects and methods
Subjects
Twenty-one healthy subjects (nine men and twelve women) aged 27 (sd 9) years living in Ede, the Netherlands, and surroundings participated in the present study. Initially, subjects were recruited from the subject population who participated in the study of Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu14, Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15). To prevent a lack of statistical power, nine subjects were recruited in-house. Subjects did not have any previous experience with atmospheric pressure chemical ionisation-MS (APcI-MS) measurements. The subjects were of normal weight, with a BMI varying between 19 and 25 kg/m2. BMI was calculated as body weight (kg) divided by height (m) squared. The present study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the ethics committee of Wageningen University. Subjects were fully informed about the course of the APcI-MS measurement and they gave their written, informed consent. To prevent response-bias, the participants were given no information about the hypothesis and nature of the predictions of the experiment. They were only aware of the fact that the study was about in vivo retro-nasal aroma release assessment.
Food product
Subjects consumed dark chocolate-flavoured custard (Royal FrieslandCampina, Deventer, the Netherlands). The physical structure of the custard can be characterised as semi-liquid with a viscosity of 43 (sd 11) s, as measured with a Posthumus funnel.
To check for a clear aroma signal in the APcI-MS measurement, the dark chocolate-flavoured custard was spiked with methyl acetate. Methyl acetate (Sigma-Aldrich Chemie GmbH, Steinheim, Germany) was applied as a volatile marker aroma molecule, which was rapidly and easily released from the food product matrix during consumption. The sensory quality of methyl acetate can be described as fruity and sweet. In finished consumer products, methyl acetate concentrations vary from 0·1 to 30 mg/kg(22). Methyl acetate solution (0·1 ml) with a concentration of 100 g/kg, i.e. diluted ten fold in propylene glycol (private label from a local pharmacy in Ede, the Netherlands), was added to 1 litre dark chocolate-flavoured custard following a standardised procedure of ten times gently stirring. The methyl acetate concentration in the dark chocolate-flavoured custard was 10 mg/kg. The dark chocolate-flavoured custard product was left overnight at 4°C to allow a good equilibration of the aroma in the product.
The viscosity of the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product was not affected by the addition of methyl acetate. Moreover, there were no sensory differences perceived between the dark chocolate-flavoured custards with or without addition of methyl acetate (P = 0·12). This was evaluated by an in-house trained sensory panel (eleven panellists), who performed a triangle test.
Measurement of the extent of retro-nasal aroma release with atmospheric pressure chemical ionisation-MS technology
To detect the aroma compounds with the highest response in retro-nasal aroma release, the dynamic headspace of the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product was measured. Aroma compounds in the air released from the artificial mouth(Reference van Ruth, Roozen, Cozijnsen, van Maarse, Taylor and Linforth23) were monitored by online sampling by an APcI-gas phase analyser attached to a VG Quattro II mass spectrometer (MS-Nose; Micromass UK Limited, Manchester, UK)(Reference Taylor, Linforth and Harvey24–Reference Weel, Boelrijk and Burger27). Compounds were ionised by a 3·0 kV discharge (source and probe temperature were 80°C) and scanned for m/z 50–250. m/z Values with the highest response were selected, i.e. m/z 69, 75 and 87. Both m/z 69 and 87 were originally present in the dark chocolate-flavoured custard, while m/z 75 represented the marker aroma molecule, methyl acetate. In vivo retro-nasal aroma release was assessed in exhaled breath of the subjects. Subjects breathed in and out through the nose. One nostril was placed over a small disposable plastic tube, allowing them to breathe and eat normally. Aroma compounds in the air released from the breath of subjects were monitored by online sampling of part of the exhaled air directly into the APcI-MS. The air was sampled (75 ml/min) through a capillary tube (0·53 mm internal diameter, heated to 100°C). The compounds were monitored in selected ion mode (0·08 s dwell on each ion). The cone voltage used was 20 V.
Acetone, present in human breath, was measured at m/z 59 (19 V) as an indicator of the breathing pattern(Reference Weel, Boelrijk and Alting28). The area of the resulting breath peaks in the aroma signal was taken as a measure of in vivo retro-nasal aroma release. Different parameters could be extracted from each individual retro-nasal aroma release curve, characterising the extent of retro-nasal aroma release, i.e. time at which maximal intensity occurs (T max), time at which half of the maximal intensity occurs, before reaching T max (T − 1/2), time at which half of the maximal intensity occurs, after reaching T max (T +1/2), maximum intensity (I max) and area under curve (AUC) (Fig. 2). Since we were interested in comparative retro-nasal aroma release between subjects, expression of the extent of retro-nasal aroma release in arbitrary units (A.U.) was sufficient to analyse the differences(Reference Taylor, Linforth and Harvey24).
Experimental design
In vivo retro-nasal aroma release was assessed in exhaled breath of the twenty-one subjects for the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product in quintuplicate according to a specific protocol established to determine the effect of bite size and oral processing time before swallowing on the extent of retro-nasal aroma release(Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu14, Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15). To this end, subjects were exposed to five different eating conditions in a cross-over design, in which bite size was either small (5 g) or large (15 g), and oral processing time before swallowing was either short (3 s) or long (9 s), or subjects were free to use their own natural eating habits, including free bite size and free oral processing time before swallowing. Treatment order for the fixed eating conditions was counterbalanced across both test days and subjects. In the free eating condition, subjects consumed five times one mouthful of dark chocolate-flavoured custard product by means of a soup spoon without any chewing protocol. It has to be noticed that the size of the soup spoon may have affected the bite size in the free eating condition. In the fixed eating conditions, subjects were instructed to put the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product (either 5 or 15 g, presented in a syringe) in the mouth, to close the mouth and to actively move the custard product in the mouth with the tongue for either 3 or 9 s, thereby simulating oral processing in the mouth, and then to swallow the entire bolus. After consumption, the syringe was weighed to determine the precise bite size of custard product per serving (Mettler-Toledo balance, Greifensee, Switzerland).
Between the different eating conditions, the mouth was rinsed with water. Blank experiments with water were recorded before consuming the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product in the different eating conditions, following the same protocol as for the first fixed eating condition. These experiments served as a baseline measurement. The protocol for either the free or the fixed eating conditions is represented schematically in Fig. 3. During the measurements, the subjects were closely observed in order to verify their compliance with the protocol. Fig. 4 illustrates a subject's retro-nasal aroma release curve for the different eating conditions.
Measurement of ad libitum food intake
For a subset of twelve subjects (five men and seven women; aged 21 (sd 3) years; BMI 22 (sd 1) kg/m2), showing no dietary restraint (i.e. men: score ≤ 2·89, women: score ≤ 3·40 on Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire), ad libitum food intake data of the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product was available from a study of Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu14, Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15). In the present study, the effect of bite size and oral processing time for dark chocolate-flavoured custard on satiation/ad libitum food intake was determined. To assess whether interpersonal differences in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release can be linked to interpersonal differences in ad libitum food intake, the amount of dark chocolate-flavoured custard consumed ad libitum in the free eating condition in the satiation experiment performed by Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15) was related to the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed for these subjects in the free eating condition.
Data analysis
The quintuplicate measurements of the characteristic retro-nasal aroma release parameters (T max, I max and AUC) for each subject per eating condition were averaged. This is allowed, since subjects are reproducible in their extent of retro-nasal aroma release during consumption of a specific food product(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8, Reference Buettner, Beer and Hanning10). Due to the lower signal-to-noise ratio for the custard-specific m/z values 69 and 87 compared with the marker aroma molecule with m/z value 75, the background noise, as measured with the blank experiments, was subtracted from the measurements with the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product in the different eating conditions per subject for all m/z values.
All data are presented as means with their standard errors. The different characteristic retro-nasal aroma release parameters were compared between the five different eating conditions per bite size or per gram consumed using the general linear model procedure for repeated measures, with participant and type of eating condition as independent variables. Least squares means were used for post hoc comparisons. Multiple regression analysis (f(x)) was performed for the characteristic retro-nasal aroma release parameters (y), and bite size (x) and oral processing time before swallowing (x).
Additionally, the extent of retro-nasal aroma release (y) was linked to subject characteristics, such as age (x) and BMI (x), and ad libitum food intake (x), applying simple regression analysis (f(x)). For all the data analyses, the statistical packages Statistical Analysis Systems (release 9.1; SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC, USA) were used. P-values < 0·05 were considered statistically significant.
Results
Bite size differences affecting the extent of retro-nasal aroma release
Bite size was 13·6 (sd 0·4) g per mouthful (range 6·7–18·2 g) in the free eating condition.
Consumption of a small v. large bite size
In the fixed eating conditions, subjects consumed 5·7 (sd 0·4) and 15·7 (sd 0·4) g for the small and large bite size, respectively. Subjects differed in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release regarding intensity (I max) and AUC of the retro-nasal aroma release profile during the consumption of either small or large bite sizes of dark chocolate-flavoured custard product in the fixed eating conditions (F(1,41) = 16·88, P < 0·01 and F(1,41) = 27·50, P < 0·01 for I max and AUC, respectively). As illustrated in Fig. 5, small bite sizes resulted in a significantly less intense retro-nasal aroma release characteristic for dark chocolate-flavoured custard product (Fig. 5(a); mean I max (means with their standard errors): 3·62E+05 ± 2·77E+04 A.U. and Fig. 5(b); mean AUC (means with their standard errors): 2·51E+06 ± 1·73E+05 A.U.) compared with large bite sizes (Fig. 5(a); mean I max (means with their standard errors): 5·23E+05 ± 2·77E+04 A.U., and Fig. 5(b); mean AUC (means with their standard errors): 3·79E+06 ± 1·73E+05 A.U.). Bite size in the free eating condition showed comparable retro-nasal aroma release characteristics to small bite sizes for AUC and to large bite sizes for I max in the fixed eating conditions (Fig. 5(a); mean I max (means with their standard errors): 4·82E+05 ± 3·54E+04 A.U., and Fig. 5(b); mean AUC (means with their standard errors): 2·75E+06 ± 2·07E+05 A.U.).
Consumption of a fixed amount in a small or large bite size
By expressing the effect of bite size on the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed, a different view of the results was obtained. As shown in Fig. 6, small bite sizes resulted in a significantly more intense retro-nasal aroma release characteristic for dark chocolate-flavoured custard product per gram consumed (Fig. 6(a); mean I max (means with their standard errors): 6·34E+04 ± 3·10E+03 A.U./g consumed, and Fig. 6(b); mean AUC (means with their standard errors): 4·38E+05 ± 1·77E+04 A.U./g consumed) compared with large bite sizes (Fig. 6(a); mean Imax (means with their standard errors): 3·33E+04 ± 3·10E+03 A.U./g consumed, and Fig. 6(b); mean AUC (means with their standard errors): 2·40E+05 ± 1·77E+04 A.U./g consumed) (F(1,41) = 47·33, P < 0·01 and F(1,41) = 62·01, P < 0·01 for I max and AUC, respectively). There is almost a difference by a factor of two in retro-nasal aroma release I max and AUC per gram consumed between the small and large bite sizes. Bite size in the free eating condition showed comparable retro-nasal aroma release characteristics per gram consumed to the large bite sizes in the fixed eating conditions (Fig. 6(a); mean I max (means with their standard errors): 4·12E+04 ± 4·56E+03 A.U./g consumed, and Fig. 6(b); mean AUC (means with their standard errors): 2·42E+05 ± 2·58E+04 A.U./g consumed). When the subjects were free to use their own natural eating habits, they consumed large bite sizes according to their extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed.
Oral processing time differences affecting the extent of retro-nasal aroma release
The morphology of the retro-nasal aroma release profile (in particular explained by T max, i.e. time at which I max occurred) was different between the short (3 s) and longer (9 s) duration of oral processing for a fixed bite size in the different fixed eating conditions (Fig. 7; mean T max (means with their standard errors) for the small bite size: 0·16 (sem 0·01) and 0·24 (sem 0·01) min for the short and longer duration of oral processing, respectively; and mean T max (means with their standard errors) for the large bite size: 0·16 (sem 0·01) and 0·27 (sem 0·01) min for the short and longer duration of oral processing, respectively). In compliance with the protocol, subjects swallowed later when they were exposed to the fixed eating condition of a given bite size and a longer duration of oral processing. Hence, a significant delay in T max was observed compared with the short duration of oral processing (P < 0·01). In the free eating condition, T max was 0·11 (sem 0·01) min (range 0·05–0·24 min). Although significantly different from the fixed eating conditions (P < 0·01), T max in the free eating condition approximated the short duration of oral processing more than the longer duration of oral processing.
Short v. long duration of oral processing during consumption of different bite sizes
Apart from differences in bite size affecting the extent of retro-nasal aroma release, differences in duration of oral processing time were also thought to be important for the extent of retro-nasal aroma release. A longer duration of oral processing resulted in a significantly higher extent of retro-nasal aroma release (AUC) for dark chocolate-flavoured custard product in the fixed eating conditions (Fig. 5(b); 2·98E+06 ± 1·13E+05 A.U. and 3·31E+06 ± 1·13E+05 A.U. for the short and longer duration of oral processing, respectively) (F(1,41) = 4·40, P = 0·04). The duration of oral processing in the free eating condition showed comparable retro-nasal aroma release characteristics to small bite sizes with both short and longer duration of oral processing for AUC and to large bite sizes with both short and longer duration of oral processing for I max in the fixed eating conditions.
Short v. long duration of oral processing during consumption of a fixed amount
During the consumption of a fixed amount of dark chocolate-flavoured custard product, a longer duration of oral processing tended to result in a higher cumulative extent of retro-nasal aroma release (AUC) for dark chocolate-flavoured custard product per gram consumed in the fixed eating conditions (Fig. 6(b); 3·22E+05 ± 1·40E+04 A.U./g consumed and 3·57E+05 ± 1·40E+04 A.U./g consumed for the short and longer duration of oral processing, respectively) (F(1,41) = 3·16, P = 0·08). The duration of oral processing in the free eating condition showed comparable retro-nasal aroma release characteristics (AUC and I max) per gram consumed to both short and longer duration of oral processing of large bite sizes. The differences in bite size affected the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed significantly compared with the difference in duration of oral processing in the fixed eating conditions (y = − 1·92E+04x+5·44E+05, R 2 0·19, P < 0·01 and y = 5·85E+03x+3·04E+05, R 2 0·01, P = 0·47 for the effect of bite size (x) and duration of oral processing (x) on the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed (AUC; y), respectively). This indicates that the bite size effect presumably overwhelmed the effect of duration of oral processing. Therefore, it is likely that in the free eating condition, bite size accounted for the present result rather than the duration of oral processing.
Discussion
Using APcI-MS technology, in vivo retro-nasal aroma release was assessed for twenty-one subjects consuming dark chocolate-flavoured custard in either a free or a fixed eating condition.
Selected aroma compounds detected in the range of m/z 20–250, i.e. aroma compounds with the highest response, showed comparable retro-nasal aroma release profiles for the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product. From previous studies, it is known that aroma release data obtained for a single aroma compound (one specific m/z value) are a good predictor for the relative release of other aroma compounds (other m/z values)(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8). Therefore, the same relative retro-nasal aroma release profile was measured, irrespective of the m/z value taken. However, it appeared that the signal-to-noise ratio for the custard-specific m/z values of 69 and 87 was lower compared with the marker aroma molecule with m/z 75. The custard-specific m/z values provided a less clear aroma signal in the APcI-MS measurements compared with the marker m/z value. Although the sensory detection thresholds of both custard-specific aroma molecules were probably low enough to be detected by a human nose via the retro-nasal route, the ‘MS-Nose’ might be less sensitive in detecting the custard-specific aroma molecules in a concentration as applied in dark chocolate-flavoured custard. After baseline correction, m/z 75 had the highest response for the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product and was selected for the in vivo retro-nasal aroma release measurements.
The extent of retro-nasal aroma release from small bite sizes of dark chocolate-flavoured custard product was significantly lower compared with large bite sizes (Fig. 5). However, for a fixed amount of dark chocolate-flavoured custard product, consumption in numerous small bite sizes resulted in a significantly higher cumulative extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed compared with a smaller number of, but larger, bite sizes (Fig. 6). These bite size effects were in line with our expectations.
When subjects consumed large bite sizes, they were probably exposed to higher quantities of aroma molecules, because in absolute value, a larger bite size contains more aroma molecules than a smaller bite size. However, it should be mentioned that from the total amount of aroma molecules present in food, only a small fraction of the aroma is released into the nasal cavity during oral processing and swallowing. The majority of aroma molecules is swallowed together with the food matrix into the stomach and is no longer available for retro-nasal aroma perception(Reference Buettner and Schieberle29). Besides, large bite sizes presumably led to more frequent swallowing. Retro-nasal aroma stimulation is mainly related to the event of swallowing, when a small volume of air is exhaled immediately after swallowing, the so-called ‘swallow-breath’. It is assumed that this pulse should contain the major part of food volatiles that have been released from the food product before swallowing and should therefore elicit a retro-nasal aroma pulse(Reference Buettner and Schieberle29, Reference Land, McGorrin and Leland30). Large bite sizes thus yielded a higher extent of retro-nasal aroma release; but per larger bite also led to a higher food intake compared with small bite sizes.
Alternatively, for the same amount of food intake, subjects had a higher cumulative extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram if they consumed the custard in a multiple of small bite sizes compared with a smaller number of large bite sizes (Fig. 6). For example, when subjects consumed a fixed amount of 15 g of dark chocolate-flavoured custard product in either one large or three small bite sizes, the multiple small bite sizes were hypothesised to evoke substantial oral processing per gram consumed and an increase in transit time in the oral cavity(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8). Accordingly, the three multiple small bite sizes of 5 g of dark chocolate-flavoured custard product were expected to lead to a higher cumulative retro-nasal aroma stimulation compared with the single large bite size of 15 g.
In addition to the bite size effect, a longer duration of oral processing before swallowing tended to result in a higher cumulative extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed compared with a short duration of oral processing before swallowing (Fig. 6). A longer duration of oral processing was thought to evoke an increase in transit time in the oral cavity and thereby result in a higher extent of retro-nasal aroma release. Besides, during a longer duration of oral processing, the temperature of the dark chocolate-flavoured custard product in the mouth may further increase towards body temperature and probably more surface of the custard product is exposed to air, both resulting in a higher quantitative release of aroma molecules from the custard matrix.
The effect of oral processing time was less strong than the effect of the bite size. With dark chocolate-flavoured custard as the experimental food product, this result was not unexpected. The morphology of the retro-nasal aroma release curve for a custard product can be described as a relatively short and spiked retro-nasal aroma release pattern(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8, Reference Weel, Boelrijk and Burger26). The more liquid food products have a relatively short transit time in the oral cavity and therefore hardly any oral processing is needed to swallow them. In the present study, subjects actively moved the custard product in the mouth with the tongue for either 3 or 9 s, thereby simulating oral processing in the mouth. However, the event of swallowing was probably not affected by varying the duration of oral processing. Therefore, the difference in duration of oral processing may not evoke a significant difference in retro-nasal aroma stimulation. The observed differences in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release between the short and longer duration of oral processing are likely to be due to intermittent opening of the connection of the oral cavity to the naso- and dorsal oropharynx. During the simulation of oral processing in the mouth, the velum was assumed to be open, which enabled air to proceed into the nasal cavity via the retro-nasal route through the velopharyngeal portal(Reference Buettner, Beer and Hanning10). To strengthen this physiological effect, increasing the difference in duration between the short and longer duration of oral processing may increase the amount of air which reaches the nasal cavity via the retro-nasal route during an increased longer duration of oral processing. Alternatively, exposing subjects to a fixed protocol for swallowing, meaning that they have to swallow more frequently during the longer duration of oral processing compared with the short duration of oral processing, may increase the effect of oral processing time. It may even be more efficient than a longer duration of oral processing with respect to the extent of retro-nasal aroma release. Moreover, the choice of a more solid food product as the experimental food product may strengthen the impact of a longer duration of oral processing on the extent of retro-nasal aroma release. Solid food products require considerable oral processing and swallowing, due to their firmer texture(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8, Reference Buettner, Beer and Hanning10). Since aroma pulses are generated along with each swallow-breath, solid food products generate a longer, more pronounced retro-nasal aroma release than liquid food products(Reference Ruijschop, Boelrijk and de Ru20).
In the present study, an interaction effect of bite size and duration of oral processing was not observed.
To summarise, small bite sizes contributed significantly to a higher cumulative extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram, whereas a longer duration of oral processing tended to result in a higher extent of retro-nasal aroma release during consumption of a fixed amount. However, the effect of oral processing time may be significant with a more solid food product.
As reported by Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu14, Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15), consumption in small bite sizes or with a longer duration of oral processing resulted in a lower ad libitum food intake. The results of the present study provided an additional, possibly complementary, explanation for the ad libitum intake results obtained by Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15) from a retro-nasal aroma release perspective, stimulating olfactory receptors. Differences in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release may thus be (partly) responsible for the decrease in food intake in certain eating conditions.
When the subjects were free to use their own natural eating habits, including free bite size and free oral processing time before swallowing, the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed reflected the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed from large bite sizes, while duration of oral processing was more difficult to estimate. T max in the free eating condition approximated the short duration of oral processing more than the longer duration of oral processing (Fig. 7). The physical structure of the custard product was probably responsible for this result. Generally, hardly any oral processing is needed under natural eating conditions to swallow (semi)-liquid food products.
In the present study, for the subset of twelve subjects, no significant relationship was observed between the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed in the free eating condition and the amount of dark chocolate-flavoured custard consumed ad libitum in the free eating condition in the satiation experiment done by Zijlstra et al. (Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars15). Moreover, the subject characteristics, such as sex, age and BMI, did not correlate with the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed. This result was not unexpected. The interpersonal differences in the extent of retro-nasal aroma release per gram consumed, which were hypothesised to be linked to the interpersonal differences in ad libitum food intake(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8), were likely to be too small within the subset of twelve subjects. Subjects included were rather homogeneous, i.e. normal weight and of similar age. A follow-up study with a larger subject population, which is more segmented with respect to age and BMI, is needed to demonstrate a significant effect of the extent of retro-nasal aroma release on ad libitum food intake(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs8). However, this is usually conflicting with the subject requirements for satiation experiments, i.e. homogeneous.
The concept that small bite sizes or a longer duration of oral processing may lead to a higher cumulative retro-nasal aroma stimulation indicates that meal termination can be accelerated by increasing the extent of retro-nasal aroma release and, subsequently, the satiation. Bite size and duration of oral processing are features which may be subjected to behavioural changes as a result of, for instance, mindful eating(Reference Wansink31). In this context, one piece of advice from dietitians when participating in a weight loss programme is to consume small bite sizes and to chew properly (e.g. ten times) on each bite before swallowing. The development of long chewable food structures that evoke substantial oral processing and an increase in transit time in the oral cavity or a reduction in bite size by tailored packaging may be product development suggestions that support the ‘right’ oral processing behaviour. These applications may lead to a higher extent of retro-nasal aroma release and sensory stimulation, which in turn may affect satiation and food intake behaviour.
Acknowledgements
We acknowledge Carina Ponne, PhD (Royal FrieslandCampina), for providing the dark chocolate-flavoured custard. R. M. A. J. R. designed the experiment, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. N. Z. performed subject recruitment and designed the experiment. A. E. M. B. provided significant advice on the study design, data analysis and helped to prepare the manuscript. A. D. collected the data and helped to analyse the data. M. J. M. B. reviewed the manuscript. C. d. G. and M. S. W.-P. provided significant advice with the design of the experiment, data analysis and writing of the manuscript. None of the authors had a personal or financial conflict of interest.