Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:15:24.582Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of different regimens of early malnutrition on behavioural development and adult avoidance learning in Swiss white mice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2008

Maureen Bush
Affiliation:
Research Department, Nestlé Products Technical Assistance, La Tour-de-Peliz, Switzerland
P. D. Leathwood
Affiliation:
Research Department, Nestlé Products Technical Assistance, La Tour-de-Peliz, Switzerland
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

1. The effects of perinatal malnutrition on behavioural development and adult shuttle-box avoidance performance were studied in Swiss white mice.

2. Mice were malnourished (a) from the 7th day of gestation until birth, (b) from birth until weaning, or (c) during both gestation and the sucking period.

3. Pups born of protein-restricted mothers had reduced birth weights, retarded development and poor adult avoidance performance, even if reared from birth by well-nourished mothers.

4. Postnatal malnutrition, induced either by restricting maternal diet or by rearing in large litters, retarded development during the second half of lactation and lowered subsequent adult avoidance performance.

Type
General Nutrition
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 1975

References

Blackwell, B.-N., Blackwell, R. Q., Yu, T. T. S., Weng, Y. & Chow, B. F. (1969). J. Nutr. 97, 79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bovet, D., Bovet-Nitti, F. & Oliverio, A. (1968). Brain Res. 10, 168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowley, J. J. & Griesel, R. D. (1966). Anim. Behav. 14, 506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobbing, J. (1968). In Applied Neurochemistry p. 287 [Davison, A. N. and Dobbing, J., editors]Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fox, W. M. (1965). Anim. Behav. 13, 234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawk, P. B. & Oser, B. (1931). Science, N.Y. 74, 369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lát, J., Widdowson, E. M. & McCance, R. A. (1960). Proc. R. Soc. B153, 347.Google Scholar
Leathwood, P. D., Bush, M. S., Berent, C. D. & Mauron, J. (1974). Life Sci. 14, 157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, S. & Mullins, R. F. (1966). Science, N.Y. 152, 1585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitsky, D. A. & Barnes, R. H. (1970). Nature, Lond. 225, 468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolis, F. L. (1969). J. Neurochem. 16, 447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauron, J. & Mottu, F. (1962). J. agric. Fd Chem. 10, 512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajalakshmi, R. & Ramakrishnan, C. V. (1972). Wld Rev. Nutr. Diet. 15, 35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seitz, P. F. D. (1954). Am. J. Psychiat. 110, 916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioural Sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Simonson, M., Sherwin, R. W., Anilane, J. K., Yu, W. Y. & Chow, B. F. (1968). J. Nutr. 98, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonson, M., Stephan, J. K., Hanson, H. M. & Chow, B. F. (1971). J. Nutr. 101, 331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smart, J. L. & Dobbing, J. (1971 a). Brain Res. 28, 85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smart, J. L. & Dobbing, J. (1971 b). Brain Res. 33, 303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smart, J. L., Dobbing, J., Adlard, B. P. F., Lynch, A. & Sands, J. (1973). J. Nutr. 103, 1327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widdowson, E. M. & McCance, R. A. (1963). Proc. R. Soc. B158, 329.Google Scholar
Winick, M. (1970). Fedn Proc. Fedn Am. Socs exp. Biol. 29, 1510.Google Scholar