Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T07:11:16.406Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The degree of saturation of fatty acids influences post-ingestive satiety*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 March 2007

Helen J. Delargy
Affiliation:
BioPsychology Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT, UK
Justine Brockman
Affiliation:
BioPsychology Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT, UK
Fiona C. Smith
Affiliation:
BioPsychology Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT, UK
John E. Blundell
Affiliation:
BioPsychology Group, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Two studies were designed to compare the effects on post-ingestive satiety of manipulating the degree of saturation of fatty acids, at a fixed chain length (18 C atoms), in a fixed energy (5·68 MJ for males; 3·97 MJ for females), high-fat (55 % energy) lunch meal. Two different groups of twenty subjects (ten males and ten females) took part in each study. All forty subjects were of normal weight and aged between 18 and 36 years. Study 1 compared the effects of fat A (oleic blend, high in monounsaturated fatty acids (MUFA)) with those of fat B (linoleic blend, high in polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA)) and fat C (stearic–oleic blend, high in saturated fatty acids (SFA)). Study 2, which was designed to confirm and extend the findings of Study 1, compared the effects of fats A, B and C with those of fat D (a linoleic–oleic blend). Energy and nutrient intakes were monitored for the rest of the day and for the following day. Profiles of hunger, fullness and other sensations were monitored by continuous tracking and end-of-day questionnaires. In each meal the fat content was exclusively composed of one particular type (A, B, C or D) and was divided equally between the main course and dessert. Study 1 revealed a significant effect of fat type (degree of saturation) on intake of nutrients at the following (dinner) meal (smallest F[2,36] 3·38, P < 0·05), on post-ingestive ratings of motivation to eat (smallest F[2,36] 4·18, P = 0·02) and on energy intake over the whole test day (F[2,36] 3·39, P < 0·01). Subjects consumed significantly more energy after consumption of the lunch containing fat A than after the lunches containing fats B or C and there was a trend for these effects to continue into the second day. In Study 2, fat C produced more similar effects on appetite to fat A and there was a tendency for subjects to consume more over the whole test day when they had consumed the lunch containing fat A than when they had consumed the lunch containing fat B. Furthermore, when the data from fat conditions A and B in both studies were combined (n 40) the results of Study 1 were confirmed. Overall, the results of these short-term studies indicate that PUFA may exert a relatively stronger control over appetite than MUFA and SFA.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 2000

Footnotes

*

Some of the results in this paper were presented as a President's poster at the 8th European Congress on Obesity, Dublin, 18-21 June 1997

References

Beardshall, K, Frost, G, Morarji, Y, Domin, J, Bloom, SR and Calam, J (1989) Saturation of fat and cholecystokinin release: implications for pancreatic carcinogenesis. Lancet ii, 10081010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, EM (1994) Why is oleic acid the major storage fatty acid? International Journal of Obesity 18(Suppl. 2), 158.Google Scholar
Blundell, JE (1977) Is there a role for serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) in feeding?. International Journal of Obesity 1, 1542.Google Scholar
Blundell, JE (1992) Serotonin and the biology of feeding. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 55, 155S159S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blundell, JE and MacDiarmid, JI (1997) Fat as a risk factor for overconsumption: satiation, satiety and patterns of eating. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 97, 63S69S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borovicka, J, Kreiss, C, Asal, K, Remy, B, Mettraux, C, Wells, A, Read, NW, Jansen, JBD'Amato, M, Delaloye, AB, Fried, M and Schwizer, W (1996) Role of cholecystokinin as a regulator of solid and liquid gastric emptying in humans. American Journal of Physiology 271, G448G453.Google ScholarPubMed
Cooling, J, Barth, J and Blundell, J (1998) The high-fat phenotype: is leptin involved in the adaptive response to a high fat (high energy) diet?. International Journal of Obesity 22, 11321135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, SJ, Dourish, CT and Barber, DJ (1990) Reversal of the anorectic effect of (+)-fenfluramine in the rat by the selective cholecystokinin receptor antagonist MK-329. British Journal of Pharmacology 99, 6570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doucet, E, Almeras, N, White, MDDespres, J-P, Bouchard, C and Tremblay, A (1998) Dietary fat composition and human adiposity. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 52, 26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, S, Mutuma, S, Francis, J, Read, N and Meijer, G (1998) The effect of fatty acid composition on intestinal satiety in man. International Journal of Obesity 22(Suppl. 3), 82S.Google Scholar
Friedman, MI (1998) Fuel partitioning and food intake. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 67(Suppl.), 513S-518S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, MI and Tordoff, MG (1986) Fatty acid oxidation and glucose utilisation interact to control food intake in rats. American Journal of Physiology 251, R840R847.Google ScholarPubMed
Friedman, MI, Tordoff, MG and Ramirez, I (1986) Integrated metabolic control of food intake. Brain Research Bulletin 17, 855859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, SM, Burley, VJ and Blundell, JE (1994) Effect of fat- and sucrose-containing foods on the size of eating episodes and energy intake in lean males: potential for causing overconsumption. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 48, 547555.Google ScholarPubMed
Holland, BWelch, AAUnwin, IBuss, DHPaul, AA & Southgate, DAT (1991) McCance and Widdowson's The Composition of Foods, 5th ed. Cambridge: Royal Society of Chemistry/Ministry of Agriculture, Fisheries and Food.Google Scholar
Jones, PJH, Pencharz, PB and Clandinin, MT (1985) Whole body utilisation of dietary fatty acids: implications for energy utilization. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 42, 769777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, PJH, Ridgen, JE, Phang, PT and Laird Birmingham, C (1992) Influence of dietary fat polyunsaturated to saturated ratio on energy substrate utilisation in obesity. Metabolism 41, 396401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, PJH and Schoeller, DA (1988) Polyunsaturated : saturated ratio of dietary fat influences energy substrate utilisation in humans. Metabolism 37, 145151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langhans, W and Scharrer, E (1987) Evidence for a vagally mediated satiety signal derived from hepatic fatty acid oxidation. Journal of the Autonomic Nervous System 18, 1321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawton, CL, Burley, VJ, Wales, JK and Blundell, JE (1993) Overeating of fat in obese women: failure of high fat intake to suppress later food intake. International Journal of Obesity 17, 409416.Google Scholar
Leyton, J, Drury, PJ and Crawford, MA (1987) Differential oxidation of saturated and unsaturated fatty acids in vivo in the rat. British Journal of Nutrition 57, 383393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullen, BJ and Martin, RJ (1992) The effect of dietary fat on diet selection may involve central serotonin. American Journal of Physiology 263, R559R563.Google ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ, Gnizak, N, Summerfelt, A and Jacobs Laster, L (1988) Food intake in dieters and nondieters after a liquid meal containing medium-chain triglycerides. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 48, 6671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJKim-Harris, S, Fischman, MW, Foltin, RW, Moran, TH and Stoner, SA (1994) Satiety after preloads with different amounts of fat and carbohydrate: implications for obesity. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 60, 476487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimomura, Y, Tamura, T and Suzuki, M (1990) Less body fat accumulation in rats fed a safflower oil diet than rats fed a beef tallow diet. Journal of Nutrition 120, 12911296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stallone, D, Nicolaidis, S and Gibbs, J (1989) Cholecystokinin-induced anorexia depends on serotoninergic function. American Journal of Physiology 256, R1138R1141.Google ScholarPubMed
Stubbs, RJ and Harbron, CG (1995) Isoenergetic substitution of MCT for LCT: effect on energy intake in ad libitum feeding men International Journal of Obesity 19(Suppl. 2), 28.Google Scholar
Stubbs, RJ, Harbron, CG, Murgatroyd, PR and Prentice, AM (1995) Covert manipulation of dietary fat and energy density: effect on substrate flux and food intake in men eating ad libitum.American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 62, 316329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stubbs, RJ, Ritz, P, Coward, WA and Prentice, AM (1995) Covert manipulation of the ratio of dietary fat to carbohydrate and energy density: effect on food intake and energy balance in free-living men eating ad libitum. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 62, 330337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stunkard, AJ and Messick, S (1985) The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 29, 7183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremblay, A, Drapeau, V, Doucet, E, Almeras, NDespres, J-P and Bouchard, C (1998) Fat balance and ageing: results from the Quebec Family Study. British Journal of Nutrition 79, 413418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wurtman, J (1985) Neurotransmitter control of carbohydrate consumption. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443, 145151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed