Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T04:50:09.523Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Consistent effect of eating rate on food and energy intake across twenty-four ad libitum meals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2024

Lise A. J. Heuven
Affiliation:
Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands Food Quality and Design group, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Marieke van Bruinessen
Affiliation:
Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Claudia S. Tang
Affiliation:
Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Markus Stieger
Affiliation:
Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands Food Quality and Design group, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Marlou P. Lasschuijt
Affiliation:
Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Ciarán G. Forde*
Affiliation:
Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University & Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Ciarán G. Forde, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Foods consumed at lower eating rates (ER) lead to reductions in energy intake. Previous research has shown that texture-based differences in eating rateER can reduce meal size. The effect size and consistency of these effects across a wide range of composite and complex meals differing considerably in texture and varying in meal occasion have not been reported. We determined how consistently texture-based differences in ER can influence food and energy intake across a wide variety of meals. In a crossover design, healthy participants consumed twelve breakfast and twelve lunch meals that differed in texture to produce a fast or slow ER. A breakfast group (n = 15) and lunch group (n = 15) completed twelve ad libitum meal sessions each (six ‘fast’ and six ‘slow’ meals), where intake was measured and behavioural video annotation was used to characterise eating behaviour. Liking did not differ significantly between fast and slow breakfasts (P = 0·44) or lunches (P = 0·76). The slow meals were consumed on average 39 % ± 9 % (breakfast) and 45 % ± 7 % (lunch) slower than the fast meals (both P < 0·001). Participants consumed on average 22 % ± 5 % less food (84 g) and 13 % ± 6 % less energy (71 kcal) from slow compared with fast meals (mean ± SE; P < 0·001). Consuming meals with a slower ER led to a reduction in food intake, where an average decrease of 20 % in ER produced an 11 % ± 1 % decrease in food intake (mean ± SE). These findings add to the growing body of evidence showing that ER can be manipulated using food texture and that this has aits consistent effect on food and energy intake across a wide variety of Hedonically equivalent meals.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

A growing body of research has shown that when meals are eaten at a faster rate this leads to greater intake(Reference Robinson, Almiron-Roig and Rutters1), with supporting evidence from acute feeding trials(Reference Bolhuis, Forde and Cheng2), prospective cohort studies(Reference Fogel, Goh and Fries3) and cross-sectional studies(Reference Teo, van Dam and Forde4,Reference Teo, van Dam and Whitton5) . The impact of faster eating rates (ER) on energy intake is further accentuated when consuming foods with a higher energy density(Reference Teo, van Dam and Whitton5,Reference McCrickerd, Lim and Leong6) . Eating rate (g/min) is defined as the amount of food consumed per unit time, while energy intake rate (EIR) (kcal/min) is the amount of energy consumed per unit of time. Both eating rate and EIR are contingent on an individual’s drive to eat and the structure and nutritive properties of the food being consumed(Reference Teo and Forde7). A food’s texture can directly impact eating rate(Reference Gonzalez-Estanol, Libardi and Biasioli8Reference Wee, Goh and Stieger10), where food textures that have shorter structure breakdown during mastication, require less lubrication and take less time to form a swallowable bolus are consumed at faster eating rates(Reference Forde and Bolhuis11Reference Hutchings and Lillford13).

Faster eating rates have been shown to increase ad libitum energy intake within meals(Reference Bolhuis, Forde and Cheng2,Reference Forde, van Kuijk and Thaler14Reference Teo, Lim and Goh16) and consuming a fixed calorie portion at a faster rate results in a lower satiety response on a calorie for calorie basis(Reference Zhu, Hsu and Hollis17Reference Miquel-Kergoat, Azais-Braesco and Burton-Freeman19). Eating rate and EIR may also exert a sustained effect on habitual energy intakes, prompting consistently higher meal size over a longer time frame. This was shown in a recent randomised controlled inpatient feeding trial which compared energy intakes from minimally processed and ultra-processed food (UPF) diets over 14 d on each diet arm. The trial showed that the UPF diet led to a sustained average higher daily energy intake of 508 kcal/d and was linked to 0·9 kg weight gain(Reference Hall, Ayuketah and Brychta20). Greater energy intakes on the UPF diet (increase of 144 kcal at breakfast, 248 kcal at lunch and 108 kcal at dinner) compared with a minimally processed diet were associated with a faster eating rate and almost 50 % increase in EIR. Further research is needed to better understand whether the observed differences in food and energy intake were causally attributable by meal texture-based differences in eating rate.

Numerous studies to date have explored the impact of food texture on eating rate and intake for model foods or individual food items given in fixed portions. In contrast, a limited number of studies have investigated the impact of texture on eating rate and intake in meals as they are commonly consumed, for example, realistic meals with multiple components(Reference Lasschuijt, Camps and Mars15,Reference Teo, Lim and Goh16) . The relationships between texture, eating rate and ad libitum food intake of composite mixed meals tend to be more complex, as meal components interact during consumption, influencing meal eating rate and total intake. Previous research has shown that eating rate can be reduced by an increased viscosity(Reference McCrickerd, Lim and Leong6,Reference Zijlstra, Mars and de Wijk21Reference Hogenkamp, Mars and Stafleu24) , increased hardness(Reference Bolhuis, Forde and Cheng2,Reference Forde, van Kuijk and Thaler9,Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu25Reference Janani, Tan and Goh28) , smaller unit size(Reference Weijzen, Liem and Zandstra29Reference Gavião, Engelen and van der Bilt34) and decreased initial moisture/lubrication(Reference van Eck, Franks and Vinyard22,Reference Heuven, de Graaf and Forde27,Reference Janani, Tan and Goh28,Reference Gavião, Engelen and van der Bilt34Reference Engelen, Fontijn-Tekamp and van der Bilt38) . While different specific texture properties of food have been shown to influence eating speed independently, combinations of texture manipulations have been shown to be most effective in slowing eating rate(Reference Heuven, de Graaf and Forde27,Reference Janani, Tan and Goh28) . However, translating this from simple textures or composite foods to complex meals requires further research on the impact of meal components’ textures on eating rate. Several proof of principal studies have suggested that the effect of meal texture on eating rate could have a meaningful impact on food and energy intake within meals(Reference Bolhuis, Forde and Cheng2,Reference McCrickerd, Lim and Leong6,Reference Forde, van Kuijk and Thaler9,Reference Teo, Lim and Goh16) . However, (i) the consistency of these effects across a wider range of composite and complex meals with different textures and meal occasions has not been explored yet. The majority of previous studies explored the effect of food texture on eating rate in either model foods, meal components or single meals. Consequently, (ii) relationships between texture-based reductions in meal eating rate and the associated effect size of the decrease in food intake across a broad range of complex, composite meals consumed at different meal occasions have not been established yet.

The aim of the current study was to determine the consistency of texture-based differences in eating rate (g/min) on food (g) and energy (kcal) intake across a wide variety of breakfast and lunch meals. Our hypothesis was that meals with textures that encourage a slower eating rate would consistently result in lower food and energy intakes compared with meals with textures that promote a faster eating rate.

Materials & methods

Study design

Two groups of participants (n 15/group) were each recruited to consume twelve meals (twenty-four total) in two full randomised crossover designs. The primary manipulation was the design of meals to have textures likely to facilitate a ‘slow’ or a ‘fast’ ER. One group (n 15) consumed breakfast (6 × fast, 6 × slow) and the other group (n 15) consumed lunch (6 × fast, 6 × slow). All participants (n 30) attended either twelve breakfast or twelve lunch sessions with one ad libitum meal per session in randomised order. Participants attended two sessions per week and at least 2 d between the sessions to minimise possible carryover effects from one ad libitum meal to another. Participants received monetary compensation upon completing the study. This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human participants were approved by Social Sciences Ethical Committee of Wageningen University under the Umbrella protocol (2022-118-SBSEB-prc) and were registered at Clinicaltrials.gov Clinical Trial registry: NCT05615350 (https://clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT05615350). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Participants

A sample size calculation (G * Power version 3.1.9.4) was used to estimate that a minimum of fifteen participants was required to obtain a significant 200 kcal difference between fast and slow meals (α = 0·05, power 1-β = 0·80), based on a previous study investigating the effect of food texture on ad libitum food intake (soft meals 756 kcal, 95 % CI 699, 814 kcal; hard meals: 556 kcal, 95 % CI 510, 601 kcal)(Reference Teo, Lim and Goh16). A conservative assumption was made that within-subject energy intake correlations was 0·1.

Inclusion criteria for the study were healthy, normal appetite, between 18 and 55 years old, BMI between 18·5 and 30 kg/m2, non-smoker, speak and understand English without difficulty, and commonly consuming three meals a day (5 d a week) around the same times. These inclusion criteria were used to have a representative sample of healthy adults. Exclusion criteria were having difficulties with eating, having braces or oral piercing, smoking, consuming on average more than twenty-one glasses of alcohol per week, not willing to stop using drugs during the study period, having allergies or intolerance to any ingredient of the test meals, not willing to eat the test food, having extensive facial hair that would interfere with video coding, following an energy-restricted diet during the last 2 months, gained or lost 5 kg of body weight over the last half year, doing intensive exercising more than 8 h per week, having a low score (≤1) for the expected liking of the individual test components on a nine-point category scale and being unfamiliar (<6 on a nine-point category scale) with more than 25 % of the test meals.

Test meals

The study consisted of twelve breakfast meals and twelve lunch meals (twenty-four meals in total). Two meal occasions were included to have a wide range of both cold meals (during breakfast) and hot meals (during lunch), and each meal occasion was evaluated by separate participant groups to minimise participants fatigue and carryover effects between test meals(Reference Blundell, De Graaf and Hulshof39). To enable comparison with recent findings linking eating rate to energy intake for UPF meals, the ingredients of the meal components were selected for all meals that met the previously described criteria for Nova 4 (‘ultra-processed’)(Reference Monteiro, Cannon and Levy40). The Nova classification system consists of four groups that range from unprocessed or minimally processed foods (group 1) to UPF (group 4). Nova group 4 defines UPF as: ‘Formulations of ingredients, mostly of exclusive industrial use, that result from a series of industrial processes’(Reference Monteiro, Cannon and Levy40). The Nova group of each ingredient of a meal was determined independently by two coders using a pre-defined Nova definition. This approach was adopted due to the subjective nature of the Nova classification scheme which is open to misinterpretation. There was disagreement for 11 % of the breakfast ingredients and 7 % of the lunch ingredients. These ingredients were classified by a third coder to reach consensus. For breakfasts, 94 % of the meal energy was derived from ultra-processed ingredients, and this was 87 % for the lunch meals.

The selection of specific foods to ensure texture-based differences in eating for each breakfast and lunch meal was based on literature(Reference Forde, van Kuijk and Thaler14,Reference Viskaal-van Dongen, Kok and de Graaf41Reference Forde, Leong and Chia-Ming43) . Meals were extensively pilot-tested where the eating rate and liking of small portions were determined with a separate panel of pilot test participants. A summary description of the breakfast and lunch meals selected and included in the trial after the extensive pilot testing is shown Table 1. Fast meals tended to have softer textures with a more lubricating consistency, whereas slow meals tended to have harder, drier, more elastic and more viscous textures. For further information on approaches used to manipulate eating rate using meal texture, see refs. 11 and 12. All meals consisted of commercially available food products purchased in supermarkets in the Netherlands. More detailed information about the meals can be found in the online Supplementary Material. For comparison purposes, breakfast and lunch meals were organised into pairs and matched for portion size (g), total energy (kcal), energy density (kcal/g), variety (number of components) and visual volume. The fast and slow meals were also matched to be overall equivalent for energy from macronutrients and fibre content served (Table 2). The meal components were served individually on plates or bowls allowing participants to self-serve their portion ad libitum (individual mini-buffet). Thrice the standard portion (1500 g) of each meal was served to facilitate ad libitum meal consumption, with additional food always made available in the event a participant completed the full portion. If a participant finished a full portion, a second full portion was offered immediately, though this occurred on only two occasions across all 360 ad libitum test meals. All meals were served with 120 ml of water, and the weight of each component and water were measured before and after consumption (g). Amount consumed (g) was converted to energy (kcal) by multiplying by the energy density (kcal/g) of each meal component.

Table 1. Description of the fast and slow matched-pairs of breakfast meals and lunch meals

FB, fast breakfast; SB, slow breakfast; FL, fast lunch; SL, slow lunch.

Table 2. Average nutritional composition of the meals. Data are presented as mean ± sd

ER, eating rate; En, energy.

* According to Nova 4, classified by two independent researchers.

Procedure

Participants were recruited by social media, flyers and mailing lists from Wageningen and surroundings. Interested participants were invited to an information and screening session. They received information about the study and its goals. To conceal the true aim of the study, participants were informed that the purpose of the study was ‘to investigate the effect of emotions on eating behaviour’. On completion of the study, participants were debriefed on the real purpose of the study (no participant guessed the aim of the study correctly). Participants signed an informed consent and were asked to complete a questionnaire detailing general health and self-reported eating rate(Reference van den Boer, Kranendonk and van de Wiel44), the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) (Reference Van Strien, Frijters and Bergers45) and the Reasons Individuals Stop Eating Questionnaire (RISE-Q) (Reference Cunningham, Roe and Hayes46). During the introductory session, participants were asked to rate their expected liking of and familiarity with all test meals and had their height and weight recorded (Seca 213 stadiometer and Seca 704 column scale). Participants’ eating rate of a 15 g piece of raw carrot (width and depth of 12·7 mm) was quantified in duplicate(Reference Janani, Tan and Goh28).

After participants with availability for only one of the two groups (breakfast or lunch group) were allocated, the other participants were randomly allocated to one of the two meal groups. Participants were randomly allocated to a time slot and scheduled for twelve ad libitum test meal sessions. Participants were asked to refrain from intensive exercise and alcohol 24 h before each test session and to eat the same amount of dinner the night before each test session (not provided) between 18:00 and 20:00. To standardise appetite on the day of each test meal, participants in the breakfast group were requested not to eat or drink anything except water before the test meal breakfast. Participants in the lunch group consumed one glass of water and a fixed portion standardised breakfast which was provided by researchers and consumed at home 4 h before each lunch test session. The standardised breakfast (559 kcal) consisted of a package of multigrain breakfast biscuits (Liga Belvita: 50 g), a package of strawberry yogurt (Melkunie Breaker: 200 g), an apple (135 g) and a carton box of apple juice (AH private label: 200 ml).

Meal sessions lasted approximately 30 min and were held in the sensory booths at the Human Research Unit at Wageningen University. Breakfast sessions were between 8:00 and 9:30 am, and lunch sessions were between 12:00 and 13:30 pm. Participants consumed their meals at the same time slot for all twelve sessions. At the beginning of each session, participants rated their pre-meal appetite (hunger, fullness, thirst, desire to eat and prospective consumption) on a 100 mm anchored line scale ranging from ‘Not at all’ to ‘Extremely’. Participants then received a test meal and were asked to take a single bite and rate individual meal components on liking, familiarity, flavour intensity and three texture attributes (hardness, thickness and dryness). Hardness was defined as ‘The force needed to take a bite. An example of “Not at all” are mashed bananas and an example of “Extremely” are roasted peanuts’. Thickness was defined as ‘The resistance of the food to flow in the mouth and is related to its viscosity. An example of “Not at all” is water and an example of “Extremely” is pudding’. Lastly, dryness was defined as ‘The sensation of the absence of liquid/moisture in the food. An example of “Not at all” is watermelon and an example of “Extremely” are crackers’. In addition to the sensory attributes, participants were asked to rate liking, desire to eat, familiarity and expected satiety of the entire meal after consuming one bite. All ratings were performed on 100 mm anchored line scale ranging from ‘Not at all’ to ‘Extremely’, except for familiarity which was rated on a nine-point category scale. After the ratings, participants were instructed to eat until they felt comfortably full. Participants were informed that they do not need consume foods in whole units (e.g. they could take a bite from a cookie and leave the rest) to limit unit bias. Meal consumption time was measured with a stopwatch and eating behaviour during the meal was recorded on video. At the end of each test meal, participants provided a reason for why they stopped eating (based on RISE-Q(Reference Cunningham, Roe and Hayes46)) and rated their post-meal appetite.

Behavioural coding of eating rate and food oral processing

Participants were video-recorded using a webcam (Logitech C310 – HD Webcam) and action camera (EKEN H9R Action-cam) positioned at face level while consuming each test meal. Participants could not see themselves while being recorded and were requested not to move their head too much while eating their meal. Post hoc behavioural coding was completed for a representative subset of videos to establish the average oral processing behaviours and microstructural parameters for each meal. A total of 165 (46 %; 7 participants × 24 meals except for 3 missing/incomplete videos) of the 360 videos (15 participants × 24 meals) were manually annotated across breakfast and lunch meal conditions. For these videos, oral processing behaviour was manually annotated by trained video coders using a coding scheme developed previously(Reference Forde, van Kuijk and Thaler14) using the ELAN version 4.9.2 (Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, The Language Archive). The coding scheme included four point events (number of bites, chews, sips and swallows) and three continuous events (eating duration per bite, duration per sip and total cumulative active consumption duration (all in seconds)). Breakfast and lunch meals were independently annotated by trained video coders and after coding 11 % of the videos, data were checked for agreement. Overall, the intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) were between 0·92 and 1·00 across all oral processing behaviours for breakfasts and lunches across the coders, indicating excellent consistency (ICC > 0·90(Reference Koo and Li47)). The remaining videos were coded separately, and the data were collated to derive summary measures of oral processing for each sample. All ICC between the coders can be found in the online Supplementary Material.

The meal eating rate (g/min) was calculated by dividing the total consumed meal weight (test food + water) by the total consumption duration measured with the stopwatch. The EIR (kcal/min) was defined as the consumed energy divided by the total consumption duration of the meal measured by stopwatch. The average bite size (g) was calculated by dividing the weight of the consumed test food by the total number of bites and sips of the test food, the number of chews per bite by dividing the number of chews by the number of bites and sips of the test food, the number of chews per gram (g–1) by dividing the number of chews by the consumed weight of the test food, oro-sensory exposure (OSE) time (min) by the summation of the bite and sip durations of the test food, OSEg (s/g) by dividing the OSE time by the weight of the test food and the chewing frequency (chews/s) by dividing the number of chews by the chewing time of solid foods. Moreover, eating rate of only the test foods (test food intake divided by OSE time) from the videos was subtracted. Since the eating rates obtained with the stopwatch (including water intake; of all participants) as proxy for the eating rates obtained with video coding (excluding water intake; of half of the participants) showed excellent reliability for the breakfasts (ICC = 0·98) and good reliability for the lunches (ICC = 0·86), only the eating rate obtained with the stopwatch is reported in the result section.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed in R version 4.1.1 (R Core Team, R Foundation for Statistical Computing). Two-tailed P values <0·05 were considered statistically significant. Descriptive data on sensory attributes, oral processing and appetite were analysed using paired t tests with the package ‘stats’. The ICC was calculated using the package ‘irr’. Analysis on the main outcomes was performed by repeated-measures linear mixed effect model using the packages ‘lme4’ and ‘lmerTest’. The models included condition (fast or slow) as fixed factor and participant as random factor. Multiple variables – including liking, test order, pre-appetite and reasons to stop eating—were investigated as possible confounders. No significant interaction effects were observed. Inclusion of liking, test order, pre-appetite and reasons to stop eating as covariate did not significantly change the outcomes of the data analysis, suggesting that these are not confounding factors of our study. Therefore, these variables (liking, test order, pre-appetite and reasons to stop eating) were not included in the final models. Separate models were made for the breakfast and lunch group. The estimation of the decrease in intake with a 20 % decrease in intake was calculated by applying a linear model to each individuals’ data. Using the intercept and slope, the percentage of decrease in intake for a 20 % decrease in eating rate was calculated, and these outcomes were than averaged across participants. One meal of one participant was excluded from this analysis since this point was an outlier for the participants’ eating rate as assessed with interquartile method (>1·5 * IQR)). Multiple factor analysis was performed on the pooled breakfast and dinner data for visual interpretation using the packages FactoMineR’ and ‘factoextra’(Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen48). Repeated-measures correlation coefficients were obtained using the package ‘rmcorr’. The β coefficients of component intake and sensory ratings of pooled data were obtained using repeated-measures linear mixed effect model with the sensory ratings as fixed factor, participant as random factor and meal as covariate (to correct for intake of components in the same meal).

Results

Participant characteristics

In total, thirty participants were included and all participants finished the study. The breakfast and dinner groups were similar on general characteristics and eating behaviour measures (Table 3).

Table 3. Participant characteristics. Data are presented as mean ± sd

* Self-reported eating rate was measured on a scale from (1) very slow to (5) very fast(Reference van den Boer, Kranendonk and van de Wiel44).

The carrot eating rate (g/min) was used to characterise the observed eating rate of participants consuming a 15 g of raw carrot stick in duplicate.

The restrained eater score was measured using the validated Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ)(Reference Van Strien, Frijters and Bergers45).

§ The physical satisfaction as reason to stop eating score was measured using the validated Reasons Individuals Stop Eating Questionnaire (RISE-Q)(Reference Cunningham, Roe and Hayes46).

Oral processing, liking, sensory properties and appetite

For all fast and slow pairs of breakfast and lunch meals, the meals with textures designed to have a fast eating rate were consumed with a faster eating rate than the corresponding meals with textures designed to have a slow eating rate. Participants consumed fast breakfasts on average with a 39 % ± 9 % higher eating rate (g/min) compared with slow breakfasts and fast lunches on average with a 45 % ± 7 % higher eating rate compared with slow lunches (mean ± s e, Table 4). Fast meals were consumed on average with a 41 % ± 7 % (breakfast) and 34 % ± 10 % (lunch) higher EIR (kcal/min) compared with the slow meals at breakfast and lunch (mean ± s e). Behavioural annotation enabled a comparison of the micro-structural patterns of eating and showed that fast meals were consumed on average with larger bite sizes, fewer chews and shorter OSE than slow meals, with a similar chewing frequency for all conditions (Table 4).

Table 4. Microstructure of oral processing behaviour of fast and slow breakfasts and lunches. Data are presented as mean ± se

OSE, oro-sensory exposure; EIR, energy intake rate.

* For subset of participants. Assessed with video coding with fast breakfasts n 42 videos; slow breakfasts meals n 42, fast lunches n 40 and slow lunches n 41.

Almost all meals (22 out of 24; 92 %) had an average liking rating >50. Average liking and familiarity for the breakfasts and lunch meals were not significantly different between the fast and slow meals (all P > 0·44, Table 5). Comparison of the sensory ratings showed that the slow meal components were on average perceived as harder, dryer and thicker compared with the fast meal components (Table 5). Pre-meal appetite ratings confirmed that participants had equivalent appetite need states before each of the ad libitum meals. Post-meal appetite ratings confirmed that participants felt comfortably full following each meal with no differences in post-meal appetite between fast and slow breakfasts or fast and slow lunches (all P > 0·05). For the reason to stop eating, participants selected ‘I was full’ 60 % of the times, ‘The food is no longer appealing to me’ 13 % and ‘I was bored with the flavour’ 8 %. Appetite ratings and the reasons of the participants to stop eating are summarised in the online Supplementary material.

Table 5. Sensory and hedonic ratings of the meal overall and of the meal components of all participants. Data are presented as mean ± se

* There were in total sixteen breakfast components and eighteen lunch components.

Only for semi-solids and liquid components of the fast breakfasts (n 7), slow breakfasts (n 5), fast lunches (n 5) and slow lunches (n 0).

Food and energy intake

Food intake was on average 21 % ± 3 % (84 g; P < 0·001) higher when participants consumed the fast breakfasts compared with the slow breakfasts and 23 % ± 9 % (85 g; P < 0·001) higher for the fast lunches compared with the slow lunches (mean ± s e; Fig. 1). Energy intake was on average 16 % ± 8 % (104 kcal; P < 0·001) higher when consuming the fast breakfasts compared with the slow breakfasts (mean ± s e). Energy intake was similar (39 kcal difference; P = 0·28) between fast and slow lunches (Fig. 2).

Fig. 1. Food intake (g) and eating rate (g/min) of the fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) breakfast meals (A) and fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) lunch meals (B) of all participants. FB, fast breakfast. SB, slow breakfast. FL, fast lunch. SL, slow lunch. Mean ± se.

Fig. 2. The energy intakes of the fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) breakfasts and the fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) lunch meals of all participants. The bars represent the mean ± se.

The relative differences in energy intake between the fast and slow meals (16 % for breakfasts and 6 % for lunch) were smaller than the relative differences in food intake (22 %), even though the served meals had similar energy densities (Table 2). The energy densities of the breakfast meals consumed was not different (P = 0·28) between the fast (1·65 ± 0·05 kcal/g) and slow breakfasts (1·70 ± 0·05 kcal/g). However, within the lunch meals the energy density consumed was 0·18 kcal/g higher (P < 0·001) for the slow lunches (1·57 ± 0·05 kcal/g) than for the fast lunches (1·39 ± 0·04 kcal/g). This difference in ad libitum energy density (kcal/g) consumed between the fast and slow meals resulted in a smaller net average difference in energy consumed (kcal) than amount consumed (g).

Consistency of the effect of eating rate on food intake

For ten of the twelve meal pairs (83 %), meals with textures leading to faster eating rates had a higher food intake compared with meals with textures leading to slower eating rates, though intake varied considerably between meal pairs (Fig. 1). Eating rate had a consistent effect on food intake (g) across all twenty-four meals (twelve breakfasts and twelve lunches) (Fig. 3). Eating rate was a good predictor of food intake for breakfast (β = 3·7, P < 0·001), lunch (β = 4·0, P < 0·001) and all meals together (β = 3·8, P < 0·001). Based on the average eating rate across all twenty-four meals (63 g/min for breakfast and 54 g/min for dinner), we estimate that a decrease of 20 % in eating rate resulted in a decrease of 10 % ± 1 % (lunch; range of 4–21 %) to 12 % ± 1 % (breakfast; range of 5–20 %) in food intake (mean ± s e).

Fig. 3. Plot of the average eating rates and intakes of the fast breakfasts (FB; purple), slow breakfasts (SB; green), fast lunches (FL; purple) and slow lunches (SL; green) of all participants. The black dashed line represent the regression line of best fit based on average values. The grey dashed lines indicate the average eating rate (59 g/min) and average intake (447 kcal) of all meals.

Drivers of meal intake

To summarise the relative influence of the main oral processing and hedonic variables on meal intake, factor analysis was performed (Fig. 4). Figure 4 shows that food intake, energy intake and EIR of a meal were positively correlated with eating rate and bite size and negatively correlated with number of chews, OSEg and chews per bite. Participants’ ratings for meal liking and familiarity were not correlated with eating rate, EIR or any of the oral processing parameters. Repeated-measures correlation coefficients showed that food intake of the whole meal had the strongest significant correlation with eating rate (Table 6).

Fig. 4. Correlation circle of multiple factor analysis performed on intake (blue; of all participants), eating behaviour (black; eating rate of all participants, other behaviours of subset of participants), liking familiarity and expected satiation (red; of all participants). The first two dimensions explain together 56 % of the variance. OSE, oro-sensory exposure.

Table 6. Repeated-measures correlation coefficients of food intake (g)

OSE, oro-sensory exposure.

* Assessed from video coding for half of the participants.

Variation in intake of individual meal components was primarily driven by liking (β = 1·71, P < 0·001), followed by the sensory attributes dryness (β = −1·41, P < 0·001) and hardness (β = −1·01, P < 0·001). A summary of this analysis can be found in the online Supplementary Material.

Discussion

This study demonstrates that eating rate can be moderated in a consistent manner using hedonically appealing food textures to slow down eating rate and reduce food intake across a wide variety of representative everyday multiple-component meals. Texture-based differences in eating rate resulted on average in a 21 % difference in food intake for both breakfast and lunch meals. Based on our findings, we estimate a texture-based reduction in eating rate of 20 % will likely generate an average 10–12 % decrease in meal intake, though the size of this effect will vary from meal to meal.

Previous research has suggested that a 20 % reduction in texture-based eating rate produces a 10–15 % reduction in energy intake(Reference Forde49), though this estimate was based on the observed differences across a much smaller set of one component ad libitum test meals(Reference Bolhuis and Forde12). The current study shows the impact of a variety of different food texture manipulations had a consistent effect on eating rate and intake across a large variety of representative meals with different consumption contexts (breakfast and lunch). Previous research has shown that children’s eating behaviours were consistently linked to food and energy intake across four ad libitum meals varying in composition, texture and portion size (chicken nuggets, macaroni, grapes and broccoli)(Reference Pearce, Neuwald and Evans50). Similarly, when the same foods are served on four consecutive weeks to adults in a similar need state, eating rate and ad libitum energy intake were consistent at an individual level. That study pooled data from four experimental studies using rice-based meals and demonstrated such strong behavioural consistency that energy intake on week four could be predicted by an individual’s eating rate in week 1(Reference McCrickerd and Forde51). When combined with the findings from the current trial, this suggests that texture-based differences in eating rate could be used to reduce eating speed and food intake in a consistent manner across a wide range of similarly hedonically appealing everyday meals.

A goal of the current study was to identify patterns of food texture combinations that consistently slow eating rate for breakfast and lunchtime meals. Many slow meals had higher perceived hardness and dryness compared with the fast meals, in line with previous research(Reference Bolhuis, Forde and Cheng2,Reference Forde, van Kuijk and Thaler9,Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu25Reference Janani, Tan and Goh28) . Lower moisture content and lubrication(Reference van Eck, Franks and Vinyard22,Reference Heuven, de Graaf and Forde27,Reference Janani, Tan and Goh28,Reference Gavião, Engelen and van der Bilt34Reference Engelen, Fontijn-Tekamp and van der Bilt38) alongside changes in chewy and textures(Reference Bolhuis and Forde12) have also been shown to reduce eating speed. The study confirmed that combinations of texture manipulations seems to have the strongest effect in reducing eating rate(Reference Heuven, de Graaf and Forde27,Reference Janani, Tan and Goh28) . An example of such a meal pair was ‘hutspot’ (FL5; mashed potato, carrot and onion with beef stew) which had a soft and moist texture and higher eating rate and intake compared with its slow counterpart ‘sauerkraut dish’ (SL 5; grated potato, spiced sauerkraut and smoked sausage) which had a hard, chewy and dry texture. Most of the meals were within the positive hedonic range, where liking was on average equivalent for the fast and slow versions of the paired meals. However, within the current trial, in a minority of cases (two out of twelve meal pairs), the hedonic appeal of the slow meal dominated intake behaviour, and although participants ate the meal slower, they ate more in response to the foods appeal. This highlights that hedonic aspects of a meal may surpass the effect of texture. The study aimed to reduce the influence of liking on food intake, but there might yet be residual influence from individual differences in liking that were not accounted for in our models. Although texture-based reductions in eating rate leads to decreases in food intake, it is important to acknowledge that eating behaviour is a multi-factorial response to the integrated sensory, structural and physical–chemical properties of the foods being consumed(Reference McCrickerd and Forde52). In addition, other individual and environmental factors such as stress, distraction, smell, sound and temperature have been shown to influence intake(Reference McCrickerd and Forde52Reference de Eguilaz, de Morentin Aldabe and Almiron-Roig54). The relationship between food texture, eating rate and intake is complex and merit further research, though findings from the current study suggest there is consistency in how consumers translate the differences in food texture into slower eating rates that affect food intake.

The significant effect of eating rate on food and energy intake found in this study is in line with recent findings that have sought to investigate the effect of meal texture on food intake for foods that would be classified as minimally or ultra-processed. Teo and colleagues showed that food texture moderates intake across both minimally or ultra-processed ad libitum test meals where the slower meals were consumed with on average 21 % less food (g) and 26 % less energy(Reference Teo, Lim and Goh16) compared with fast meals. Similarly, Lasschuijt and colleagues compared ad libitum intake for breakfast, lunch, dinner and dessert across a day of minimally or ultra-processed slow and fast meals. The faster meals were consumed at faster eating rate (46 %) and led to a 14 % increase in amount consumed (g) and 33 % higher energy intake compared with slower meals(Reference Lasschuijt, Camps and Mars15). In both cases, there was no influence of degree of food processing on eating rate, amount or energy consumed and the texture-based differences in eating rate determined intake behaviour(Reference Lasschuijt, Camps and Mars15,Reference Teo, Lim and Goh16) . These studies highlight the possibility that higher energy intakes reported for UPF could be in part be mediated by the texture-based eating rates of the meals tested(Reference Hall, Ayuketah and Brychta20,Reference Forde, Mars and de Graaf55) . Whereas food intake results of the current study are in line with previous findings, the overall net effect on energy intake was smaller than expected, especially for lunchtime meals. This may be a result of the meals used in this study, where several components were combined to represent realistic multi-component meals. Energy density was matched at the level of the meal offered ad libitum, though the constituent meal components differed in energy densities, creating opportunities in the ad libitum paradigm for participants to select the components that they wanted to consume. An artefact of this approach was the selection and intake of some of the more energy-dense meal components within the slow meals at lunchtime. This resulted in differences in the energy density of the meal consumed and a compression of the differences in the net energy (kcal) consumed when compared with observed differences in amount (g). A similar trend was observed in the previous RCT to test the impact of minimally vs ultra-processed diets, where energy density consumed was significantly higher for the UPF diet than for the minimally processed diet(Reference Hall, Ayuketah and Brychta20). Taken together, these findings highlight that foods higher in energy can have a strong impact on energy intake when consumed quickly, but there is an opportunity to accentuate the observed reduction in energy intake in slower textured foods by also reducing the energy density of these foods. Future research is needed to investigate synergies between the combinations of energy density and eating rate reductions to better control meal size and energy intake.

Limitations of the current study were the small sample size for descriptive and hedonic ratings and the controlled eating behaviour research unit environment. The ecological validity of the study should be explored in the future using controlled dietary interventions studies at the home of participants. A strength of the current study was the wide variety of commonly consumed meals that were matched for energy density, energy from macro-nutrients, liking, variety and volume. The current study used composite meals rather than individual food items or homogenous foods, which made the approach more complex but also increased the external validity of the findings. These findings will help to better understand how natural variations in the texture and eating rate of commonly consumed meals can influence habitual energy intakes from meals comprising components that are defined as ultra-processed. In line with recent studies that have investigated the effect of meal texture on eating rate and energy intake from UPF meals(Reference Lasschuijt, Camps and Mars15,Reference Teo, Lim and Goh16) , the current findings support the suggestion that greater energy intakes from diets dominated by UPF foods are likely to be at least partially explained by faster eating rates and higher energy density(Reference Hall, Ayuketah and Brychta20). Whether texture-based differences in meal eating rate can lead to sustained changes in food and energy intake over a longer time period and across different populations and cultural groups remains to be seen. For example, faster eaters tend to have a higher BMI and higher risk of cardiometabolic diseases compared with slower eaters as evidenced by research results from Japan, Korea, Singapore and the Netherlands(Reference Teo, van Dam and Whitton5,Reference van den Boer, Werts and Siebelink42,Reference Lee, Kim and Jang56Reference Ohkuma, Fujii and Iwase58) . Whether the eating behaviour of populations with different BMI respond similar to changes of a foods’ texture is not clear(Reference Lee, Kim and Jang56,Reference Nagahama, Kurotani and Pham57) . If the effect of texture on eating rate is consistent and sustained for different populations, the effect of reducing eating rate using food texture creates an opportunity to steer behaviours that reduce the risk of excessive energy intakes.

In conclusion, our research highlights that meal texture has a consistent effect on eating rate and energy intake across a wide variety of hedonically equivalent breakfast and lunchtime meals. The consumption of meals with a slower eating rate consistently reduced the intake for both breakfast and lunchtime meals and was associated with smaller average bite sizes, greater chews per bite and longer OSE time. Future research should seek to investigate whether the acute effects observed on texture-based reductions in eating rate and intake can persist over a longer time period. These insights offer new opportunities to develop food-based strategies to either promote or limit intake of target foods through a combination of nutritive and sensory–behavioural approaches.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Els Siebelink, Fréderique Pruim, Daniela Hernandez Morales, Vlada Butuc, Orestis Ioannou and Jiachun Li for their contribution to the research.

This work was supported by the Dutch Top-Consortium for Knowledge and Innovation Agri & Food (TKI-Agri-food) Project Restructure (TKI 22·150). The ‘Restructure’ project is a public–private partnership on precompetitive research on the influence of food texture and eating rate on energy intake. For more information, go to https://restructureproject.org/. Data are available upon request.

The authors’ contributions were as follows: L. A. J. H.: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project Administration, Visualization, Writing – Original Draft, Writing – Review & Editing. M. van B.: Data Curation, Investigation. C. S. T.: Data Curation, Investigation. M. S.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – Review & Editing. M. P. L.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources Supervision, Writing – Original Draft, Writing – Review & Editing. C. P. F.: Conceptualization, Funding Acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – Original Draft, Writing – Review & Editing.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest in relation to the current manuscript. CF is a member of the Global Scientific Advisory Committee of the Kerry Health and Nutrition Institute and has received travel expenses for speaking about processed foods at scientific meetings.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114524001478

References

Robinson, E, Almiron-Roig, E, Rutters, F, et al. (2014) A systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effect of eating rate on energy intake and hunger. Am J Clin Nutr 100, 123151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, DP, Forde, CG, Cheng, Y, et al. (2014) Slow food: sustained impact of harder foods on the reduction in energy intake over the course of the day. PLOS ONE 9, e93370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogel, A, Goh, AT, Fries, LR, et al. (2017) Faster eating rates are associated with higher energy intakes during an ad libitum meal, higher BMI and greater adiposity among 4· 5-year-old children: results from the growing up in Singapore towards healthy outcomes (GUSTO) cohort. Br J Nutr 117, 10421051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, PS, van Dam, RM & Forde, CG (2020) Combined impact of a faster self-reported eating rate and higher dietary energy intake rate on energy intake and adiposity. Nutrients 12, 3264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, PS, van Dam, RM, Whitton, C, et al. (2021) Consumption of foods with higher energy intake rates is associated with greater energy ntake, adiposity, and cardiovascular risk factors in adults. J Nutr 151, 370378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrickerd, K, Lim, CM, Leong, C, et al. (2017) Texture-based differences in eating rate reduce the impact of increased energy density and large portions on meal size in adults. J Nutr 147, 12081217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, PS & Forde, CG (2020) The Impact of eating rate on energy intake, body composition, and health. Handbook of Eating and Drinking: Interdisciplinary Perspectives. Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Estanol, K, Libardi, M, Biasioli, F, et al. (2022) Oral processing behaviours of liquid, solid and composite foods are primarily driven by texture, mechanical and lubrication properties rather than by taste intensity. Food Funct 13, 50115022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forde, C, van Kuijk, N, Thaler, T, et al. (2013) Texture and savoury taste influences on food intake in a realistic hot lunch time meal. Appetite 60, 180186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wee, MSM, Goh, AT, Stieger, M, et al. (2018) Correlation of instrumental texture properties from textural profile analysis (TPA) with eating behaviours and macronutrient composition for a wide range of solid foods. Food Funct 9, 53015312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forde, CG & Bolhuis, D (2022) Interrelations between food form, texture, and matrix influence energy intake and metabolic responses. Curr Nutr Rep 11, 124132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, DP & Forde, CG (2020) Application of food texture to moderate oral processing behaviors and energy intake. Trends Food Sci Technol 106, 445456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchings, JB & Lillford, P (1988) The perception of food texture-the philosophy of the breakdown path. J Texture Stud 19, 103115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forde, C, van Kuijk, N, Thaler, T, et al. (2013) Oral processing characteristics of solid savoury meal components, and relationship with food composition, sensory attributes and expected satiation. Appetite 60, 208219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lasschuijt, M, Camps, G, Mars, M, et al. (2023) Speed limits: the effects of industrial food processing and food texture on daily energy intake and eating behaviour in healthy adults. Eur J Nutr 62, 29492962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teo, PS, Lim, AJ, Goh, AT, et al. (2022) Texture-based differences in eating rate influence energy intake for minimally processed and ultra-processed meals. Am J Clin Nutr 116, 244254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, Y, Hsu, WH & Hollis, JH (2013) Increasing the number of masticatory cycles is associated with reduced appetite and altered postprandial plasma concentrations of gut hormones, insulin and glucose. Br J Nutr 110, 384390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goh, AT, Choy, JYM, Chua, XH, et al. (2021) Increased oral processing and a slower eating rate increase glycaemic, insulin and satiety responses to a mixed meal tolerance test. Eur J Nutr 60, 27192733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miquel-Kergoat, S, Azais-Braesco, V, Burton-Freeman, B, et al. (2015) Effects of chewing on appetite, food intake and gut hormones: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Physiol Behavior 151, 8896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, KD, Ayuketah, A, Brychta, R, et al. (2019) Ultra-processed diets cause excess calorie intake and weight gain: an inpatient randomized controlled trial of ad libitum food intake. Cell Metab 30, 6777 e63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zijlstra, N, Mars, M, de Wijk, RA, et al. (2008) The effect of viscosity on ad libitum food intake. Int J Obes (Lond) 32, 676683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Eck, A, Franks, E, Vinyard, CJ, et al. (2020) Sauce it up: influence of condiment properties on oral processing behavior, bolus formation and sensory perception of solid foods. Food Funct 11, 61866201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mosca, AC, Torres, AP, Slob, E, et al. (2019) Small food texture modifications can be used to change oral processing behaviour and to control ad libitum food intake. Appetite 142, 104375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogenkamp, PS, Mars, M, Stafleu, A, et al. (2010) Intake during repeated exposure to low- and high-energy-dense yogurts by different means of consumption. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 841847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zijlstra, N, Mars, M, Stafleu, A, et al. (2010) The effect of texture differences on satiation in 3 pairs of solid foods. Appetite 55, 490497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lasschuijt, MP, Mars, M, Stieger, M, et al. (2017) Comparison of oro-sensory exposure duration and intensity manipulations on satiation. Physiol Behav 176, 7683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heuven, LA, de Graaf, K, Forde, CG, et al. (2023) Al dente or well done? How the eating rate of a pasta dish can be predicted by the eating rate of its components. Food Qual Preference 108, 104883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janani, R, Tan, VWK, Goh, AT, et al. (2022) Independent and combined impact of texture manipulation on oral processing behaviours among faster and slower eaters. Food Funct 13, 93409354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weijzen, PL, Liem, DG, Zandstra, E, et al. (2008) Sensory specific satiety and intake: the difference between nibble-and bar-size snacks. Appetite 50, 435442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohyama, K, Nakayama, Y, Yamaguchi, I, et al. (2007) Mastication efforts on block and finely cut foods studied by electromyography. Food Qual Preference 18, 313320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langlet, B, Tang Bach, M, Odegi, D, et al. (2018) The effect of food unit sizes and meal serving occasions on eating behaviour characteristics: within person randomised crossover studies on healthy women. Nutrients 10, 880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, UJ, Suh, HJ, Yang, SO, et al. (2012) Distinct foods with smaller unit would be an effective approach to achieve sustainable weight loss. Eating Behav 13, 7477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geier, AB, Rozin, P & Doros, G (2006) Unit bias: a new heuristic that helps explain the effect of portion size on food intake. Psychol Sci 17, 521525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gavião, MBD, Engelen, L & van der Bilt, A (2004) Chewing behavior and salivary secretion. Eur J Oral Sci 112, 1924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Eck, A, Wijne, C, Fogliano, V, et al. (2019) Shape up! How shape, size and addition of condiments influence eating behavior towards vegetables. Food Funct 10, 57395751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Eck, A, Hardeman, N, Karatza, N, et al. (2019) Oral processing behavior and dynamic sensory perception of composite foods: toppings assist saliva in bolus formation. Food Qual Preference 71, 497509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mosca, AC, Moretton, M, Angelino, D, et al. (2022) Effect of presence of gluten and spreads on the oral processing behavior of breads. Food Chem 373, 131615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelen, L, Fontijn-Tekamp, A & van der Bilt, A (2005) The influence of product and oral characteristics on swallowing. Arch Oral Biol 50, 739746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blundell, J, De Graaf, C, Hulshof, T, et al. (2010) Appetite control: methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obesity Rev 11, 251270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monteiro, CA, Cannon, G, Levy, RB, et al. (2019) Ultra-processed foods: what they are and how to identify them. Public Health Nutr 22, 936941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viskaal-van Dongen, M, Kok, FJ & de Graaf, C (2011) Eating rate of commonly consumed foods promotes food and energy intake. Appetite 56, 2531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Boer, J, Werts, M, Siebelink, E, et al. (2017b) The availability of slow and fast calories in the Dutch diet: the current situation and opportunities for interventions. Foods 6, 87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forde, C, Leong, C, Chia-Ming, E, et al. (2017) Fast or slow-foods? Describing natural variations in oral processing characteristics across a wide range of Asian foods. Food Funct 8, 595606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Boer, J, Kranendonk, J, van de Wiel, A, et al. (2017a) Self-reported eating rate is associated with weight status in a Dutch population: a validation study and a cross-sectional study. Int J Behav Nutr Physical Activity 14, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Strien, T, Frijters, JE, Bergers, GP, et al. (1986) The Dutch eating behavior questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int J Eating Disorders 5, 295315.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, PM, Roe, LS, Hayes, JE, et al. (2021) Development and validation of the reasons individuals stop eating questionnaire (RISE-Q): a novel tool to characterize satiation. Appetite 161, 105127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koo, TK & Li, MY (2016) A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J Chiropractic Med 15, 155163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A, Brockhoff, PB & Christensen, RH (2017) lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J Stat Software 82, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forde, CG (2018) From perception to ingestion; the role of sensory properties in energy selection, eating behaviour and food intake. Food Qual Preference 66, 171177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearce, A, Neuwald, N, Evans, J, et al. (2024) Child eating behaviors are consistently linked to intake across meals that vary in portion size. Appetite 196, 107258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrickerd, K & Forde, CG (2017) Consistency of eating rate, oral processing behaviours and energy intake across meals. Nutrients 9, 891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrickerd, K & Forde, C (2016) Sensory influences on food intake control: moving beyond palatability. Obesity Rev 17, 1829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroebele, N & De Castro, JM (2004) Effect of ambience on food intake and food choice. Nutr 20, 821838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Eguilaz, MHR, de Morentin Aldabe, BM, Almiron-Roig, E, et al. (2018) Multisensory influence on eating behavior: hedonic consumption. Endocrinología, Diabetes Nutrición (English ed) 65, 114125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forde, CG, Mars, M & de Graaf, K (2020) Ultra-processing or oral processing? A role for energy density and eating rate in moderating energy intake from processed foods. Curr Dev Nutr 4, nzaa019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K, Kim, D, Jang, J, et al. (2013) Eating rate is associated with cardiometabolic risk factors in Korean adults. Nutrition, Metab Cardiovasc Dis 23, 635641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagahama, S, Kurotani, K, Pham, NM, et al. (2014) Self-reported eating rate and metabolic syndrome in Japanese people: cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 4, e005241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohkuma, T, Fujii, H, Iwase, M, et al. (2013) Impact of eating rate on obesity and cardiovascular risk factors according to glucose tolerance status: the Fukuoka diabetes registry and the Hisayama study. Diabetologia 56, 7077.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Description of the fast and slow matched-pairs of breakfast meals and lunch meals

Figure 1

Table 2. Average nutritional composition of the meals. Data are presented as mean ± sd

Figure 2

Table 3. Participant characteristics. Data are presented as mean ± sd

Figure 3

Table 4. Microstructure of oral processing behaviour of fast and slow breakfasts and lunches. Data are presented as mean ± se

Figure 4

Table 5. Sensory and hedonic ratings of the meal overall and of the meal components of all participants. Data are presented as mean ± se

Figure 5

Fig. 1. Food intake (g) and eating rate (g/min) of the fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) breakfast meals (A) and fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) lunch meals (B) of all participants. FB, fast breakfast. SB, slow breakfast. FL, fast lunch. SL, slow lunch. Mean ± se.

Figure 6

Fig. 2. The energy intakes of the fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) breakfasts and the fast (n 6 meals) and slow (n 6 meals) lunch meals of all participants. The bars represent the mean ± se.

Figure 7

Fig. 3. Plot of the average eating rates and intakes of the fast breakfasts (FB; purple), slow breakfasts (SB; green), fast lunches (FL; purple) and slow lunches (SL; green) of all participants. The black dashed line represent the regression line of best fit based on average values. The grey dashed lines indicate the average eating rate (59 g/min) and average intake (447 kcal) of all meals.

Figure 8

Fig. 4. Correlation circle of multiple factor analysis performed on intake (blue; of all participants), eating behaviour (black; eating rate of all participants, other behaviours of subset of participants), liking familiarity and expected satiation (red; of all participants). The first two dimensions explain together 56 % of the variance. OSE, oro-sensory exposure.

Figure 9

Table 6. Repeated-measures correlation coefficients of food intake (g)

Supplementary material: File

Heuven et al. supplementary material

Heuven et al. supplementary material
Download Heuven et al. supplementary material(File)
File 9.3 MB