Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T05:48:55.679Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive performance and its relationship with postprandial metabolic changes after ingestion of different macronutrients in the morning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 March 2007

Karina Fischer*
Affiliation:
INW Nutrition Biology, Department of Agriculture and Food Science, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Paolo C. Colombani
Affiliation:
INW Nutrition Biology, Department of Agriculture and Food Science, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Wolfgang Langhans
Affiliation:
INW Physiology, Department of Agriculture and Food Science, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Caspar Wenk
Affiliation:
INW Nutrition Biology, Department of Agriculture and Food Science, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author: Karina Fischer, fax +41 1 632 1128, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

The effect of carbohydrate, protein and fat ingestion on simple as well as complex cognitive functions and the relationship between the respective postprandial metabolic changes and changes in cognitive performance were studied in fifteen healthy male students. Subjects were tested in three sessions, separated by 1 week, for short-term changes in blood variables, indirect calorimetry, subjective performance and different objective performance tasks using a repeated-measures counterbalanced cross-over design. Measurements were made after an overnight fast before and hourly during 3 h after test meal ingestion. Test meals consisted of either pure carbohydrates, protein or fat and were served as isoenergetic (1670 kJ) spoonable creams with similar sensory properties. Most aspects of subjective performance did not differ between test meals. For all objective tasks, however, postprandial cognitive performance was best after fat ingestion concomitant with an almost constant glucose metabolism and constant metabolic activation state measured by glucagon:insulin (G:I). In contrast, carbohydrate as well as protein ingestion resulted in lower overall cognitive performance, both together with partly marked changes (P<0.001) in glucose metabolism and metabolic activation. They also differently affected specific cognitive functions (P<0.05) in relation to their specific effect on metabolism. Carbohydrate ingestion resulted in relatively better short-term memory and accuracy of tasks concomitant with low metabolic activation, whereas protein ingestion resulted in better attention and efficiency of tasks concomitant with higher metabolic activation. Our findings support the concept that good and stable cognitive performance is related to a balanced glucose metabolism and metabolic activation state.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 2001

References

Bellisle, F, Blundell, JE, Dye, L, Fantino, M, Fern, E, Fletcher, RJ, Lambert, J, Roberfoid, M, Specter, S, Westenhofer, J & Westerterp–Plantenga, MS (1998) Functional food science and behaviour and psychological functions. British Journal of Nutrition 80 (Suppl. 1), S173S193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, EM, Growdon, JH, Wurtman, JJ, Caballero, B & Wurtman, RJ (1991) A balanced carbohydrate:protein diet in the management of Parkinson's disease. Neurology 41, 12951297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borrell, J, de–Kloet, ER & Bohus, B (1984) Corticosterone decreases the efficacy of adrenaline to affect passive avoidance retention of adrenalectomized rats. Life Sciences 34, 99104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahill, L & McGaugh, JL (1996) Modulation of memory storage. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 6, 237242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, A (1986) Acute effects of meals on perceptual and cognitive efficiency. Nutrition Reviews 44 Suppl., 163171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunliffe, A, Obeid, OA & Powell, TJ (1997) Post–prandial changes in measures of fatigue: effect of a mixed or a pure carbohydrate or pure fat meal. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 51, 831838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, EA, Soong, SA, Byrne, GC & Jones, TW (1996) Acute hyperglycaemia impairs cognitive function in children with IDDM. Journal of Pediatric Endocrinology and Metabolism 9, 455461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De–#Feo, P, Gallai, V, Mazzotta, G, Crispino, G, Torlone, E, Perriello, G, Ventura, MM, Santeusanio, F, Brunetti, P & Bolli, GB (1988) Modest decrements in plasma glucose concentration cause early impairment in cognitive function and later activation of glucose counterregulation in the absence of hypoglycemic symptoms in normal man. Journal of Clinical Investigation 82, 436444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, P & LeBlanc, J (1988) A role for insulin in cephalic phase of postprandial thermogenesis in dogs. American Journal of Physiology 254, E625E632.Google ScholarPubMed
Dolezal, V & Tucek, S (1982) Effects of choline and glucose on atropine–induced alterations of acetylcholine synthesis and content in the caudate nuclei of rats. Brain Research 240, 285293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durkin, TP, Messier, C, de–Boer, P & Westerink, BH (1992) Raised glucose levels enhance scopolamine–induced acetylcholine overflow from the hippocampus: an. in vivo microdialysis study in the rat Behavioural Brain Research 49, 181188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernstrom, JD (1977) Effects on the diet on brain neurotransmitters. Metabolism 26, 207223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernstrom, JD & Wurtman, RJ (1972) Elevation of plasma tryptophan by insulin in rat. Metabolism 21, 337342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fery, YA, Ferry, A, Vom, HA & Rieu, M (1997) Effect of physical exhaustion on cognitive functioning. Perceptual and Motor Skills 84, 291298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, PE (1995) Role of glucose in regulating the brain and cognition. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 61, 987S995S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, PE & Stone, WS (1988) Neuroendocrine effects on memory in aged rodents and humans. Neurobiology of Aging 9, 709717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, CS (1987) Metabolic control and auditory information processing at altered glucose levels in insulin–dependent diabetes. Brain and Cognition 6, 161174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt, SH, Miller, JC, Petocz, P & Farmakalidis, E (1995) A satiety index of common foods. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 49, 675690.Google ScholarPubMed
Huber, C (1994) Profile of Mood States, version 1.00. MÖdling. Austria: Dr G. Schuhfried GmbH.Google Scholar
Kanarek, R (1997) Psychological effects of snacks and altered meal frequency. British Journal of Nutrition 77(Suppl. 1), S105S118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keul, J, Huber, G, Lehmann, M, Berg, A & Jakob, EF (1982) Einfluss von Dextrose auf Fahrleistung, Konzentrationsfähigkeit, Kreislauf und Stoffwechsel im Kraftfahrzeug–Simulator (Doppelblindstudie im cross–over–design) (Influence of dextrose on driving performance, ability to concentrate, circulation and metabolism in the automobile simulator (Double–blind study in crossover design)). Aktuelle Ernährungsmedizin in Klinik und Praxis 7, 714.Google Scholar
Kopf, SR & Baratti, CM (1995) The impairment of retention induced by insulin in mice may be mediated by a reduction in central cholinergic activity. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 63, 220228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lä#ubli, T, Meile, U, Rosselli, E, Scheller, M, Sethre, T & Krueger, H (1998) Kombinationstest für psychotoxikologische Untersuchungen (Combi–test for psychotoxicological examinations). Schweizerische Medizinische Wochenschrift 128, 10S.Google Scholar
Lieberman, HR, Corkin, S, Spring, BJ, Wurtman, RJ & Growdon, JH (1985) The effects of dietary neurotransmitter precursors on human behavior. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 42, 366370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, HR, Spring, BJ & Garfield, GS (1986) The behavioral effects of food constituents: strategies used in studies of amino acids, protein, carbohydrate and caffeine. Nutrition Reviews 44(Suppl.), 6170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, HM, Green, MW & Rogers, PJ (1994) Mood and cognitive performance effects of isocaloric lunches differing in fat and carbohydrate content. Physiology and Behavior 56, 5157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNair, DM, Lorr, M & Droppleman, LF (1971) Profile of Mood States Manual. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar
Mackay, C, Cox, T, Burrows, G & Lazzerini, T (1978) An inventory for the measurement of self–reported stress and arousal. British Journal of Social Clinical Psychology 17, 283284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielson, KA & Jensen, RA (1994) Beta–adrenergic receptor antagonist antihypertensive medications impair arousal–induced modulation of working memory in elderly humans. Behavioral and Neural Biology 62, 190200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, OE, Morgan, AP, Kemp, HG, Sullivan, JM, Herrera, MG & Cahill, GF (1967) Brain metabolism during fasting. Journal of Clinical Investigation 46, 15891595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, DS & Benton, D (1994) The impact of raising blood glucose on reaction times. Neuropsychobiology 30, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parsons, MW & Gold, PE (1992) Glucose enhancement of memory in elderly humans: an inverted–U dose–response curve. Neurobiology of Aging 13, 401404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paz, A & Berry, EM (1997) Effect of meal composition on alertness and performance of hospital night–shift workers. Do mood and performance have different determinants?. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism 41, 291298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollitt, E, Leibel, RL & Greenfield, D (1981) Brief fasting, stress, and cognition in children. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 34, 15261533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porrini, M, Santangelo, A, Crovetti, R, Riso, P, Testolin, G & Blundell, JE (1997) Weight, protein, fat, and timing of preloads affect food intake. Physiology and Behavior 62, 563570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, PJ (1995) Food, mood and appetite. Nutrition Research Reviews 8, 243269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santeusanio, F, Bolli, G, Massi, BM, De–#Feo, P, Angeletti, G, Compagnucci, P, Calabrese, G & Brunetti, P (1981) Counterregulatory hormones during moderate, insulin–induced, blood glucose decrements in man. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 52, 477482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santiago, JV, Clarke, WL, Shah, SD & Cryer, PE (1980) Epinephrine, norepinephrine, glucagon, and growth hormone release in association with physiological decrements in the plasma glucose concentration in normal and diabetic man. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 51, 877883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santucci, AC, Schroeder, H & Riccio, DC (1990) Homeostatic disruption and memory: effect of insulin administration in rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology 53, 321333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sayegh, R, Schiff, I, Wurtman, J, Spiers, P, McDermott, J & Wurtman, R (1995) The effect of a carbohydrate–rich beverage on mood, appetite, and cognitive function in women with premenstrual syndrome. Obstetrics and Gynecology 86, 520528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlich, P (1993) Uses of change–over designs and repeated measurements in sensory and consumer studies. Food Quality and Preference 4, 223235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A, Kendrick, A, Maben, A & Salmon, J (1994) Effects of fat content, weight, and acceptability of the meal on postlunch changes in mood, performance, and cardiovascular function. Physiology and Behavior 55, 417422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A, Leekam, S, Ralph, A & McNeill, G (1988) The influence of meal composition on post–lunch changes in performance efficiency and mood. Appetite 10, 195203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spring, B, Maller, O, Wurtman, J, Digman, L & Cozolino, L (1982) Effects of protein and carbohydrate meals on mood and performance: interactions with sex and age. Journal of Psychiatric Research 17, 155167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, AB, McKane, WR, Bell, PM, Bell, P, King, DJ & Hayes, JR (1989) Psychomotor performance and counterregulatory responses during mild hypoglycemia in healthy volunteers. Diabetes Care 12, 1217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weir, JBdV (1949) New methods for calculating metabolic rate with special reference to protein metabolism. Journal of Physiology 109, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wurtman, JJ, Brzezinski, A, Wurtman, RJ & Laferrere, B (1989) Effect of nutrient intake on premenstrual depression. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 161, 12281234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wurtman, RJ & Fernstrom, JD (1975) Control of brain monoamine synthesis by diet and plasma amino acids. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 28, 638647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed