There has been a seismic shift in clinical understanding of social cognition over the past two decades. The idea that social cognition is separate to non-social cognition and can be independently affected is a relatively recent concept although descriptors of people with brain damage with normal cognition, but poor social skills have been in the literature for a long time. EVR, for example, was a highly intelligent individual who suffered extensive resection of orbit medial frontal cortex due to removal of a meningioma (Eslinger & Damasio, Reference Eslinger and Damasio1985). As a result, his cognitive skills on conventional tests remained superior, but his social conduct deteriorated suggestive of primary impairment of the social self.
Even so, it is only relatively recently that social cognition has come under systematic examination. One of the clearest, most cogent accounts of the unique aspects of social cognition was first provided by Penn, Corrigan, Bentall, Racenstein and Newman (Reference Penn, Corrigan, Bentall, Racenstein and Newman1997). They argued that, as schizophrenia was essentially an interpersonal disorder, the problems experienced by people with schizophrenia must be conceptualised as problems of social cognition. Further, while non-social cognitive impairments are likely to contribute to difficulties in the social realm, they argued that the perception and interpretation of social information could not simply be understood as a subset of general cognitive ability. They pointed out that social cognition requires the ability to make inferences about unobservable matters (such as thoughts), and to deal with mutable information that is personally relevant rather than neutral, objective and stable and likely to cause its own reaction in the observer and change as a result.
Since that time, there has been a groundswell of interest in social cognition and recognition of the pivotal role that it plays in community integration. Clinicians who work with people with brain disorders, whether these are developmental, psychiatric or due to acquired injury or disease, are only too aware that social and emotional impairments are a major obstacle to successful psychosocial functioning. Social cognition impairments not only characterise disorders of schizophrenia, but are also seen as the defining characteristic of Autism Spectrum Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and manifest in other intellectual and genetic developmental disabilities (Roelofs, Wingbermühle, Kessels, & Egger, Reference Roelofs, Wingbermühle, Egger and Kessels2017). In the field of traumatic brain injury (TBI), 84% of clinicians reported that disorders of social cognition affect at least half of their clients (Kelly, McDonald, & Frith, Reference Kelly, McDonald and Frith2016). Social cognitive disorders are also diagnostic of specific dementias, notably fronto-temporal dementias (Hazelton, Irish, Hodges, Piguet, & Kumfor, Reference Hazelton, Irish, Hodges, Piguet and Kumfor2017; Kipps, Nestor, Acosta-Cabronero, Arnold, & Hodges, Reference Kipps, Nestor, Acosta-Cabronero, Arnold and Hodges2009; Lough et al., Reference Lough, Kipps, Treise, Watson, Blair and Hodges2006).
Defining the Sub-components of Social Cognition
As a result of this increased interest in social cognition, the nature of its inter-related components is rapidly being delineated. At base, there is general agreement that social cognition refers to the ability to use social cues to infer the meaning and intentions behind the behaviour of others in order to respond in a socially adaptive manner (McDonald, Reference McDonald2013). Typically, social cognition is thought to include emotion perception, that is, the ability to recognise the emotional states of others from facial, vocal and body cues, and Theory of Mind (ToM), which is the ability to judge what another person may be thinking and the intentions behind their behaviour. ToM is critical to understanding everyday conversational pragmatic inferences such as sarcasm, hyperbole, hints, etc. Social cognition also includes empathy that refers to the capacity to ‘put oneself in another's shoes’, to see things from their point of view (cognitive empathy), or to share their feelings (affective empathy). To be able to do this, one must also be self-aware, able to distinguish between feelings for others and one's own feelings. Attributional bias, which refers to the personal filter through which the behaviour of others is interpreted (such as seeing ambiguous social behaviour as hostile) has been associated with schizophrenia (Savla, Vella, Armstrong, Penn, & Twamley, Reference Savla, Vella, Armstrong, Penn and Twamley2013) but is also relevant to other conditions such as brain injury (Cassels, McDonald, Kelly, & Togher, Reference Cassels, McDonald, Kelly and Togher2016). Other facets of social cognition such as the development of social knowledge and morality are particularly relevant to children in whom maturational processes are complicated by the presence of brain injury, as discussed in this volume (Chiasson, Elkaim, Well, Crevier, & Beauchamp, Reference Chiasson, Elkaim, Well, Crevier and Beauchamp2017). Social cognition also impinges on behaviour such as the capacity to make effective decisions to enhance prosocial engagement (Adlam, Adams, Turnbull, Yeates, & Gracey, Reference Adlam, Adams, Turnbull, Yeates and Gracey2017) while inhibiting antisocial urges (Honan, Allen, Fisher, Osborne-Crowley, & McDonald, Reference Honan, Allen, Fisher, Osborne-Crowley and McDonald2017).
Using Social Cognition to Predict Outcomes
Now that we are becoming better equipped to describe and identify problems in social cognition, we can use this information to better predict psychosocial outcomes. Indeed, social cognitive abilities have been shown to be better able to predict community function than non-social cognitive variables in people with schizophrenia (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, Van Os and Krabbendam2011). In adults with TBIs, problems recognising emotions in others have been shown to be predictive of poor communication skills and use of humour (McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, English, Randal, Longman, Togher and Tate2013; McDonald, Flanagan, Martin, & Saunders, Reference McDonald, Flanagan, Martin and Saunders2004; Milders, Ietswaart, Crawford, & Currie, Reference Milders, Ietswaart, Crawford and Currie2008; Watts & Douglas, Reference Watts and Douglas2006) and poor social integration (Knox & Douglas, Reference Knox and Douglas2009).
Children with brain disorders are especially vulnerable to disorders of social cognition as these skills underpin the maturation of skills needed for increasingly complex social interactions through childhood and adolescence (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Bigler, Dennis, Gerhardt, Rubin, Stancin and Vannatta2007). Long-term social outcomes following paediatric TBI are predicted by poor executive function, social information processing and pragmatic language skills (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Swift, Taylor, Wade, Drotar, Stancin and Minich2004). Further, while poor social outcomes have been frequently attributed to severe, diffuse injuries such as TBI, in this volume, Chiasson et al. (Reference Chiasson, Elkaim, Well, Crevier and Beauchamp2017) found that 15 children with focal brain lesions had impaired social cognitive skills, specifically moral reasoning, decision making and empathy. They also found these children experienced socio-behavioural problems suggesting a possible link between the two.
Changes in social cognition have been found to predict psychosocial function in dementia although this varies according to the nature of dementia profile. Loss of empathy predicts carer burden in semantic dementia and loss of a caring relationship in the behavioural variant of FTD (Hsieh, Irish, Daveson, Hodges, & Piguet, Reference Hsieh, Irish, Daveson, Hodges and Piguet2013). As reported in this volume, loss of affective empathy in people with primary progressive aphasic disorders is also associated with increased carer burden (Hazelton et al., Reference Hazelton, Irish, Hodges, Piguet and Kumfor2017). Interesting, while people with Alzheimer's disease (AD) also experience changes to social cognition, there is less evidence that this affects the quality of their relationships. Poveda, Laidlaw, Macleod, Power, & Osborne-Crowley (Reference Poveda, Laidlaw, Macleod, Power and Osborne-Crowley2017) report that while their group of adults with AD suffered problems with social cognition as assessed by The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT), this was not related to either carer distress or relationship continuity. It may be that deficits on performance-based social cognition measures in AD are less indicative of reduced socio-emotional functioning than the impact of non-social cognitive impairments, especially in the earlier stages of the disease. This may be why the correlation with carer relationships is not evident. Indeed, in a separate study, when people with AD were rated by their carers for their levels of empathy, they were found to have significantly higher cognitive and affective empathy than people with FTD. Further, empathy ratings did not predict carer burden in AD but did in FTD (Hsieh et al., Reference Hsieh, Irish, Daveson, Hodges and Piguet2013).
Social Cognition and the Psychosocial Context: A Two Way Street?
Overall, these studies show that, by focusing on social cognition, more nuanced relationships with psychosocial outcomes are revealed. This is a welcome advance given that assessments of (non-social) cognitive abilities in clinical groups have had only modest ability to predict everyday functioning (Odhuba, van den Broek, & Johns, Reference Odhuba, van den Broek and Johns2005; Silver, Reference Silver2000). Of course, it is also important to consider that factors other than the clinical disorder itself may impact upon social cognition. This is especially true when considering children, who are developing their social cognitive skills in the context of an injury or other disorder (Chiasson et al., Reference Chiasson, Elkaim, Well, Crevier and Beauchamp2017; Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Mihaljevic, Beauchamp, Catroppa, Crossley, Hearps and Anderson2017). Ryan et al.(Reference Ryan, Mihaljevic, Beauchamp, Catroppa, Crossley, Hearps and Anderson2017) present data on children with mild to severe TBI in which they found that severity of injury did not predict poor social cognition but family emotional responsivity was strongly associated. Thus, the nature of the family context and the emotional and interpersonal milieu in which the child is raised makes an important contribution to their social cognitive development independent of brain injury. There are numerous other factors that may interact with social cognitive skills that need to be considered in both children and adults at risk of social cognitive disorders. For example, mood disorders are associated with disorders of social cognition (George et al., Reference George, Huggins, McDermut, Parekh, Rubinow and Post1998; Lahera et al., Reference Lahera, Benito, Montes, Fernandez-Liria, Olbert and Penn2013), are highly prevalent in neurological conditions and may exacerbate or compound social cognitive impairments arising from brain injury (McDonald, Fisher, & Flanagan, Reference McDonald, Fisher and Flanagan2015).
Another factor that is often overlooked in social cognitive studies is that there is significant inter-individual variation in social cognitive skills that does not, necessarily, parallel non-social cognitive abilities. Importantly, unlike non-social cognition, there are gender differences in social cognition that are apparent from early childhood (Dadds et al., Reference Dadds, Hawes, Frost, Vassallo, Bunn, Hunter and Merz2009) through adolescence (McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Fisher, Togher, Tate, Rushby, English and Francis2015) with a female advantage for recognition of emotions from face and voice (Collignon et al., Reference Collignon, Girard, Gosselin, Saint-Amour, Lepore and Lassonde2010) and self-reported empathy (Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Bowen, Holt, Allison, Auyeung, Lombardo and Lai2015). As reported in this volume, this advantage appears to maintain even following brain injury (Zupan, Babbage, Neumann, & Willer, Reference Zupan, Babbage, Neumann and Willer2017) and needs to be considered when evaluating levels of social cognitive impairment.
Issues in the Clinical Assessment of Social Cognition
The assessment of non-social cognition has a long tradition. Clinicians have access to standardised tests that have been extensively researched and normed and, in many cases, translated across languages. They are also well defined according to which profession has responsibility for the particular domain (language = speech pathology, memory = neuropsychology, etc.). This is not the case for the relatively new area of social cognition. While there are tools available from the clinical and experimental literature, assessment of social cognition is not the norm. Indeed, as reported by Kelly, Frith, & McDonald (Reference Kelly, Frith and McDonald2017) in this volume, less than 16% of clinicians report using any kind of formal assessment of social cognition.
Further, there appears to be differences within professions as to who is held responsible for the assessment of social cognition with clinical psychologists, clinical neuropsychologists, occupational therapists and speech pathologists each believing the responsibility primarily lies with the other (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, McDonald and Frith2016). This is probably inevitable given that social cognition is a new domain that crosses boundaries between perception, executive function, emotional processing, personality, behaviour, language and everyday function. The coming decade will be interesting to see who claims social cognition within their professional realm of expertise.
Assessing Emotion Perception, ToM and Pragmatic Inference
A major similarity between social cognition and non-social cognition is that many attributes, such as emotion perception, ToM, recognition of pragmatic inferences (which rely on inferring the intention of speakers), moral judgements etc., can be assessed with performative tasks with associated normative data. There is huge scope to develop such tests. My own test, TASIT (McDonald, Flanagan, Rollins, & Kinch, Reference McDonald, Flanagan, Rollins and Kinch2003), which uses audio-visual vignettes of actors, has proven to be useful in a range of clinical disorders including TBI, schizophrenia, right hemisphere damage, autism spectrum disorders and frontotemporal dementia (McDonald, Reference McDonald2012). However, it only assesses specific aspects of social cognition, namely emotion perception, ToM and conversational inference, and specifically as these pertains to adults and adolescents. These aspects of social cognition are the most commonly assessed. For example, several other batteries such as the Florida Affect Battery (Bowers, Blonder, & Heilman, Reference Bowers, Blonder and Heilman1991) and the Diagnostic Analysis of Non-Verbal Accuracy (Nowicki, Reference Nowicki2010) also assess emotion perception and are backed up with normative data. The child assessment battery NEPSY-II (Korkman, Kirk, & Kemp, Reference Korkman, Kirk and Kemp2007) assesses both emotion and ToM in children from 3 to16 years. Some speech pathology language assessments such as the Social Language Development Test (Bowers, Huisingh, & LoGiudice, Reference Bowers, Huisingh and LoGiudice2010) and the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (Wiig & Secord, Reference Wiig and Secord2014) assess conversational and pragmatic inference from child to early adulthood.
One question for assessment, even of these well-established constructs, is whether performance as assessed using de-contextualised social stimuli, is actually indicative of real-life judgements in situations where far more cues are typically present. Turkstra and colleagues (Reference Turkstra, Kraning, Riedeman, Mutlu, Duff and VanDenHeuve2017) in this volume examine this issue with respect to emotion recognition. Reassuringly, they found that de-contextualised facial expressions (as typically presented) were recognised as accurately as those placed in context in both normal healthy adults and people with brain injury. However, they also found that when allowed to respond in a free format rather than multiple choice, at least 50% of responses could not be categorised as one of the six ‘basic’ emotions (happy, sad, angry, disgusted, fearful and surprised) supposedly depicted by the stimuli and that people with TBI gave quite different responses. This raises important questions about how we should be assessing emotion perception.
Another criticism of established social cognition tests is that they usually require making third person judgements, for example, passively watching a scene and judging what the people in the scene are doing and thinking about each other. In contrast, we are usually participants in the social contexts in which we make social judgements. Some of these judgements will require interpreting social cues that are directed towards ourselves. One way to enhance the interactive nature of social cognitive assessments is the use of virtual reality. Canty, Neumann, Fleming and Shum (Reference Canty, Neumann, Fleming and Shum2015) developed such a measure where actors in videoed contexts talk directly to camera, giving a first person quality to the interactions. This test that measures both cognitive and affective mental judgements proved to be sensitive to social functioning and holds great promise as a clinical assessment instrument.
Assessing Morality, Decision Making and Social Inhibition
For other aspects of social cognition there are, in fact, very few clinical instruments. As a significant advance, in this volume, Chiasson and colleagues (Reference Chiasson, Elkaim, Well, Crevier and Beauchamp2017) describe a social moral assessment instrument that has proven sensitive to brain injury in children and is useful for determining whether children have reached appropriate developmental stages in their moral reasoning. As with the instrument devised by Canty et al. (Reference Canty, Neumann, Fleming and Shum2015), the The Socio-Moral Reasoning Aptitude Level Task (So-Moral: Beauchamp, Dooley, & Anderson, Reference Beauchamp, Dooley and Anderson2013) uses visual vignettes from a first person perspective to increase participant engagement. Furthermore, So-Moral has been adapted into an iPad version, PEERS, which is specifically for clinical use and near completion with 500+ child norms already collected (Anderson, personal communication). The translation of experimental social cognitive tasks into clinical tools is a very welcome advance. It is not always, however, a straightforward process as is apparent in efforts to find a good measure of social decision making. Decision making in socio-emotional contexts has been assessed in the past using the Iowa Gambling Test (IGT) (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, Reference Bechara, Damasio, Tranel and Damasio2005). This card based gambling task purports to tap into ‘somatic markers’, whereby individuals are guided by their own physiological responses. These responses are triggered by anticipation of reward or punishment in complex situations and may index learning even before the individual is consciously aware of the contingency (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, Reference Bechara, Damasio, Tranel and Damasio1997). The IGT is a complex test and its practical use in clinical assessment is questionable. In this volume, Adlam and colleagues (Reference Adlam, Adams, Turnbull, Yeates and Gracey2017) test the clinical utility of a shorter, simpler card-based gambling test, the Bangor Gambling Task and find that this, also, has problems associated with its sensitivity to decision making impairment. Another option is to assess decision making in a clearly social context. Kelly, McDonald and Kellet (Reference Kelly, McDonald and Kellet2014) developed the Social Decision Making Test (SDMT) where, rather than winning at cards, the aim was to be included by others in a virtual ‘ball game’. The SDMT was sensitive to brain injury where the IGT was not, so this may be the way forward in developing tasks of social decision making.
Another important facet of social cognition is the ability to inhibit socially undesirable responses for those that are more likely to enhance social interactions. There are a number of measures of inhibition within the neuropsychological domain such as the Haylings Sentence Completion Test (Burgess & Shallice, Reference Burgess and Shallice1997) and various Stroop type tasks. Once again, however, these lack a social context so whether they are likely to be sensitive to specific loss of social inhibition is unclear. One approach with which to address this dilemma was taken by Osborne-Crowley, McDonald and Francis (Reference Osborne-Crowley, McDonald and Francis2016) using an interview format in which participants were asked open questions such as ‘tell me about an embarrassing moment you have had’. Independent raters identified people with brain injury as having greater self-disclosure and disinhibition than controls and this correlated with relative's reports of disinhibited behaviour indicating good validity. It was not associated with traditional neuropsychological measures of inhibition, strengthening the argument that these constructs may be different (Osborne-Crowley et al., Reference Osborne-Crowley, McDonald and Francis2016). The drawback is this measure is not easy to use clinically as it requires two raters to achieve good reliability.
An alternative approach has been developed by Honan et al. (Reference Honan, Allen, Fisher, Osborne-Crowley and McDonald2017) and reported in this volume. Modelled on the Haylings Sentence Completion test, the Social Disinhibition Test (SDT) uses pictures of people engaged in awkward social situations and requires participants to describe characters in the scenes under three conditions: without restriction; with the requirement that nothing negative is stated and with requirement that something positive is stated. Scores for items range from 0 to 2. People with brain injury had difficulty especially when required not to be negative. A relationship was found between the SDT and the Haylings test as well as TASIT, suggesting it taps into disinhibition as measured more broadly and also social cognition. Disappointingly, the relationship with informant rated loss of (emotional) control was low. This may reflect the fact that emotional loss of control is different to verbal disinhibition, but further research is needed to determine this.
Using Questionnaires to Measure Social Cognition
Performative tests of social cognition can be supplemented by self or informant-report questionnaires. For some attributes of social cognition, notable empathy, self-report questionnaires are highly appropriate as empathy is essentially a subjective state. While there are concern that many people with brain injury may have limited insight and unable to complete questionnaires reliably, research has demonstrated that this need not be the case (Kinsella, Moran, Ford & Ponsford, Reference Kinsella, Moran, Ford and Ponsford1988). Numerous studies have used self-report empathy questionnaires such as the Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale (Mehrabian, Reference Mehrabian2000), the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI: Davis, Reference Davis1983) and the Empathy Quotient (EQ: Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, Reference Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright2004) to report deficits in empathy following brain injury (de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, McDonald, Rushby, Li, Dimoska and James2011; Neumann, Zupan, Malec, & Hammond, Reference Neumann, Zupan, Malec and Hammond2013; Rushby et al., Reference Rushby, McDonald, Fisher, Kornfeld, De Blasio, Parks and Piguet2016; Wood & Williams, Reference Wood and Williams2008) and also Autism Spectrum Disorders (Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Bowen, Holt, Allison, Auyeung, Lombardo and Lai2015).
If it is unlikely that the person can complete a questionnaire reliably, a close informant can provide a reasonable assessment of empathic behaviour. A number of studies have used the IRI filled in by a close relative to examine loss of empathy in people with dementia. Using this, loss of empathy has been found in the frontotemporal dementias, including the behavioural variant (Dermody et al., Reference Dermody, Wong, Ahmed, Piguet, Hodges and Irish2016) and semantic dementia, but not AD (Rankin, Kramer, & Miller, Reference Rankin, Kramer and Miller2005). Further, in this volume, Hazelton et al. (Reference Hazelton, Irish, Hodges, Piguet and Kumfor2017) demonstrate that people with dementia characterised as non-fluent primary progressive aphasia, i.e., both progressive non-fluent aphasia and logopenic progressive aphasia also experience reduced cognitive and affective empathy.
The Push for Rehabilitation of Social Cognition
Rehabilitation of social cognitive deficits has a relatively long tradition, especially in schizophrenia and to a lesser extent developmental conditions such as Autism Spectrum Disorders. In contrast, there is relatively little remediation research for social cognition impairments in people with acquired brain injuries (see Cassels et al. (Reference Cassels, McDonald, Kelly and Togher2016) for review). The primary focus of social cognitive remediation has been to improve skills in emotion perception, ToM and attributional bias. Treatment has been conducted with a range of approaches from a singular treatment focused on a particular skill to more comprehensive approaches targeting two or more such skills and possibly also non-social cognition. As reported in this volume, Roelofs Wingbermühle, Egger, & Kessels (Reference Roelofs, Wingbermühle, Egger and Kessels2017) conducted a meta-analysis of 41 studies reporting on treatment of social cognitive disorders in over 1500 patients with acquired brain disorders or neuropsychiatric symptoms. Encouragingly, they found that targeted treatments were especially effective, particularly for emotion perception, ToM and social perception (the ability to use social cues to determine social roles, rules, etc). Comprehensive and broad based interventions were effective also, and had impact upon social functioning.
This meta-analysis also attempted to include alexithymia, which is a disorder in emotional self-awareness, however, no studies met inclusion criteria. Self-awareness is an important ingredient in social cognition, because to have empathy, one must be able to both share feelings with another while recognising that one's own feelings are separate. Indeed, it is increasingly thought that much of social cognition relies upon the ability to simulate the experiences of others in oneself (Decety & Grèzes, Reference Decety and Grèzes2006), with the attendant need to know the difference between the two. In general, there are few studies that have attempted to address emotional self-awareness in neuropsychiatric and neurological disorders, although remediation of self-awareness more generally has been the subject of research in acquired brain injury (Ownsworth, Fleming, Desbois, Strong, & Kuipers, Reference Ownsworth, Fleming, Desbois, Strong and Kuipers2006; Ownsworth, McFarland, & Young, Reference Ownsworth, McFarland and Young2000). In this volume, Lamberts, Fasotti, Boelen and Spikman (Reference Lamberts, Fasotti, Boelen and Spikman2017) report on a treatment trial that used self-monitoring to improve executive function. They found that self-awareness was improved as a result. Importantly, emotion perception was significantly correlated to self-awareness pre-treatment. This accord with the view that self-awareness is a central construct to social cognition. It also raises the important point that social cognitive deficits may present an obstacle to any rehabilitation, if the treatment relies heavily on social interaction with the therapists and other members of the treatment group.
Conclusion
In conclusion, social cognition is emerging as a highly relevant target for clinical management in a wide range of neurological, developmental and psychiatric disorders. Because it is a relatively new domain of cognition, clinicians are yet to decide who will take responsibility for its assessment and management. New tests are needed that take into account the special features of social cognition, including the interactive nature of the relationship between the observer and his/her social context and inherent differences in social cognitive ability reflecting individual differences in gender, family upbringing, mood and potentially many other social-emotional influences. There is good reason to be optimistic that interventions can be effective for social cognition, but new research is needed to apply these to people with acquired brain injury, and to really consider the importance of self-awareness in social cognitive ability. The papers in this volume represent exciting new initiatives looking at each of these topics.