Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T04:30:52.716Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Testing the social competition hypothesis of depression using a simple economic game

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Aleksandra Kupferberg*
Affiliation:
Psychiatric University Hospital, Division of Molecular Psychiatry, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
Oliver M. Hager
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital; Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research, Department of Economics, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Urs Fischbacher
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, Economics University, Konstanz, Germany; Thurgau Institute of Economics, Kreuzlingen, Switzerland
Laura S. Brändle
Affiliation:
Psychiatric University Hospital, Division of Molecular Psychiatry, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
Melanie Haynes
Affiliation:
Psychiatric University Hospital, Division of Molecular Psychiatry, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
Gregor Hasler
Affiliation:
Psychiatric University Hospital, Division of Molecular Psychiatry, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
*
Gregor Hasler, Psychiatric University Hospital, Division of Molecular Psychiatry, University of Bern, Bolligenstrasse 111, CH-3000 Bern 60, Switzerland. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.
Background

Price's social competition hypothesis interprets the depressive state as an unconscious, involuntary losing strategy, which enables individuals to yield and accept defeat in competitive situations.

Aims

We investigated whether patients who suffer from major depressive disorder (MDD) would avoid competition more often than either patients suffering from borderline personality disorder (BPD) or healthy controls.

Method

In a simple paper-folding task healthy participants and patiens with MDD and BPD were matched with two opponents, one with an unknown diagnosis and one who shared their clinical diagnosis, and they had to choose either a competitive or cooperative payment scheme for task completion.

Results

When playing against an unknown opponent, but not the opponent with the same diagnosis, the patients with depression chose the competitive payment scheme statistically less often than healthy controls and patients diagnosed with BPD.

Conclusion

The competition avoidance against the unknown opponent is consistent with Price's social competition hypothesis.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Non-Commercial, No Derivatives (CC BY-NC-ND) licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2016

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

G.H. received research support, consulting fees and speaker honoraria from Lundbeck, AstraZeneca, Servier, Eli Lilly, Roche and Novartis.

References

1 Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Demler, O, Jin, R, Koretz, D, Merikangas, KR, et al. The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). JAMA 2003; 289: 3095–105.Google Scholar
2 Mathers, CD, Loncar, D. Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med 2006; 3: e442.Google Scholar
3 Shapira, B, Zislin, J, Gelfin, Y, Osher, Y, Gorfine, M, Souery, D, et al. Social adjustment and self-esteem in remitted patients with unipolar and bipolar affective disorder: a case-control study. Compr Psychiatry 1999; 40: 2430.Google Scholar
4 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth DEdition, Text ERevision: DSM-IV-TR. American Psychiatric Publishing, 2000.Google Scholar
5 Weissman, MM, Paykel, ES, Siegel, R, Klerman, GL. The social role performance of depressed women: comparisons with a normal group. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1971; 41: 390405.Google Scholar
6 Beach, SRH, Sandeen, E, O'Leary, KD. Depression in Marriage: A Model for Etiology and Treatment. Guilford Press, 1990.Google Scholar
7 Hammen, C, Brennan, PA. Depressed adolescents of depressed and nondepressed mothers: tests of an interpersonal impairment hypothesis. J Consult Clin Psychol 2001; 69: 284–94.Google Scholar
8 Lewinsohn, PM, Rohde, P, Seeley, JR. Major depressive disorder in older adolescents: prevalence, risk factors, and clinical implications. Clin Psychol Rev 1998; 18: 765–94.Google Scholar
9 Zlotnick, C, Kohn, R, Keitner, G, Della Grotta, SA. The relationship between quality of interpersonal relationships and major depressive disorder: findings from the National Comorbidity Survey. J Affect Disord 2000; 59: 205–15.Google Scholar
10 Whooley, MA, Kiefe, CI, Chesney, MA, Markovitz, JH, Matthews, K, Hulley, SB, et al. Depressive symptoms, unemployment, and loss of income: the CARDIA study. Arch Intern Med 2002; 162: 2614–20.Google Scholar
11 Kessler, RC, Walters, EE, Forthofer, MS. The social consequences of psychiatric disorders, III: probability of marital stability. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155: 1092–6.Google Scholar
12 Kessler, RC, Berglund, PA, Foster, CL, Saunders, WB, Stang, PE, Walters, EE. Social consequences of psychiatric disorders, II: teenage parenthood. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154: 1405–11.Google Scholar
13 Kessler, RC, Foster, CL, Saunders, WB, Stang, PE. Social consequences of psychiatric disorders, I: educational attainment. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152: 1026–32.Google Scholar
14 Merikangas, KR. Divorce and assortative mating among depressed patients. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141: 74–6.Google Scholar
15 Ingram, RE, Price, JM. Vulnerability to Psychopathology, Second PEdition: Risk Across the Lifespan. Guilford Press, 2009.Google Scholar
16 Zhang, H, Sun, D, Lee, TMC. Impaired social decision making in patients with major depressive disorder. Brain Behav 2012; 2: 415–23.Google Scholar
17 Clark, C, Thorne, CB, Hardy, S, Cropsey, KL. Cooperation and depressive symptoms. J Affect Disord 2013; 150: 1184–7.Google Scholar
18 McClure, EB, Parrish, JM, Nelson, EE, Easter, J, Thorne, JF, Rilling, JK, et al. Responses to conflict and cooperation in adolescents with anxiety and mood disorders. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2007; 35: 567–77.Google Scholar
19 Andrews, PW, Thomson, JA. The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems. Psychol Rev 2009; 116: 620–54.Google Scholar
20 Price, J, Sloman, L, Gardner, R Jr, Gilbert, JP, Rohde, P. The social competition hypothesis of depression. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 164: 309–15.Google Scholar
21 Price, JS, Gardner, R Jr, Erickson, JM. Can depression, anxiety and somatization be understood as appeasement displays? J Affect Disord 2004; 79: 111.Google Scholar
22 Sloman, L. A new comprehensive evolutionary model of depression and anxiety. J Affect Disord 2008; 106: 219–28.Google Scholar
23 Brown, GW, Harris, TO, Hepworth, C. Loss, humiliation and entrapment among women developing depression: a patient and non-patient comparison. Psychol Med 1995; 25: 721.Google Scholar
24 Gilbert, P. Depression: The Evolution of Powerlessness. Guilford Press, 1992.Google Scholar
25 Malatynska, E, Pinhasov, A, Crooke, JJ, Smith-Swintosky, VL, Brenneman, DE. Reduction of dominant or submissive behaviors as models for antimanic or antidepressant drug testing: technical considerations. J Neurosci Methods 2007; 165: 175–82.Google Scholar
26 Pinhasov, A, Crooke, J, Rosenthal, D, Brenneman, D, Malatynska, E. Reduction of submissive behavior model for antidepressant drug activity testing: study using a video-tracking system. Behav Pharmacol 2005; 16: 657–64.Google Scholar
27 Camerer, C, Fehr, E. Measuring social norms and preferences using experimental games: a guide for social scientists. In Henrich, J, Boyd, R, Bowles, S, Camerer, C, Fehr, E, Gintis, H (eds) Foundations of Human Sociality: Economic Experiments and Ethnographic Evidence from Fifteen Small-Scale Societies: 5596. Oxford University Press, 2004.Google Scholar
28 Niederle, M, Vesterlund, L. Do women shy away from competition? Do men compete too much? Q J Econ 2007; 122: 1067–101.Google Scholar
29 Stanley, B, Siever, LJ. The interpersonal dimension of borderline personality disorder: toward a neuropeptide model. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 167: 2439.Google Scholar
30 Gunderson, JG. Borderline Personality Disorder, Second DEdition: A Clinical Guide. American Psychiatric Publishing, 2009.Google Scholar
31 Datta Gupta, N, Poulsen, A, Villeval, MC. Male and Female Competitive Behavior: Experimental Evidence. Institute for the Study of Labor, 2005.Google Scholar
32 Kivikangas, JM, Kätsyri, J, Järvelä, S, Ravaja, N. Gender differences in emotional responses to cooperative and competitive game play. PLoS One 2014; 9: e100318.Google Scholar
33 Hautzinger, M, Bailer, M, Worrall, H, Keller, F. Das Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI). Überarbeitet und ergänzte Neuauflage. Hans Huber, 1995.Google Scholar
34 Wittchen, H-U, Fydrich, T, Zaudig, M. SKID: Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV; Achse I und II. Achse II: Persönlichkeitstörungen. SKID-II. Hogrefe, Verlag Hfür Psychologie, 1997.Google Scholar
35 Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Ball, R, Ranieri, WF. Comparison of beck depression inventories-IA and II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess 1996; 67: 588–97.Google Scholar
36 Montgomery, SA, Asberg, M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry 1979; 134: 382–9.Google Scholar
37 Zanarini, MC, Vujanovic, AA, Parachini, EA, Boulanger, JL, Frankenburg, FR, Hennen, J. Zanarini Rating Scale for Borderline Personality Disorder (ZAN-BPD): a continuous measure of DSM-IV borderline psychopathology. J Personal Disord 2003; 17: 233–42.Google Scholar
38 Fydrich, T, Sommer, G, Tydecks, S, Brähler, E. Fragebogen zur sozialen Unterstützung (F-SozU): Normierung der Kurzform (K-14) [Social Support Questionnaire (F-SozU): standardization of short form (K-14)]. Z Für Med Psychol 2009; 18: 43–8.Google Scholar
39 Watson, D, Clark, LA, Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 1063–70.Google Scholar
40 Gilbert, P, McEwan, K, Bellew, R, Mills, A, Gale, C. The dark side of competition: how competitive behaviour and striving to avoid inferiority are linked to depression, anxiety, stress and self-harm. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract 2009; 82: 123–36.Google Scholar
41 Skodol, AE, Grilo, CM, Keyes, KM, Geier, T, Grant, BF, Hasin, DS. Relationship of personality disorders to the course of major depressive disorder in a nationally representative sample. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168: 257–64.Google Scholar
42 Margraf, J, Beck, AT, Ehlers, A. Beck-Angst-Inventar: BAI; Manual. Harcourt Test Services, 2007.Google Scholar
43 Jørgensen, CR, Freund, C, Bøye, R, Jordet, H, Andersen, D, Kjølbye, M. Outcome of mentalization-based and supportive psychotherapy in patients with borderline personality disorder: a randomized trial. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2013; 127: 305–17.Google Scholar
44 Turner, RM. Naturalistic evaluation of dialectical behavior therapy-oriented treatment for borderline personality disorder. Cogn Behav Pract 2000; 7: 413–19.Google Scholar
45 Horesh, N, Sever, J, Apter, A. A comparison of life events between suicidal adolescents with major depression and borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry 2003; 44: 277–83.Google Scholar
46 Shankman, SA, Nelson, BD, Sarapas, C, Robison-Andrew, EJ, Campbell, ML, Altman, SE, et al. A psychophysiological investigation of threat and reward sensitivity in individuals with panic disorder and/or major depressive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2013; 122: 322–38.Google Scholar
47 Henriques, JB, Davidson, RJ. Decreased responsiveness to reward in depression. Cogn Emot 2000; 14: 711–24.Google Scholar
48 Pizzagalli, DA, Iosifescu, D, Hallett, LA, Ratner, KG, Fava, M. Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatr Res 2008; 43: 7687.Google Scholar
49 Gilbert, P, Psychotherapy and Counselling for Depression. Sage, 2007.Google Scholar
50 Leary, MR. Self Presentation: Impression Management and Interpersonal Behavior. Westview Press, 1996.Google Scholar
51 Dykman, BM. Integrating cognitive and motivational factors in depression: initial tests of a goal-orientation approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998; 74: 139–58.Google Scholar
52 Gilbert, P. Human Nature and Suffering. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1992.Google Scholar
53 Allan, S, Gilbert, P. Submissive behaviour and psychopathology. Br J Clin Psychol 1997; 36: 467–88.Google Scholar
54 Gilbert, P, Allan, S. The role of defeat and entrapment (arrested flight) in depression: an exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychol Med 1998; 28: 585–98.Google Scholar
55 Thibaut, JW, Kelley, HH. The Social Psychology of Groups. Wiley, 1959.Google Scholar
56 Hagen, EH. Evolutionary theories of depression: a critical review. Can J Psychiatry 2011; 56: 716–26.Google Scholar
57 Watson, PJ, Andrews, PW. Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: the social navigation hypothesis. J Affect Disord 2002; 72: 114.Google Scholar
58 Von Helversen, B, Wilke, A, Johnson, T, Schmid, G, Klapp, B. Performance benefits of depression: sequential decision making in a healthy sample and a clinically depressed sample. J Abnorm Psychol 2011; 120: 962–8.Google Scholar
59 Schmidt, PJ, Daly, RC, Bloch, M, Smith, MJ, Danaceau, MA, St Clair, LS, et al. Dehydroepiandrosterone monotherapy in midlife-onset major and minor depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: 154–62.Google Scholar
60 Peixoto, C, Devicari Cheda, JN, Nardi, AE, Veras, AB, Cardoso, A. The effects of dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) in the treatment of depression and depressive symptoms in other psychiatric and medical illnesses: a systematic review. Curr Drug Targets 2014; 15: 901–14.Google Scholar
61 Khera, M. Patients with testosterone deficit syndrome and depression. Arch Esp Urol 2013; 66: 729–36.Google Scholar
62 Zarrouf, FA, Artz, S, Griffith, J, Sirbu, C, Kommor, M. Testosterone and depression: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Pract 2009; 15: 289305.Google Scholar
63 Mehta, PH, Josephs, RA. Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to compete again. Horm Behav 2006; 50: 684–92.Google Scholar
64 Hasler, G, Drevets, WC, Manji, HK, Charney, DS. Discovering endophenotypes for major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29: 1765–81.Google Scholar
65 Hasler, G, Northoff, G. Discovering imaging endophenotypes for major depression. Mol Psychiatry 2011; 16: 604–19.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Kupferberg et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Kupferberg et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 93.6 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.