Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T18:51:49.518Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship between view of context, psychosocial malaise and problematic internet use: mediation analysis using partial least squares structural equation modelling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 June 2022

Lucrezia Ferrante*
Affiliation:
Department of History, Society and Human Studies, University of Salento, Lecce, Italy
Claudia Venuleo
Affiliation:
Department of History, Society and Human Studies, University of Salento, Lecce, Italy
Giovanna Alessia Sternativo
Affiliation:
Department of History, Society and Human Studies, University of Salento, Lecce, Italy
Simone Rollo
Affiliation:
Department of History, Society and Human Studies, University of Salento, Lecce, Italy
Jun-Hwa Cheah
Affiliation:
School of Business and Economics, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Seri Kembangan, Selangor, Malaysia
Sergio Salvatore
Affiliation:
Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University of Roma ‘La Sapienza’, Rome, Italy; and Department of History, Society and Human Studies, University of Salento, Lecce, Italy
Enrico Ciavolino
Affiliation:
Department of History, Society and Human Studies, University of Salento, Lecce, Italy
*
Correspondence: Lucrezia Ferrante. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

According to more recent approaches on problematic internet use (PIU), using the internet can be seen as a way of compensating for psychosocial malaise. Taking semiotic cultural psychology theory as its theoretical framework, this study examines the role of affect-laden assumptions concerning the world, known as latent dimensions of sense (LDSs), in promoting (or not) adaptive responses, including internet use as a maladaptive strategy against problems and difficulties.

Aims

To test a theoretical model in which PIU is predicted by LDSs through the mediation of high levels of psychosocial malaise.

Method

We measured PIU (using the Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale 2), LDSs (View of Context questionnaire), negative affect (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule), social anxiety (Interaction Anxiousness Scale) and loneliness (Italian Loneliness Scale) in 764 Italian adolescents (mean age 15.05 years, s.d. = 1.152 years). LDSs were detected using a multiple correspondence analysis; after confirmatory composite analysis, partial least squares structural equation modelling with higher-order components was performed to test the mediation model.

Results

The results show a relationship between LDSs corresponding to an extreme negative evaluation of the sociocultural context, experienced as absolutely unreliable, and PIU through the mediation of psychosocial malaise (95% CI 0.101– 0.171; P = 0.000).

Conclusions

Overall, the findings suggest that PIU might be a way of compensating for unpleasant states in a context perceived in an extremely negative and homogenising way, i.e. as totally lacking resources and trustworthy people.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

The internet is an integral element of today's life, not just allowing people to interact each other, but especially offering potentially unlimited possibilities in commercial, entertainment, information as well as professional and academic fields. Its rapid and pervasive spread has raised questions among scholars about its impact on people's health; the condition of a clinically significant impairment in the personal, social and academic/work spheres is known as problematic internet use (PIU).Reference Anderson, Steen and Stavropoulos1

Despite there being no consensus on conceptualisation of PIU,Reference Ferrante and Venuleo2 a well-recognised aspect is that it may serve as a way of coping with unpleasant moods and experiences.Reference Caplan3

Prevalence estimates vary considerably in PIU studies, ranging from 1 to 36%, probably reflecting population differences, plurality of instruments and different criteria for PIU behaviours.Reference Ioannidis, Treder, Chamberlain, Kiraly, Redden and Stein4 According to the European Parliamentary Research Service,Reference Lopez Fernandez and Kuss5 the majority of individuals with problems related to internet use are adolescents. In Italy, the context of the current study, there is a substantial lack of studies assessing the prevalence of PIU in adolescent samples.

One of the most popular interpretations of adolescents’ vulnerability to problematic behaviours sees teenagers as having characteristics connected to their developmental age that make them vulnerable to substance and behavioural addictions. Although there is no doubt that adolescents share some characteristics (e.g. the level of development of the nervous system or increased levels of sensation-seeking and greater difficulties in emotion regulation) that differentiate them from other groups,Reference Chambers, Taylor and Potenza6 the insistence on common aspects is also critical, because it tends to obscure differences related to psychosocial factors that may play a role in exposing them to the risk of PIU.Reference Fumero, Marrero, Voltes and Penate7 A compensating hypothesis describing the link between social variables and extensive media use was first introduced by Davis & Kraus;Reference Davis and Kraus8 more recently, Kardefelt-WintherReference Kardefelt-Winther9 proposed a ‘compensatory internet use model’, which explains PIU development and maintenance as the result of an unhealthy, persistent, rigid use of the internet to deal with negative affective experiences, such as social anxiety and depression.Reference Venuleo, Ferrante and Rollo10 Several studies have considered the role of intrapsychic factors in adolescent PIU, such as depression and anxiety, personality traits and low self-esteem.Reference Obeid, Saade, Haddad, Sacre, Khansa and Al Hajj11 Other studies addressed the role of factors related to the interpersonal sphere, such as family and school environment.Reference Sela, Zach, Amichay-Hamburger, Mishali and Omer12, Reference Martins, Formiga, Santos, Sousa, Resende and Campos13

Less attention has been paid to the role of the sociocultural environment in increasing psychosocial malaise and in constructing the meaning of internet use. In this paper, we argue that this role needs to be considered for at least two reasons. First, the sociocultural environment itself may act as a source of malaise when it constrains people's lives and lacks resources and opportunities (e.g. formative and professional chances; recreational settings such as cinema, theatres, cultural associations) essential for young people's growth, including supportive social networks. Internet use may be a way to satisfy sociability and identity needs. Second, the cultural environment offers the semiotic resources and cues underlying ways of perceiving and experiencing and therefore dealing with the social world.Reference Salvatore, Rosa and Valsiner14 For instance, it is reasonable that person A, who sees the social environment as devoid of development opportunities and inhabited by selfish people, and person B, who expresses trust in people from their community and sees the context as full of resources, express different levels of social malaise, as well as a different way of using the internet, for instance as a maladaptive strategy against feelings/situations perceived as unsustainable.

Framed within semiotic cultural psychology theory,Reference Salvatore, Rosa and Valsiner14 the present study postulates and emphasises the need to consider the deep interconnection between the individual and society in understanding maladaptive patterns of behaviour like PIU. A semiotic, cultural and psychosocial perspective allows us to reflect on the role of cultural context, understood as a net of interconnected meanings, grounding the way of perceiving and experiencing a social environment, and enabling individuals to orient themselves in their material and social world; in other words, such an approach leads to investigating PIU not as the result of an intrapsychic structure, rather as the precipitate of a specific modality of relationship between the individual and the meanings active within the semiotic context in which they are inscribed.Reference Salvatore, Rosa and Valsiner14

Not neglecting the role of both the individual and relational dimensions, the study aims to contribute to the existing debate on adolescent PIU by considering sociocultural features in terms of adolescents’ views of their context. Indeed, available resources and opportunities, needs and motivation, social values and norms may act as constraints on the multiple ways adolescents can think and act, including the willingness to engage in problematic behaviours to compensate for their malaise. Recognising that the individual level dynamically interacts with the sociocultural level can offer a contribution to the way PIU – as well as problematic behaviours in general – can not only be understood, but also addressed: if PIU is not just a matter of individual health, multi-level interventions will be able to consider the individual and systemic dimensions in integrated and synergic ways.Reference Marinaci, Venuleo, Ferrante and Della Bona15

Semiotic cultural psychology theory

Semiotic cultural psychology theory argues for the role of the sense-making process in the way people view and deal with the social world.Reference Salvatore, Rosa and Valsiner14 According to this perspective, people give meaning to their life events in terms of symbolic resources (beliefs, knowledge, values) grounded on implicit, generalised world-views (symbolic universes, in the terms of the theory), made up by affect-laden basic latent dimensions of sense (LDSs).Reference Salvatore, Rosa and Valsiner14 The LDSs have a bipolar structure, for example Salvatore and colleaguesReference Salvatore, Fini, Mannarini, Veltri, Avdi and Battaglia16 mapped three LDSs: pleasant versus unpleasant, passivity versus engagement and demand for systemic resources versus demand for community bond.

LDSs are not a property of the individual, do not emerge in a social vacuum, but are the by-product of a dynamic process (sense-making), where individuals, embedded in a specific system (e.g. family, school, workplace) and culture recursively interact with each other. Broader contextual dimensions (e.g. policies in the health and economic field, media communication, scientists’ and politicians’ discourses) set boundaries on the attribution of meaning to the events, difficulties, challenges and conditions of their lives. Previous studies have shown the relationship between a negative way of perceiving the social environment – namely as an anomic place with no future and no one to rely on – and problematic behaviours such as harmful drinking, smoking, gambling, as well PIU itself.Reference Venuleo, Calogiuri and Rollo17,Reference Venuleo, Rollo, Marinaci and Calogiuri18

Semiotic cultural psychology theory highlights two aspects of LDSs related to their capacity to promote adaptive responses.

The first is their variable degree of salience.Reference Salvatore, Picione, Vincenzo, Mannino, Langher and Pergola19 Interpretations of reality characterised by a high salience correspond to a rigid, polarised way of thinking, so that objects and situations are homogenised (typically, organised by the friend/enemy, pleasure/displeasure opposition) without considering their particular nature. Such homogenising interpretations of reality are associated with a reduced capacity to regulate thoughts and behaviours on the basis of social constraints and requirements. Conversely, interpretations of reality characterised by a low salience are associated with a way of thinking able to differentiate objects of experience. Flexible thinking is also able to produce a plurality of meanings and allows personal and/or community resources to be used more effectively. Barrett and colleaguesReference Barrett, Gross, Christensen and Benvenuto20 expressed a similar concept, suggesting that individuals with poorly differentiated emotional experiences are less able to regulate emotions and respond adaptively to events. Previous studies support the hypothesis of a significant relationship between the degree of affective salience and adaptation, showing how drug addicts, problem gamblers and internet users reported global, homogenising and generalising embodied affect-laden interpretations of reality, at the cost of more fine-grained and differentiated analytical thought. The scientific literature recognises that problematic behaviour is often linked to emotional processes, suggesting that the lower the ability to modulate the valence and intensity of one's emotional experience, the higher the risk of problem behaviour,Reference Velotti, Rogier, Zobel and Billieux21 including PIU.Reference Gioia, Rega and Boursier22

The second is the degree to which the beliefs, feelings and actions underpinned by LDSs are consistent with interpersonal and social tasks, rules and goals. Similarly, the notion of ‘cultural consensus’Reference Dressler23 highlights that levels of psychosocial distress as well as the risk of harmful behaviours may be the result of low proximity with widely shared cultural models.

Based on semiotic cultural psychology theory and the view of PIU as a compensating mechanism, the present study aims to explore a theoretical model (Fig. 1) in which PIU is predicted by the LDSs grounding adolescents’ view of the world – through the mediation of high levels of psychosocial malaise. Our hypothesis is that homogenised affect-laden interpretations of the context and a low identification with social tasks, rules and goals may fuel negative affect, which in turn increases the risk of PIU. Indeed, within a negative and distrustful view of the relational and social context, internet use can be seen as the only way to cope with problems in life.

Fig. 1 The theoretical model: problematic internet use is predicted by latent dimensions of sense through the mediation of high levels of psychosocial malaise.

Psychological malaise is detected considering adolescents’ levels of loneliness, social anxiety and negative emotions: indeed, internet use can be a mood-regulation strategy when experiencing an unpleasant state and a way to compensate for the sense of isolation.Reference Venuleo, Ferrante and Rollo10

Method

Participants

Public high schools situated in southern Italy (the Salento area) were contacted by a letter describing the purpose of the study. Four schools agreed to participate, three situated in the city of Lecce, one in a smaller town of the province. In the schools participating, grade 9 classes, attended by adolescents from 13 to 15 years of age, and grade 11 classes, attended by adolescents from 16 to 19 years of age, were selected for the study so as to include the age group with the highest rates of PIU in Europe.Reference Tsitsika, Janikian, Schoenmakers, Tzavela, Olafsson and Wójcik24 In each school, four classes were randomly selected. Parental consent was required for students under 18: between 1 and 2% of the parents did not give consent.

In total, 764 students (mean age 15.05 years, s.d. = 1.152 years) were involved in the study. The sociodemographic characteristics of the sample, disaggregated by gender, are shown in Table 1. Significant differences related to gender were found for the school year attended.

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample

Instruments

Problematic internet use (PIU)

The Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale 2 (GPIUS-2)Reference Caplan3 was used for self-assessment of PIU; the scale consists of 15 items rated on an eight-point Likert scale (from ‘definitely disagree’ to ‘definitely agree’). Its Italian versionReference Fioravanti, Primi and Casale25 includes four factors: preference for online social interaction (POSI), mood regulation (MORE), deficient self-regulation (DSRE) and negative outcome (NOUT).

Latent dimensions of sense (LDSs)

The View of Context (VOC)Reference Ciavolino, Redd, Evrinomy, Falcone, Fini and Kadianaki26 questionnaire was used to map the LDSs through which people interpret their social context. Respondents are asked to report their opinions about the social environment, for instance by evaluating the place where they live, or the degree of reliability of services (e.g. health services and schools), as well as to state the moral/social values in which they believe, for instance studying or respecting each other. The questionnaire is composed of 45 items associated with a four-point Likert scale (‘not at all’, ‘not much’, ‘quite a lot’, ‘a lot’ or ‘very unreliable’, ‘rather unreliable’, ‘quite reliable’, ‘very reliable’) and exploring three main domains: ‘reliability of the context’, ‘agency’ and ‘success’.

Psychosocial malaise

We used three instruments to assess aspects of psychosocial malaise:

  1. (a) the Interaction Anxiousness Scale (IAS),Reference Leary27 with its 15 items rated on a five-point Likert scale (from ‘not at all characteristic of me’ to ‘extremely characteristic of me’), was used to measure the levels of anxiety in social interaction; the IAS validated in Italy considers a higher-order latent variable, with four lower-order latent variables;Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen28

  2. (b) the Negative Affect subscale from the Italian versionReference Ciavolino, Ferrante, Sternativo, Cheah, Rollo and Marinaci29 of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)Reference Terracciano, McCrae and Costa30 was employed to investigate the presence of negative affect; the respondent self-reports how much his or her experience is consistent with the adjective reported, ranging on a 5-point Likert scale (from ‘not at all’ to ‘very much’;

  3. (c) the General Loneliness subscale from Italian Loneliness Scale (ILS),Reference Zammuner31 a self-report questionnaire for subjective loneliness. ILS General Loneliness consists of seven items rated on a four-point Likert scale (from ‘I often feel this way’ to ‘I never feel this way’).

Procedure

Google Forms was used to administer, via computer, the set of instruments used for data collection. Two members of the research team introduced the study, highlighting that the voluntary nature of participation and the anonymity of responses were assured. Participants were also informed that the data would be analysed collectively and that only the research team would have access to them. Participants aged 18 and over provided their written informed consent to participate in the study. A parent or guardian of participants under the age of 18 provided written informed consent. Participants spent an average of 30 min responding. All procedures used in this research comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human participants/patients were approved by the Ethics Commission for Research in Psychology of the Department of History, Society and Human Studies of the University of Salento (25 March 2021; protocol no. 0056300).

Data analysis

Detection of the LDSs

Variability in responses on the VOC was considered in order to obtain LDSs.Reference Ciavolino, Redd, Evrinomy, Falcone, Fini and Kadianaki26 For instance, a statement such as ‘I can only count on myself’ may convey the symbolic meaning of ‘agency’ if it is associated with statements such as ‘life is determined by actions’. However, the same expression ‘I can only count on myself’ may convey the meaning of ‘anomie’ if it is associated with statements such as ‘to succeed in life, it is important to have few scruples’.

Multiple correspondence analysis (MCA)Reference Lebart, Hayashi, Yajima, Bock, Ohsumi, Tanaka and Baba32 can be used to map such variability. It consists of an explorative technique that enables LDSs to be defined by extracting a limited number of factorial dimensions to sum up the relations observed between nominal or ordinal data. To explore the possible patterns existing among the answers related to adolescents’ representations of their context, we carried out an MCA with the variables from the VOC questionnaire, namely ‘reliability of the context’, ‘agency’, and ‘success’. Each factorial dimension extracted describes the juxtaposition of two patterns of strongly associated response modes, corresponding to the opposition between two generalised meanings. This means that a factorial polarity collects the modalities of responses that are linked together as the effect of an LDS, independent of its specific content/semantic relationship. Therefore, a single answer (e.g. ‘the police are not reliable at all’) can occur in more than one dimension, but what makes it interpretable is the way such a statement is linked to the other answers – grouped on the same factor – in a pre-semantic, affect-laden way.

The respondent's position (coordinate) on the main factorial dimension is considered: a higher score indicates high proximity between the respondent's responses and the affect-laden interpretations underpinning that factorial dimension. This analysis was run on the software SPAD 8.2 for Windows (https://ia-data-analytics.com/data-mining-software/).

Mediation model for higher-order modelling with PLS-SEM

To explore the mediation effects, it is necessary to have strong a priori theoretical support, which can be formalised by a direct or indirect effect between two variables.Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan33 In the proposed mediation model (Fig. 2), LDSs predict PIU through the mediation of psychosocial malaise. Moreover, psychosocial malaise and PIU are defined with higher-order modelling, where the higher-order component represents a multidimensional concept that occurs at a certain level of abstraction, which is related to lower-order components representing their underlying and more concrete subdimension.Reference Becker, Klein and Wetzels34 The model is thus defined considering as the dependent variable ξIIIPIU, expressed as a second-order latent variable, measured through the first-order latent variables POSI, DSRE, NOUT and MORE.

Fig. 2 Mediation model for higher-order modelling.

POSI, preference for online social interaction; DSRE, deficient self-regulation; VOC, View of Context questionnaire; IPI, psychosocial malaise; PIU, problematic internet use; NOUT, negative outcome; IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; MORE, mood regulation; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; ILS, Italian Loneliness Scale.

The measurement of the higher-order models can be reflective–reflective or formative–reflective when the higher-order component represents a more general construct simultaneously explaining all the underlying lower-order components, and reflective–formative or formative–formative when the lower-order components form the general higher-order component without sharing a unique cause.Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan33Reference Cheah, Ting, Ramayah, Memon, Cham and Ciavolino35

In our model, two types were adopted:

  1. (a) Reflective–reflective, for the second-order latent variables named IAS and PIU. This choice finds a justification in the theoretical concepts behind social anxiousness (measured on the IAS)Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen28 and problematic use of the internet (PIU).Reference Fioravanti, Primi and Casale25 They are measures representing the effects of an underlying first-order latent variable. Therefore, since a reflective measure dictates that all indicators are caused by the same construct (psychosocial malaise and PIU), their indicators should be highly correlated with each other. Finally, individual items should be interchangeable, and any single item can be left out without changing the principal construct.Reference Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle and Gudergan33

  2. (b) Reflective–formative, for the third-order latent variable named psychosocial malaise. The adoption of the reflective–formative model is justified by the theoretical concept behind the higher-order component psychosocial malaise, which is formed by the three lower-order components ILS, PANAS and IAS. Each dimension can vary independently of the others, i.e. the specific lower-order components (PANAS, IAS and ILS) do not have a unique and shared cause, but they form the general higher-order component.

To estimate the mediation model, structural equation modelling based on partial least squares (PLS-SEM)Reference Hair, Hult, Ringle and Sarstedt36 with higher-order components was implemented using Smart PLS 3.3.2 for Mac OSX (www.smartpls.com) and R 4.1.0 for Mac OSX (www.r-project.org). The measurement model evaluation was performed via confirmatory composite analysis (CCA).Reference Schuberth, Henseler and Dijkstra37 In addition, the embedded two-stage approachReference CM, Sarstedt and Straub38 was used for specifying and estimating higher-order latent variables (psychosocial malaise and PIU) in PLS-SEM, which is appropriate for reflective–formative type higher-order components,Reference Becker, Klein and Wetzels34 in particular when a component, such as psychosocial malaise, plays a mediation role in the nomological network as defined in Fig. 2. The two stages of this approach are as follows.

The first stage coincides with the standard repeated indicators approach. This step is used for the assessment of the measurement model and to estimate the scores of the lower-order components used in the second stage as indicators of the higher-order components.

In the second stage the estimated scores in the previous step define the indicators (manifest variables) of the higher-order components. Specifically, the third-order psychosocial malaise component will be formatively measured using the estimated scores on the IAS, PANAS and ILS. The second-order component PIU is measured by the estimated scores for POSI, DSRE, NOUT and MORE. The parameter estimation in this step was performed by considering the consistent version of the PLS estimator (PLSc) in such a way as to reduce the bias of the formative measurement model. PLSc provides a correction for estimates of the path coefficients, construct correlations and indicator loadings.Reference Dijkstra and Henseler39

Assessment of the measurement model with CCA

To assess the higher-order measurement model, a two-stage process has to be followed.

In the first stage the lower-order reflective measurement model (IAS 1/2/3/4, ILS, PANAS, POSI, MORE, DSRE, NOUT) is assessed considering the internal consistency (composite reliability), convergent validity (average variance extracted, AVE), discriminant validity and indicator loading and their significance (bootstrap procedure).

In the second stage the higher-order measurement model (psychosocial malaise, IAS and PIU) is assessed as follows:

  1. (a) the third-order formative construct (psychosocial malaise) is assessed by considering as items the lower-order components scores, interpreting the relationships between this and lower-order components as weights, and assessing convergent validity (AVE), collinearity between indicators (variance inflation factor, VIF) and significance and relevance of the outer weights (bootstrap procedure);

  2. (b) the second-order IAS and PIU constructs are assessed through the internal consistency (composite reliability), convergent validity (AVE), discriminant validity and indicator loading and their significance (bootstrap procedure).

Assessment of structural model

PLS-SEM results are evaluated considering the coefficients of determination (R 2) and the significance of the path coefficients, together with the f2 effect sizes, predictive relevance (Q 2) and q2 effect sizes.

Assessment of PLS-SEM outcomes has been extended examining the mediating effects of psychosocial malaise on the relationship between the view of context (VOC1 and VOC2 extracted through MCA) and PIU.

Results

Detection of LDSs

After applying the formula of inertia adjustment,Reference Benzécri40 it was found that the first factorial dimension of the VOC (VOC1) accounts for 48.62% and the second (VOC2) for 16.41%, thus the two factors account for 65.03% of the total inertia.

VOC1: models of relationship with the social environment

This dimension (Table 2) contrasts two patterns of answers interpreted as two models of relationship with one's social environment: moderate reliability (−) versus absolute unreliability (+). On the moderate reliability polarity, moderate answers co-occur, i.e. items associated with intermediate points on a Likert scale (e.g. ‘somewhat agree’, ‘somewhat disagree’); such items concern the future, the possibility of acting to change the state of things, and the degree of trustworthiness of people and institutions. The absolute unreliability polarity collects items that adopt extreme scores on Likert scales (e.g. ‘strongly agree’, ‘not at all’) and have negative content (e.g. services and institutions are not available, it is not possible to change in the future, there is no faith in the recovery of the country). An anomic view emerges, where the only rule seems to be not having scruples and siding with the strongest.

Table 2 Response modes most significantly associated with the first factorial dimension (VOC1) of the View of Context questionnaire

VOC2: modes of evaluating the social environment

The opposite patterns of responses of the second factorial dimension (Table 3) were interpreted in terms of two ways of evaluating the social environment: idealising (−) versus moderate (+). Overall, the response modality is reflected in the tendency to generalise positive evaluations of objects and situations or to modulate judgements.

Table 3 Response modes most significantly associated with the second factorial dimension (VOC2) of the View of Context questionnaire

On the idealising polarity, responses associated with extreme points on the Likert scales are aggregated (e.g. ‘very’, ‘much better’), with a set of homogeneously positive evaluations which depict the context as a pleasant place: there is faith that the future will be better, people are considered able to change, importance is given to rules and knowledge. On the moderate polarity, responses associated with the intermediate point on the Likert scales are aggregated (e.g. ‘not very’, ‘somewhat’). The content of the items is both positive and negative: on the one hand, some elements of the macro social environment are perceived as tending to be untrustworthy; on the other hand, traces of faith in people's possibility to improve their condition are expressed.

Assessment of the measurement model with CCA

First stage

Table 4 reports the results of the lower-order reflective measurement model. All the standardised outer loadings of the latent variables are above the threshold values (0.40–0.708), with significant bootstrap intervals, which suggests a sufficient level of indicator reliability. Composite reliability for all the latent variables involved in this study exceeded the threshold of 0.70 and the AVE value above 0.50 suggests an adequate convergent validity for all the constructs except for IAS (AVE = 0.493), which can be very well approximated to 0.50, and PANAS (AVE = 0.396), the values of which in several studiesReference Joiner, Sandín, Chorot, Lostao and Marquina41,Reference Crocker42 have ranged from 0.17 to 0.43.

Table 4 Assessment of factor loadings, composite reliability (CR) and convergent validity (AVE)

AVE, average variance extracted; IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; ILS, Italian Loneliness Scale. GPIUS, Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale, version 2; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule.

Table 5 shows the results of the Fornell & LarckerReference Fornell and Larcker43 criterion assessment: the square roots of the reflective lower-order construct AVE (shown in bold) are all higher than the correlations with the other latent variables. This means that all the constructs are able to measure unique concepts.

Table 5 Assessment of discriminant validitya

DSRE, deficient self-regulation; IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; ILS, Italian Loneliness Scale; MORE, mood regulation; NOUT, negative outcome; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; POSI, preference for online social interaction.

a. Entries highlighted in bold show the square root of the convergent validity (average variance extracted, AVE). The other entries show the bivariate correlations between the constructs.

Second stage

The higher-order measurement model (IAS, PIU and psychosocial malaise) is assessed as follows.

For the second-order reflective IAS and PIU constructs, composite reliability values are respectively 0.791 and 0.923, exceeding the threshold of 0.70. The AVE values are respectively 0.504 and 0.749, above the threshold of 0.50, suggesting an adequate convergent validity for both second-order latent variables. In this case too, the bootstrap method was used with 1000 subsamples and we found that all relationships are significant (P < 0.05) for both IAS and PIU. Table 6 reveals that all weights of reflective indicators have significant t-values so that all the indicators can be maintained; additionally, confidence intervals and t-values for reflective indicators also support the significance of weights (i.e. 0 did not occur between the higher and lower values). The second-order discriminant validity of PIU and IAS is shown in Table 7.

Table 6 Second-order (IAS and PIU) estimated loadings and bootstrap results

IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; PIU, problematic internet use; DSRE, deficient self-regulation; MORE, mood regulation; NOUT, negative outcome; POSI, preference for online social interaction.

Table 7 Assessment of second-order discriminant validitya

a. Entries highlighted in bold show the square root of the convergent validity (AVE). The other entry shows the bivariate correlation between the constructs.

For the third-order formative construct (psychosocial malaise) assessment, considering the constructs IAS, PANAS and ILS as predictors of psychosocial malaise, the VIF values are less than the threshold of 5 (Table 8), therefore collinearity is not an issue between the constructs’ formative indicators.

Table 8 Variance inflation factors for the third-order formative constructs

IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; ILS, Italian Loneliness Scale; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule.

Through the bootstrapping procedure and using 1000 resamples, the indicators’ weights are well above the recommended value of 0.1. Table 9 reveals that all weights of formative indicators have significant t-values, which provides empirical support for retaining all the indicators; the confidence intervals as well as t-values for the formative indicators provided additional evidence regarding the significance of weights, as 0 did not occur between the higher and lower values of the confidence intervals.

Table 9 Third-order (psychosocial malaise) estimated loadings and bootstrap results

IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; ILS, Italian Loneliness Scale; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule.

Convergent validity cannot be performed without global or multiple items for redundancy analysis.

The ‘variance-explained’ (R 2) value of the endogenous variable psychosocial malaise is 0.133, exceeding the threshold of 0.10. It shows a good proportion of variance in the dependent variable explained by the independent variable.

The predictive validity (Q 2), assessed through the blindfolding method, was obtained through a cross-validated redundancy procedure. Table 10 shows that 0.230 was the Q 2 value for psychosocial malaise, which represents high relevance for the endogenous construct (i.e. psychosocial malaise).

Table 10 Third-order (psychosocial malaise) estimated loadings and blindfolding results

Bold denotes high relevance for the endogenous construct; SSE, sum of squares error; SSO, sum of squares of observations; IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; ILS, Italian Loneliness Scale; MORE, mood regulation; NOUT, negative outcome; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule.

Measurement model evaluation in mediation analysis

In the literature there are three types of mediation: complementary (or partial mediation), competitive (or partial mediation) and indirect-only (or full) mediation.Reference Becker, Klein and Wetzels34

The evaluation of mediation analysis was conducted via the bootstrap procedure.

Table 11 reports the bootstrap results for the direct and indirect mediation effects of VOC1. The direct effect (VOC1 → PIU) is not statistically significant (P = 0.316 and 95% CI −0.112 to 0.035), whereas the indirect mediation (VOC1 → psychosocial malaise → PIU) effect, with an estimated coefficient equal to 0.134, is statistically significant (P = 0.000 and 95% CI 0.101–0.171), showing a situation of indirect-only mediation.

Table 11 Bootstrap results for the direct and indirect mediation effects of VOC1

PIU, problematic internet use; VOC1, first factorial dimension of the View of Context questionnaire (i.e. absolute unreliability).

This finding suggests that the mediator (psychosocial malaise) fully complies with the expectation. Fig. 3 shows a graphical representation of the relationship between components of the model.

Fig. 3 Relationships between components of the model. The dashed line indicates a non-significant effect.

Thus, psychosocial malaise fully mediates the VOC1 → PIU relationship, with a statistically significant mediation coefficient of 0.134, so our findings empirically support the mediating role of psychosocial malaise in the relationship between absolute unreliability and PIU.

Table 12 shows the bootstrap results for the direct and indirect mediation effects of VOC2. The direct effect (VOC2 → PIU) is not statistically significant (P = 0.315 and 95% CI −0.137 to 0.040), and the indirect effect (VOC2 → psychosocial malaise → PIU), with an estimated coefficient equal to −0.027, is not statistically significant (P = 0.114 and 95% CI −0.059 to 0.009).

Table 12 Bootstrap results for the direct and indirect mediation effects of VOC2

PIU, problematic internet use; VOC2, second factorial dimension of the View of Context questionnaire.

Thus, by contrast, psychosocial malaise does not mediate the VOC2 → PIU relationship, with a statistically non-significant mediation coefficient of −0.027, indicating a lack of empirical support for the mediating role of psychosocial malaise in the model and the relationship between moderate evaluations of the social environment and PIU.

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the relationship between the way adolescents interpret their social context (i.e. their LDSs) and PIU, considering this relationship mediated by psychosocial malaise. Our hypothesis was that a view of the context underpinned by homogenising affect-laden interpretations, inconsistent with social tasks, rules and goals, may fuel negative affect, which in turn increases the need to indulge in internet use.

The results support this model, demonstrating the role of the latent dimensions of sense in predicting PIU through the mediation of high levels of psychosocial malaise.

As concerns the first hypothesis – adaptive responses are a function of the variable degree of affective salience of the generalised meanings comprising the LDSs – the findings showed that an LDS sense corresponding to a polarised way of viewing the context is associated with adolescents’ PIU through the mediation of psychosocial malaise in terms of high social anxiety, loneliness and negative emotions.

More specifically, the adolescents’ tendency to express negative and extreme evaluations of the social environment (the ‘absolute unreliable’ polarity of the LDS labelled ‘models of relationship with the social environment’) is associated with higher psychosocial malaise, which, in turn, is associated with higher levels of PIU. Accordingly, adolescents’ psychosocial malaise can be understood as reflecting the way of perceiving the sociocultural environment as completely lacking in people and institutions to count on. Within this scenario, there is no hope in the future and the only possibility to get by seems to be by breaking rules, living without scruples and forming alliances with the strongest. As responses on the Likert scale indicate, connotations and evaluations of their environment are rigid, polarised and extremely negative: the variability of objects or events of the experience is not recognised; instead, everything is viewed in a homogenising way. This means that evaluations are not based on an analytic representation of the object, but projected in a generalised category (i.e. the category of ‘good things’ versus the category of ‘bad things’), expressed also with the ‘affect-as-information’ concept.Reference Storbeck and Clore44 Accordingly, affect is embodied information about the value and importance of a particular object/stimulus, and such affective valence plays a role in how people judge and then behave. Consistent with our findings, previous studies highlighted how participants who had reactive, extreme and negative attitudes towards the micro and macro social environment were at higher risk of developing pathological gambling.Reference Venuleo, Calogiuri and Rollo17,Reference Venuleo, Rollo, Marinaci and Calogiuri18 According to our perspective, this reactive tendency of adolescents to express very negative evaluations, homogenising objects and situations, can be interpreted as a sign of an intense affective activation (the same thing happens to a very upset person who sees everything around as a stimulus for their anger). This intense affective activation compromises the regulation of one's thoughts, desires and beliefs and makes it more difficult to act consistently with social constraints and requirements.Reference Salvatore, Picione, Vincenzo, Mannino, Langher and Pergola19

Other studies, framed within the theory of mind,Reference Baron-Cohen45 highlight the role of mentalisation or reflective function in behavioural addictions: considering mentalisation as the capacity to recognise one's own and others’ feelings, thoughts, desires and intentions, adolescents with problematic gaming, gambling and social network useReference Ciccarelli, Cosenza, Nigro, Griffiths and D'Olimpio46,Reference Musetti, Starcevic, Boursier, Corsano, Billieux and Schimmenti47 seem to have deficits in their reflective function. Such deficits, namely assumptions with little or no correspondence to observable evidence, besides not taking into account the mind's complexity, are strictly associated with emotion regulation problemsReference Ciccarelli, Cosenza, Nigro, Griffiths and D'Olimpio46 and may result in difficulties in regulating one's behaviour. Similarly, other cognitive studies on the link between emotions, cognitions and behaviourReference Schreuder, Wichers, Hartman, Menne-Lothmann, Decoster and van Winkel48 highlighted that the more a person shows poorly differentiated emotion, the more they are unable to respond flexibly to events, ultimately showing maladaptation and malaise.

The role of VOC1 in promoting psychosocial malaise and, in turn, PIU is also consistent with the second hypothesis – adaptive responses are a function of the coherence between the latent dimensions of sense and the role demands made on the participants by the social environment. Indeed, exclusively negative terms aggregated by the ‘absolute unreliability’ polarity are used to describe the relationship with the social environment. The feeling of an adolescent positioned here and expressing higher social malaise and a higher level of PIU is that nobody makes an effort to improve the present and the future of the country, not the ordinary people, not the politicians, not other institutions, so that ‘you can only live day by day’. Conversely, the feeling of congruence between the individual's demands and the environment's responses (as in the ‘moderate reliability’ polarity) works as a protective factor for adolescents, preventing them from suffering and from using the internet as a maladaptive compensation strategy. The findings are consistent with previous studies conducted among young adults and adults in which gambling, drinking and PIUReference Dijkstra and Henseler39,Reference Benzécri40 were associated with a negative and anomic view of the social environment, suggesting that social disruption can encourage problematic, maladaptive behaviours as a sort of reaction to this situation. Other scholars have found problematic behaviours in association with antisociality and hostility.Reference Rogier, Velotti and Zavattini49

These findings are consistent with Dressler'sReference Dressler23 cultural consensus theory – arguing that negative health outcomes can be the result of the lack of approximation between an individual's behaviours and beliefs and the cultural models that surround them – as well as in sociological and anthropological studies.Reference Bjarnason50 These suggest that problematic behaviour among adolescents is not the result of an existential crisis, or the mere outcome of developmental characteristics which make them ‘naturally’ vulnerable. Rather, it should be attributed to the present-day condition characterised by rapid change, instability and an uncertain future. This leads to a state of anomie, which is characterised precisely by ‘the general idea that the absence of clear rules of behaviour and ambiguity in rules and goals create a state where the individual faces uncertain, conflicting expectations and ambiguous norms and values’.Reference Thorlindsson51 Growing feelings of uncertainty and ambivalence seem to be the main characteristics of our time, often described as ‘liquid modernity’:Reference Bauman52 social structures – such as education, health, social security and family – appear fluid, unable to hold their shape. The related lack of solid bonds with no clear line between public and private lives also influences media consumption among youths,Reference Ligocki and Trifonas53 in terms of constant consumption with no clear beginning or end.

From the perspective of semiotic cultural psychology theory, a condition of uncertainty leads sense-makers to adopt affect-laden, homogenised world-views as a way to re-establish stability,Reference Salvatore, Rosa and Valsiner14 as suggested by the extreme responses of our sample.

The findings also showed a lack of a direct or mediated relationship between the LDS labelled ‘modes of evaluating the social environment’ and PIU. The polarity covers moderate positions in the evaluation of social services (e.g. public offices, health services) and institutions (e.g. the education system, police) and an average proximity to moral values (e.g. the importance of learning and respecting rules). From this perspective, the findings support the idea that maladaptation may not emerge if the adolescent recognises the relevant specificity of the objects/events of the experience and is able to produce differentiated meanings. As for the idealisation polarity, despite the extremism of the response modality, in this case, the social environment is perceived as a comfortable and reliable place – that is, compatible with the individual's demands and needs – and therefore use of the internet as a way to cope with psychosocial malaise does not occur.

Limitations

Some methodological limitations of the present study have to be considered. First, a convenience sample was used so the results need to be seen in relation to the specific cultural context under analysis, that is southern Italy. In other countries, characterised by different social conditions, other LDSs may emerge. Second, owing to the cross-sectional nature of the study it is not possible to make causal inferences from our data. Third, the study did not take into account the role of potential variables such as perceived social support and sense of community, which may shed further light on how LDSs vary across social segments; the role of emotion dysregulation should be better taken into account because of the recognised part played by intense affective activation; the specific activity conducted online (e.g. social network use, online gaming) should be considered in future research too.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author contributions

L.F., C.V. and S.R. contributed to the study conception and design; L.F. and S.R contributed to data collection: G.A.S., J.-H.C. and E.C. contributed to data analysis; L.F. contributed to the manuscript preparation; C.V., E.C. and S.S. contributed to supervising all the phases of the study. All of the co-authors have contributed to and agree with the contents of the final manuscript.

Fundings

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Anderson, E, Steen, E, Stavropoulos, V. Internet use and problematic internet use: a systematic review of longitudinal research trends in adolescence and emergent adulthood. Int J Adolesc Youth 2016; 22: 430–54.10.1080/02673843.2016.1227716CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrante, L, Venuleo, C. Problematic internet use among adolescents and young adults: a systematic review of scholars’ conceptualizations after the publication of DSM5. Mediterr J Clin Psychol 2021; 9(2).Google Scholar
Caplan, SE. Theory and measurement of generalized problematic internet use: a two-step approach. Comput Human Behav 2010; 26: 1089–97.10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, K, Treder, MS, Chamberlain, SR, Kiraly, F, Redden, SA, Stein, DJ, et al. Problematic internet use as an age-related multifaceted problem: evidence from a two-site survey. Addict Behav 2018; 81: 157–66.10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.02.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez Fernandez, O, Kuss, D. Harmful Internet Use – Part I: Internet Addiction and Problematic Use. European Parliamentary Research Service, 2019 (http://www.europarl.europa.eu/RegData/etudes/STUD/2019/624249/EPRS_STU(2019)624249_EN.pdf).Google Scholar
Chambers, RA, Taylor, JR, Potenza, MN. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 1041–52.10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fumero, A, Marrero, RJ, Voltes, D, Penate, W. Personal and social factors involved in internet addiction among adolescents: a meta-analysis. Comput Human Behav 2018; 86: 387400.10.1016/j.chb.2018.05.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, MH, Kraus, LA. Social contact, loneliness, and mass media use: a test of two hypotheses. J Appl Soc Psychol 1989; 19: 1100–24.10.1111/j.1559-1816.1989.tb01242.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kardefelt-Winther, D. Conceptualizing Internet use disorders: addiction or coping process? Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2017; 71: 459–66.10.1111/pcn.12413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venuleo, C, Ferrante, L, Rollo, S. Facing life problems through the internet: the link between psychosocial malaise and problematic internet use in an adolescent sample. J Gambl Issu 2020; 4: 107–31.Google Scholar
Obeid, S, Saade, S, Haddad, C, Sacre, H, Khansa, W, Al Hajj, R, et al. Internet addiction among Lebanese adolescents: the role of self-esteem, anger, depression, anxiety, social anxiety and fear, impulsivity, and aggression – a cross-sectional study. J Nerv Ment Dis 2019; 207: 838–46.10.1097/NMD.0000000000001034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sela, Y, Zach, M, Amichay-Hamburger, Y, Mishali, M, Omer, H. Family environment and problematic internet use among adolescents: the mediating roles of depression and fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav 2020; 106: 106226.10.1016/j.chb.2019.106226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martins, MV, Formiga, A, Santos, C, Sousa, D, Resende, C, Campos, R, et al. Adolescent internet addiction – role of parental control and adolescent behaviours. Int J Pediatr Adolesc Med 2020; 7: 116–20.10.1016/j.ijpam.2019.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatore, S. Cultural psychology as the science of sensemaking: a semiotic-cultural framework for psychology. In The Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (eds Rosa, A, Valsiner, J): 3548. Cambridge University Press, 2018.10.1017/9781316662229.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marinaci, T, Venuleo, C, Ferrante, L, Della Bona, S. What game we are playing: the psychosocial context of problem gambling, problem gaming and poor well-being among Italian high school students. Heliyon 2021; 7(8): e07872.10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e07872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatore, S, Fini, V, Mannarini, T, Veltri, GA, Avdi, E, Battaglia, F, et al. Symbolic universes between present and future of Europe: first results of the map of European societies’ cultural milieu. PLoS One 2018; 13(1): e0189885.10.1371/journal.pone.0189885CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venuleo, C, Calogiuri, S, Rollo, S. Unplanned reaction or something else? The role of subjective cultures in hazardous and harmful drinking. Soc Sci Med 2015; 139: 917.10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.06.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venuleo, C, Rollo, S, Marinaci, T, Calogiuri, S. Towards a cultural understanding of addictive behaviours. the image of the social environment among problem gamblers, drinkers, internet users and smokers. Addict Res Theory 2016; 24: 274–87.10.3109/16066359.2015.1126257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salvatore, S, Picione, R, Vincenzo, B, Mannino, G, Langher, V, Pergola, F, et al. The affectivization of the public sphere: the contribution of psychoanalysis in understanding and counteracting the current crisis scenarios. Subj Action Soc 2021; 1: 330.Google Scholar
Barrett, LF, Gross, J, Christensen, TC, Benvenuto, M. Knowing what you're feeling and knowing what to do about it: mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cogn Emot 2001; 15: 713–24.10.1080/02699930143000239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velotti, P, Rogier, G, Zobel, SB, Billieux, J. Association between gambling disorder and emotion (dys)regulation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2021; 87: 102037.10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, F, Rega, V, Boursier, V. Problematic internet use and emotional dysregulation among young people: a literature review. Clin Neuropsychiatry 2021; 18: 4154.Google ScholarPubMed
Dressler, WW. Culture and the Individual: Theory and Method of Cultural Consonance. Routledge, 2017.10.4324/9781315164007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsitsika, A, Janikian, M, Schoenmakers, TM, Tzavela, EC, Olafsson, K, Wójcik, S, et al. Internet addictive behavior in adolescence: a cross-sectional study in seven European countries. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2014; 17: 528–35.10.1089/cyber.2013.0382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fioravanti, G, Primi, C, Casale, S. Psychometric evaluation of the Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale 2 in an Italian sample. CyberpsycholBehav Soc Netw 2013; 16: 761–6.10.1089/cyber.2012.0429CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciavolino, E, Redd, R, Evrinomy, A, Falcone, M, Fini, V, Kadianaki, I, et al. Views of context. an instrument for the analysis of the cultural milieu. a first validation study. Electron J Appl Stat Anal 2017; 10: 599628.Google Scholar
Leary, MR. Social anxiousness: the construct and its measurement. J Pers Assess 1983; 47: 6675.10.1207/s15327752jpa4701_8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D, Clark, LA, Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 1063–70.10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciavolino, E, Ferrante, L, Sternativo, GA, Cheah, JH, Rollo, S, Marinaci, T, et al. A confirmatory composite analysis for the Italian validation of the interactions anxiousness scale: a higher-order version. Behaviormetrika 2022; 49: 2346.10.1007/s41237-021-00151-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terracciano, A, McCrae, RR, Costa, PT Jr. Factorial and construct validity of the Italian Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Eur J Psychol Assess 2003; 19: 131–41.10.1027//1015-5759.19.2.131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zammuner, VL. Italians’ social and emotional loneliness: the results of five studies. Int J Soc Sci 2008; 3(2): 108–20.Google Scholar
Lebart, L. Correspondence analysis, discrimination, and neural networks. In Data Science, Classification, and Related Methods (Studies in Classification, Data Analysis, and Knowledge Organization) (eds Hayashi, C, Yajima, K, Bock, HH, Ohsumi, N, Tanaka, Y, Baba, Y): 423–30. Springer Japan, 1998.Google Scholar
Hair, JF Jr, Sarstedt, M, Ringle, CM, Gudergan, SP et al. Advanced Issues in Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling. Sage Publications, 2017.Google Scholar
Becker, J-M, Klein, K, Wetzels, M. Hierarchical latent variable models in PLS-SEM: guidelines for using reflective-formative type models. Long Range Plann 2012; 45: 359–94.10.1016/j.lrp.2012.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheah, J-H, Ting, H, Ramayah, T, Memon, MA, Cham, T-H, Ciavolino, E. A comparison of five reflective–formative estimation approaches: reconsideration and recommendations for tourism research. Qual Quant 2019; 53: 1421–58.10.1007/s11135-018-0821-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hair, JF Jr, Hult, GTM, Ringle, C, Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). Sage Publications, 2016.Google Scholar
Schuberth, F, Henseler, J, Dijkstra, TK. Confirmatory composite analysis. Front Psychol 2018; 9: 2541.10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
CM, Ringle, Sarstedt, M, Straub, DW. Editor's comments: a critical look at the use of PLS-SEM in “MIS quarterly.”. MIS Q 2012; 36(1): iii–xiv.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, TK, Henseler, J. Consistent and asymptotically normal PLS estimators for linear structural equations. Comput Stat Data Anal 2015; 81: 1023.10.1016/j.csda.2014.07.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benzécri, JP. Sur le calcul des taux d'inertie dans l'analyse d'un questionnaire (On the calculation of inertia rates in the analysis of a questionnaire). Cah Anal des Données 1979; 4: 377–8.Google Scholar
Joiner, TE Jr, Sandín, B, Chorot, P, Lostao, L, Marquina, G. Development and factor analytic validation of the SPANAS among women in Spain: (more) cross-cultural convergence in the structure of mood. J Pers Assess 1997; 68: 600–15.10.1207/s15327752jpa6803_8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crocker, PRE. A confirmatory factor analysis of the Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) with a youth sport sample. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1997; 19: 91–7.10.1123/jsep.19.1.91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fornell, C, Larcker, DF. Structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error: algebra and statistics. J Mark Res 1981; 18: 382–8.10.1177/002224378101800313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storbeck, J, Clore, GL. Affective arousal as information: how affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 2008; 2: 1824–43.10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00138.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. Theory of mind in normal development and autism. Prisme 2001; 34: 74183.Google Scholar
Ciccarelli, M, Cosenza, M, Nigro, G, Griffiths, M, D'Olimpio, F. Gaming and gambling in adolescence: the role of personality, reflective functioning, time perspective and dissociation. Int Gamb Stud 2022; 22: 161–79.10.1080/14459795.2021.1985583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musetti, A, Starcevic, V, Boursier, V, Corsano, P, Billieux, J, Schimmenti, A. Childhood emotional abuse and problematic social networking sites use in a sample of Italian adolescents: the mediating role of deficiencies in self-other differentiation and uncertain reflective functioning. J Clin Psychol 2021; 77: 1666–84.10.1002/jclp.23138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreuder, MJ, Wichers, M, Hartman, CA, Menne-Lothmann, C, Decoster, J, van Winkel, R, et al. Lower emotional complexity as a prospective predictor of psychopathology in adolescents from the general population. Emotion (Epub ahead of print) 13 Jul 2020. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogier, G, Velotti, P, Zavattini, GC. Gioco d'azzardo ed antisocialità: riflessioni su un'emergenza sociale (Gambling and antisociality: reflections on a social emergency). Giornale Italiano di Psicologia 2017; 44: 219–48.Google Scholar
Bjarnason, T. Anomie among European adolescents: conceptual and empirical clarification of a multilevel sociological concept. Sociol Forum 2009; 24: 135–61.10.1111/j.1573-7861.2008.01089.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorlindsson, T. Durkheim's theory of social order and deviance: a multi-level test. EurSociol Rev 2004; 20: 271–85.Google Scholar
Bauman, Z. Liquid Modernity. John Wiley & Sons, 2013.Google Scholar
Ligocki, D. Youth consumption of media and the need for critical media literacy in the time of liquid modernity. In Handbook of Theory and Research in Cultural Studies and Education (ed Trifonas, P): 501–10. Springer, 2020.10.1007/978-3-319-56988-8_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 The theoretical model: problematic internet use is predicted by latent dimensions of sense through the mediation of high levels of psychosocial malaise.

Figure 1

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Mediation model for higher-order modelling.POSI, preference for online social interaction; DSRE, deficient self-regulation; VOC, View of Context questionnaire; IPI, psychosocial malaise; PIU, problematic internet use; NOUT, negative outcome; IAS, Interaction Anxiousness Scale; MORE, mood regulation; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; ILS, Italian Loneliness Scale.

Figure 3

Table 2 Response modes most significantly associated with the first factorial dimension (VOC1) of the View of Context questionnaire

Figure 4

Table 3 Response modes most significantly associated with the second factorial dimension (VOC2) of the View of Context questionnaire

Figure 5

Table 4 Assessment of factor loadings, composite reliability (CR) and convergent validity (AVE)

Figure 6

Table 5 Assessment of discriminant validitya

Figure 7

Table 6 Second-order (IAS and PIU) estimated loadings and bootstrap results

Figure 8

Table 7 Assessment of second-order discriminant validitya

Figure 9

Table 8 Variance inflation factors for the third-order formative constructs

Figure 10

Table 9 Third-order (psychosocial malaise) estimated loadings and bootstrap results

Figure 11

Table 10 Third-order (psychosocial malaise) estimated loadings and blindfolding results

Figure 12

Table 11 Bootstrap results for the direct and indirect mediation effects of VOC1

Figure 13

Fig. 3 Relationships between components of the model. The dashed line indicates a non-significant effect.

Figure 14

Table 12 Bootstrap results for the direct and indirect mediation effects of VOC2

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.