Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T02:55:40.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Auditory verbal hallucinations in first-episode psychosis: a phenomenological investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rachel Upthegrove*
Affiliation:
School of Clinical & Experimental Medicine, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK Birmingham and Solihull Mental Health Foundation Trust, Birmingham, UK
Jonathan Ives
Affiliation:
Society and History, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
Matthew R. Broome
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Warneford Hospital, Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing, Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
Kimberly Caldwell
Affiliation:
School of Clinical & Experimental Medicine, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK Birmingham and Solihull Mental Health Foundation Trust, Birmingham, UK
Stephen J. Wood
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne & Melbourne Health, Carlton, Victoria, Australia
Femi Oyebode
Affiliation:
School of Clinical & Experimental Medicine, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK Birmingham and Solihull Mental Health Foundation Trust, Birmingham, UK
*
Rachel Upthegrove, MBBS MRCPsych PhD, Clinical Senior Lecturer, School of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Birmingham, 25 Vincent Drive, Birmingham B15 2F, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

In dimensional understanding of psychosis, auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH) are unitary phenomena present on a continuum from non-clinical voice hearing to severe mental illness. There is mixed evidence for this approach and a relative absence of research into subjective experience of AVH in early psychosis.

Aims

To conduct primary research into the nature of subjective experience of AVH in first-episode psychosis.

Method

A phenomenological study using diary and photo-elicitation qualitative techniques investigating the subjective experience of AVH in 25 young people with first-episode psychosis.

Results

AVH are characterised by: (a) entity, as though from a living being with complex social interchange; and (b) control, exerting authority with ability to influence. AVH are also received with passivity, often accompanied by sensation in other modalities.

Conclusions

A modern detailed phenomenological investigation, without presupposition, gives results that echo known descriptive psychopathology. However, novel findings also emerge that may be features of AVH in psychosis not currently captured with standardised measures.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Non-Commercial, No Derivatives (CC BY-NC-ND) licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2016

Hallucinations are considered a fundamental symptom within psychiatry. Reference Waters, Allen, Aleman, Fernyhough, Woodward and Badcock1 Auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH) in psychosis were richly explored by Jaspers, Bleuler and Kraepelinin in the early 20th century Reference Oyebode2 and from this legacy focus has continued on AVH as a sensory perception in the auditory modality, principally in the form of speech, in the absence of external stimuli. Reference Oyebode2,Reference Anthony3 However, it is increasingly recognised that AVH can occur in otherwise healthy individuals with rates of up to 20% of the population Reference Bak, Myin-Germeys, Hanssen, Bijl, Vollebergh and Delespaul4 and can also occur in a range of non-psychotic illnesses, including post-traumatic stress disorder and borderline personality disorder. Reference Larøi, Sommer, Blom, Fernyhough, Ffytche and Hugdahl5 To what extent AVH in non-clinical and non-psychotic populations is the same phenomena as AVH as a symptom of psychosis is unclear. One recent qualitative study of AVH in a mixed clinical and non-clinical sample, reporting experiences online, showed no clear distinctions between these two groups. Reference Woods, Jones, Alderson-Day, Callard and Fernyhough6 Slotema et al similarly found no clear distinctions of AVH described by patients with schizophrenia as opposed to patients with borderline personality disorder. Reference Slotema, Daalman, Blom, Diederen, Hoek and Sommer7 In neurobiological investigations, there is more evidence of a distinction; Looijestijn et al Reference Looijestijn, Diederen, Goekoop, Sommer, Daalman and Kahn8 investigated sound localisation and demonstrated that only in psychosis were external AVH related to increased activity in the left planum temporale and right middle frontal gyrus cluster. Howes et al, Reference Howes, Shotbolt, Bloomfield, Daalman, Demjaha and Diederen9 in a positron emission tomography study, showed altered dopamine synthesis capacity only underlies hallucinations occurring in patients with psychosis. One difficulty may be that current studies often use standardised measures of AVH or questionnaires with yes/no responses, and these do not capture the rich features that may distinguish subtypes of AVH or unique features in psychosis. Stanghellini et al emphasise this possibility, having investigated the distinction between psychotic and non-psychotic AVH over and above that gained by standard AVH measures. Participants were encouraged to provide further detailed descriptions, and those provided by patients with schizophrenia were very different to that provided by the non-psychotic group. Reference Stanghellini, Langer, Ambrosini and Cangas10 AVH, as currently defined, may also be a poor way to distinguish a prodrome of a psychotic disorder. The occurrence of attenuated or infrequent AVH has been used, in part, to define the risk of developing a psychotic illness. ‘Transition’ to psychosis is made at a point predefined by standard criteria heavily influenced by the frequency of AVH, yet the clinical significance of this transition and role of AVH therein is challenged. Reference O'Connor, Nelson, Lin, Wood, Yung and Thompson11

Thus, as currently defined, AVH may show poor specificity for psychosis, and we suggest that an increase in primary phenomenological data is needed. The term ‘phenomenology’ has been used to label the detailed description of clinical features, signs and symptoms observed in pathological conditions, as well as being a method of analysing subjective experience that is rooted in a particular philosophical tradition. A phenomenological approach in this latter sense begins with the premise that the analysis of direct description can provide significant understanding and insight into the nature of the experience itself. Here, phenomenology is defined by a commitment to lay bare the ‘essence’ of any experience, independent of its causation. Reference Broome, Harland, Owen and Stringaris12 In the context of a research methodology, phenomenology is characterised by a systematic investigation of subjectivity and a consideration of experience from the first-person perspective and can be viewed as the foundational science for psychopathology. Reference Stanghellini and Broome13 The need to get at a true subjective experience may be particularly challenging in the experience of voice hearing in severe mental illness, whereby other positive symptoms such as persecutory delusions and cognitive and negative symptoms may form barriers to the communication of detailed accounts. Various methods have been used in the past to gain improved access to rich subjective data in psychosis in studies that focus on help-seeking, recovery and the meaning of experience. Reference Lataster, Valmaggia, Lardinois, van Os and Myin-Germeys14Reference Goldsmith16 When using a phenomenological approach to understand actual symptoms, we have previously used photo-elicitation ethnography in post-psychotic depression, finding this methodology helpful in increasing participants’ ability to communicate their experiences. Reference Sandhu, Ives, Birchwood and Upthegrove17

In this study, we aim for a modern description of AVH in psychosis based on a ‘naive’ exploration of the ‘concrete life worlds’ of those who experience this symptom. We aim to understand AVH phenomenology using an approach that does not attempt to relate an individual's experience to known or existing diagnostic or explanatory factors, and thus acknowledge the possibility of an entirely new framework emerging.

Method

A qualitative cross-sectional study using phenomenological and ethnographic methods was conducted with participants experiencing AVH as part of first-episode psychosis. We selected early psychosis as this would allow access to participants with clinically significant AVH at a time prior to repeated interactions with healthcare staff, whose interview questions and communication may frame an individual's understanding of their own experience.

Participants were recruited from the Birmingham Early Intervention Service, which covers areas of a diverse range of socioeconomic and ethnic communities. Reference Upthegrove, Birchwood, Ross, Brunett, McCollum and Jones18

Participants were included if they had the capacity to consent and were currently experiencing AVH at least every other day; this was to ensure that voice hearing was present currently and participants were not relying on memory of past experiences. The only exclusion criterion was an inability to communicate adequately in English. All patients meeting inclusion criteria, within 1 year of treatment onset, were invited to participate regardless of diagnostic category within psychosis. A pro forma was used to gather baseline demographic data of age, gender, occupational status and ethnicity. Diagnosis was as defined by consultant psychiatrist after clinical interview using ICD-10 RCD criteria. 19 National Health Service ethical approval was obtained and included the anonymous publication of direct quotes (NRES 13/WM/0277).

Phenomenological interviews

An in-depth interview was conducted with each participant. We used three approaches aimed at facilitating communication of individual experience; diary, photo-elicitation ethnography and an open walking interview. Diary entries and photos were used to stimulate discussion during interviews, where questions were flexible and unstructured, based on topics the participant wishes to discuss.

Diary and photo elicitation

One week prior to the interview, each participant was provided with a diary and asked to write down their experiences of AVH (i.e. what the event is like and how it makes them feel). We gave participants broad instructions about what to record.

Concurrently with the diaries, participants received a disposable camera and were asked to take photos of anything they believe represents aspects of their experiences of AVH. The photo-elicitation approach has proven to be a valuable method of obtaining a deeper understanding of people, their outlook on the world and, thus, their own lives. Reference Erdner and Magnusson20 Photos and their subsequent significance reveal meanings which are generally not accessed in a typical interview situation. Reference Harper21 Photo elicitation allows for higher levels of reflective thinking, as well as adding credibility to the data. Reference Sandhu, Ives, Birchwood and Upthegrove17,Reference Harrington and Lindy22

Diaries and cameras were collected 7 days after being given to participants, and 2–3 days before the qualitative interview. Films were developed and photos printed, so that participants and interviewer could handle the photos during the walking interview. We discussed thoroughly the use of a visual medium (photos) to aid conveyance of the experience of auditory phenomena; we concluded that the ‘heard’ nature of AVH was a presupposition and that the use of photos may allow a different understanding to emerge.

Walking interview

Rather than adopting a room-based style of interview, which may replicate negative power dynamics within healthcare or lead to a closed down ‘question and answer’ interaction, participants were asked to describe their experiences in an open environment. The mobile interview is considered a useful method of enabling participants to express their thoughts more clearly, since discussion takes place side to side (i.e. and not face to face), putting participants at ease. Reference Sandhu, Ives, Birchwood and Upthegrove17 In a project that seeks to open up the possibility for new understandings, it is important to ensure that participants are not in a frame of mind where they simply replicate what they have been told about their condition in clinical encounters. Having an interview experience that was as unlike a clinical interview as much as possible was therefore seen as advantageous.

Opening questions included: ‘Could you tell me a bit about this particular (diary) entry’ and ‘Could you explain how this particular photograph relates to your experience?’ Thus, interviews were based on each individual's communication of the nature of their own experiences and bespoke to each participant.

All dialogue was digitally recorded and transcribed verbatim. Recruitment continued until data saturation, that is, no codes or themes emerging (refer to Analysis and Results sections).

Analysis

Data were analysed using conventional content analysis. Reference Krippendorff23 The stepwise analytic process involved: (a) immersion in the data and making sense of it as a whole. Transcripts were read several times before units of analysis (codes) were selected; and (b) data organisation through a process of coding, followed by categorisation to describe the phenomena were generated, and grouped into sub/higher-order categories. Reference Hsieh and Shannon24 Care was taken to ensure that the findings were both credible and trustworthy, with various ‘triangulation’ checks performed on the analysis as it emerged. Initial analysis was conducted by K.C., who had regular meetings with R.U. and J.I. to discuss and develop the emerging codes and categories. Further, less regular meetings were held between K.C., R.U., J.I., S.J.W., M.R.B. and F.O. to discuss the emerging analysis and consider alternative perspectives.

Results

Twenty-five participants with first-episode psychosis met inclusion criteria and consented to participate in the study. At this point of recruitment, data saturation was reached. See Table 1 for demographic and clinical description of the sample. In this paper, we report findings related to the quality of AVH experience. Two overarching themes captured participants’ descriptions of the quality of their voices: entity and control. Within each theme are codes and subcodes.

Table 1 Clinical and demographic details

Characteristic
Age, years (range) 26 (17–37)
Male, % 69
Ethnicity, n (%)
   White 14 (56)
   Asian 7 (28)
   Black 4 (16)
Occupational status, n (%)
   Employed 1 (4)
   Unemployed 18 (72)
   Away from work 1 (4)
   Student 3 (12)
   Other 2 (4)
Diagnosis, n (%)
   Schizophrenia 11 (44)
   Schizoaffective 2 (8)
   Psychosis not otherwise specified 12 (48)
Duration of untreated psychosis, days
   Mean 279
   Median 209

Entity

Codes within this theme reflect AVH experienced as though the voice belongs to an entity. We use the term entity here to encompass all those features that speak to the reality and veridical nature of their experience, rather than participants reporting a visitation or other additional experience of entity. However, as demonstrated by the number of codes within this theme, a label of ‘reality’ was felt to be insufficient on its own. AVH were experienced as speaking with all the features of character, interaction and individuality of a living being (Fig. 1 and Table 2).

Fig. 1 Themes and codes.

Table 2 Theme 1: entity

Code Number endorsing code Description Quote
Locus 17 Varied experience that changed with time and situation
Location of voices did not appear to affect auditory quality or ‘realness’ of the experience
Identifying where voices were coming from could be confusing
‘When they're shouting it sounds like they're coming from in my head’
‘I hear it as an exterior voice, it's not as if I have a thought…’
‘It just seems like they're inside your head, I dunno’
Clarity and volume 9 Audible quality of voices described as being both clear and loud ‘It sounds like, imagine two men just standing behind you having a conversation in a pub, that's how loud they are’
‘When it does come loud all of a sudden you'll stand up and think… what was that?’
‘When it's loud it's a booming voice… it's like I struggle not to hear it, it's so loud’
Multiple 11 Male and female, adults and children, taking it in turns to speak or talking simultaneously. Multiple voices described in terms of their personal characteristics ‘Multiple voices, five six voices at the same time… Right after each other like a runaway train’
‘Some were good, some were greedy, some were bad’
‘Voices through the TV said Lorraine's a complete nut job, he goes stay away from her Tommy’
Veridicality
   Real



   Familiar
10



12
Realness’ voices, difficult to distinguish between reality
Surprise in realising that others could not hear voice
‘It does feel so real, it's just like someone's talking to me right next to me sometimes’
‘I only found out that the voices weren't real when I asked people I knew, I said can you hear these and they said no we can't’
Perceived familiarity of voices
Voices were recognised and tended to be heard as though part of a conversation
Unique and personal manner of voices
‘At first, it was my Uncle's that I could hear… “You've got to chill out and that lad”, man…’
‘They're using voices that you recognise from back in the day’
‘It's like, say like a mother duck, you've got a duckling, the duckling knows that call innit, so if it's family and that, speaking and that’

Clarity and volume

Overall, the audible quality of voices was described as being both clear and loud, but volume could vary with voice intent and content.

Veridicality

Real.   Participants frequently spoke about the absolute concrete presence of their voices, typically comparing this to the likeness of having or overhearing an actual conversation. As a result, many found it difficult to distinguish between reality and what they were currently experiencing. Participants talked about surprise in realising that others could not hear their voices.

Familiarity.   There was a perceived familiarity of voices reported. Voices spoke using voices from participants’ significant family members or from other individuals within their personal history in an intimate and personal communication.

Multiple

It was common (11 out of 25 participants) to hear multiple voices; these could be a combination of male and female, adults and children, taking turns to speak or talking simultaneously. Several participants reported that their voices could speak different languages, with one individual referring to these as his ‘community’. Those who frequently experienced multiple voices often described these in terms of the voices’ personal characteristics, good or bad, nasty, etc.

Locus

Voices are experienced as either internal (i.e. coming from inside participants’ heads) or external (i.e. coming from elsewhere) occurrences. Although the location of voices was clearly a factor raised by participants, this was a varied experience that changed with time and situation. The location of voices did not appear to affect their auditory quality or the ‘realness’ of the experience. Participants reported identifying where voices were coming from could be difficult to establish.

Control

Many codes were related to a passive experience of AVH; this was experienced as more than a commanding voice, although this did occur. Control not only reflects voice authority but also the ability to exert physical and emotional influence and be disruptive to day-to-day life. It also reflects the participants’ perception of voice impositions and being a passive recipient of an imposed experience, often occurring in more than one sensory modality.

Authority

In general, voices were experienced as being authoritative and powerful, demonstrating an ability to succeed in attempts to control the experience. Participants frequently referred to their voices as ‘strong’ and, in contrast, described themselves as being too ‘weak’ to stop them. Some reported that their voices took complete charge over thoughts and lives. One participant spoke about her struggle to control her voice, describing how she often felt overpowered by its ‘pull’. The power of voices was also associated with their unpredictability.

Ability to control behaviour

Participants often reported feeling their behaviour was influenced by their voices. This was experienced in many forms.

Demanding.   Voices were frequently described as being demanding, giving specific instructions to be followed. In total, 13 of 25 (52%) participants reported a demanding voice. This included commands which would often be given in a manner that prevented opportunity for further thought or discussion from the participants. We have chosen the label ‘demand’ rather than ‘command’ as although the content is an instruction, the term command infers a power with the order being given with the expectation it will be followed. This was not always present in participant's direct reports. A demanding voice implies a struggle between the request given, albeit firmly asked, and the authority with which it is given. A smaller number, 9 of 25 (32%) participants, reported a demanding voice with authority.

Commenting.   Several participants reported experiencing a commenting voice, which would often make statements about their daily activities.

Threatening: violent, derogatory and angry.   Voices asserted control by physically threatening participants. The content of threats ranged from somewhat ambiguous to specific threats with graphic violent content. Threatening comments would typically be emphasised by an aggressive tone of voice.

Ability to control emotions

Participants reported that their voices could influence their emotions to the extent that the voice was in control of what was experienced. This was conducted through a variety of complex exchanges and interactions.

Manipulative/tricking.   Voices were also described in terms of having emotional control over participants. This includes the manipulative nature of voices, in particular their ability to ‘use’ the individual's own information to trick participants into following their authority. Participants typically felt as though their voices were trying to mislead them.

Blaming and mocking.   Several participants reporting hearing an accusatory voice, which would criticise choices or decisions they had made. Participants often believed that they were being punished by their voices. The general tone of statements made by voices was mocking and, in some instances, proceeded by sarcastic laughter. Participants also described how their voices would ridicule them, calling them names, such as ‘stupid’ or ‘idiot’.

Helpful/guiding.   Occasionally voices were found to be helpful, offering participants guidance and support. The advice that was given was broad in content, yet also could be as specific as ‘telling me to avoid certain roads or… maybe get out a bit earlier or do certain things for my own good’. Those who experienced a helpful voice often spoke about this as being ‘protective’, able to assert itself and to defend participants in times of need.

Disruption

Participants felt that their voices had disrupted and, in numerous cases, completely halted their lives. The majority spoke about how they were unable to concentrate, which, for some, resulted in loss of roles, relationships and employment.

Interfering.   Day-to-day life became a constant battle, in which participants would be continually hindered by their voices. Participants described how even the simplest of tasks could suddenly become extremely arduous. Several participants reported that their voices were so interfering, to the extent that their lives had been destroyed.

Constant.   A common theme throughout interviews was the constant occurrence of voices, with many participants reporting that this was an ongoing experience. Some described their voices as continually being in the background, whereas other voices were more prominent and harassing. Participants tended to report that voices were typically unrelenting.

Compelling.   Participants often found their voices difficult to ignore. Several described feeling urged to follow their voices, not necessarily through their own choice, but because they found them so compelling. In some cases, participants’ attention would be diverted solely to their voices, making them the absolute focus of awareness.

Imposed

Experienced as being ‘done to’.   Participants frequently described themselves as being completely passive in the experience of their voices and, consequently, helpless in situations where their voices would occur. Described as more than a lack of control over voices, there was a reported perception of passivity in the experience; one participant reporting ‘my ears are made to hear it’.

Physical imposition and additional sensations.   Participants also reported voice experience as a physically imposed experience. Voices were accompanied by pain, itching or physical force. Overall, there was a real sense that voices forced themselves on participants that in totality resulted in more than an auditory experience.

See the data supplement for illustrations linked to codes.

Discussion

This extensive qualitative study of AVH in first-episode psychosis from participant's primary descriptions reports on the complex and intricate nature of the quality of experiences. We have grouped these into two overarching themes of entity and control. Our results provide not only confirmation but also additional information of the phenomenology of AVH experienced in psychosis.

Participants reported AVH along dimensions of clarity and volume, that voices had an experience of utter ‘realness’, were experienced often as many voices and spoke in complex sentences. From first-hand report, hallucinations thus resemble true perceptions and have all the vividness of real experience. They were accompanied by a subjective sense of a lack of control, authority and were disruptive by their interfering, constant and compelling nature. Our results thus confirm previous literature dating back to early psychopathology. Reflecting Jaspers, we have shown a pattern of complex and veridical AVH in early psychosis, from subjective descriptions and before repeated questioning and framing of experience within healthcare settings. Our results suggest complexity is evident from the first episode of psychosis. In 1999, in their seminal paper, Nayani & David reported complexity increased with severity, and our results would concur with this within a first-episode psychosis sample. Reference Nayani and David25 Many other previous studies using standardised measures report volume, controllability, commenting and number of voices as key aspects. These latter features were also highlighted as those with better ability to distinguish from clinical and non-clinical groups. Reference Daalman, Boks, Diederen, de Weijer, Blom and Kahn26 Alternatively, voice location was described in our results as a confusing dimension; participants not only reported internal, external and mixed locations but also that this was unclear, difficult to describe and a changeable part of their experience. This suggests that this may indeed be the least clinically useful aspect and one that would not lend itself to robust research.

Novel findings from our study include the extent of AVH trying to control behaviour and emotions, the experience of AVH as an entity with complex social interactions and voices being imposed on individuals and occurring with multisensory additional factors.

In terms of AVH as controlling experiences, our results add additional evidence to a perceived lack of control and voice power in clinical populations. Reference Upthegrove, Ross, Brunet, McCollum and Jones27 Many studies also focus on the power patients ascribe to AVH, and this is an aspect with clinical relevance, in terms of AVH as a traumatic experience, suicidal ideation, acting on commands and persistence of voices, that our results also support. Reference Upthegrove, Ross, Brunet, McCollum and Jones27,Reference Birchwood, Iqbal and Upthegrove28 A number of participants reported the voice making demands, which may be likened to commands, but the assumption of instruction with power implied by a commanding voice should not be assumed. Voices were also often perceived as threatening and commenting on actions. These codes, which we have themed control of behaviour, are reflective of that already documented in descriptive psychopathology. Reference Oyebode2 Although voice authority was a feature from our primary data, this was a nuanced and more complex experience whereby voices were trying to gain authority and power through extensive and complex social interactions: manipulating comments, mocking, tricking and blaming. Emotional content is suggested as a discriminating feature of AVH in psychotic disorders by Daalman et al. Reference Daalman, Boks, Diederen, de Weijer, Blom and Kahn26 Our theme of emotional control concurs with this as a key area of future research and one that may have further clinical relevance in terms of risk of suicidal ideation and acts.

The suggestion of voice entity, over and above a realness of voice hearing, is also a novel finding which closely ties in with work that suggests voices are more than hallucinated sound. Bell proposes a social cognitive model of voice hearing in hallucinated social identities and one that is primarily a social relationship, with social environment predisposing to voice hearing from early childhood and later development. Reference Bell29 We have previously shown that within psychosis, childhood trauma may have a symptom-specific risk for hallucinatory experiences; however, the aetiological process whereby people who hear voices acquire what amounts to internalised social actors is under-researched. Reference Stanghellini, Langer, Ambrosini and Cangas10,Reference Upthegrove, Chard, Jones, Gordon-Smith, Forty and Jones30 In terms of neurocognitive models of AVH experience in psychosis, self-monitoring deficits may explain positive symptoms including hallucinations and passivity phenomena in schizophrenia with sensory confusion of self from non-self. Reference McGuire, David, Murray, Frackowiak, Frith and Wright31 deWeijer et al and others suggested AVH could be related to an error in ‘corollary discharge’. This is the process by which the initiation of a thought or act, such as speech, is accompanied by an alteration in neuronal discharges that alert the individual to the fact that such a thought or act is self-generated. Reference de Weijer, Neggers, Diederen, Mandl, Kahn and Pol32 With a self-generated thought or movement, accompanying signals to the sensory and motor cortex inform parts of the brain about an intended action, allowing the sensory areas to predict this possibility and code the sensory consequences as the expected consequence of this action. Allen et al Reference Allen, Modinos, Hubl, Shields, Cachia and Jardri33 suggested that in schizophrenia there is a predictive coding error that accounts for the perceived externality or misattribution of internally generated speech to an external source. Thus, this may explain the realness and auditory quality of voices. The development of complex social interactions with voices are highlighted in our results and this area remains open to investigation.

Our other novel findings of AVH experience include an emphasis on imposition; the sensation of ‘being done to’ with passivity. Traditionally, ‘true hallucinations’ occurring in psychotic disorders are conceived of as indistinguishable from real perceptions except that there is no stimulus. Jaspers’ view is that normal perceptions and true hallucinations are received with an attitude of passivity. By this he meant that the experience could not be altered by force of will. Reference Jaspers34 Our data further suggest that for patients with psychosis and AVH, this aspect of experience is accentuated. With ordinary speech, there is the option to ignore what someone is saying, to block it out, to change focus to other things or simply leave the room. With AVH in psychosis one cannot escape the experience and the verbal hallucination has a compelling and controlling dimension that renders it inescapable. Again, the role of corollary discharge may be relevant in the generation of this imposed AVH experience. Reference Heinks-Maldonado, Mathalon, Houde, Gray, Faustman and Ford35 Alternatively, within a psychosis sample, positive symptoms that are the result of salience dysregulation would be expected to be accompanied by a subjective experience of ‘draw’ or compulsion to a given salient stimuli. This would be in keeping with Howes et al Reference Howes, Shotbolt, Bloomfield, Daalman, Demjaha and Diederen9 and their distinction between psychotic and non-psychotic voices driven by dopamine dysregulation.

Finally, our novel findings include AVH experienced with additional multisensory factors, including pain, pressure and itching. Together with the reported feelings of imposition and control, the suggestion is that in this population there is an overlap between AVH and other positive symptoms of psychosis, such as hallucinations in other modalities or passivity phenomena. This may suggest distinguishing features that could be elicited in the phenomenology of voice-hearing in psychosis. However, recently Woods et al also report phenomenological data from a mixed sample using an online methodology, which included one open question subject to qualitative analysis; voices were also being experienced in a physical dimension, with 18% of their sample reporting AVH occurring with bodily sensations, including being in a dream-like state, feelings of heat or accompanied by tingling sensations. Reference Woods, Jones, Alderson-Day, Callard and Fernyhough6 Thus, this area needs further research and replication.

McCarthy-Jones et al Reference McCarthy-Jones, Thomas, Strauss, Dodgson, Jones and Woods36 call for subtyping to be a key advance needed in AVH research. Cutting across diagnostic groups, antecedent and treatment responses, and neurological categories, they propose that phenomenological subtypes include ‘hypervigilance’, ‘inner speech’, ‘autobiographical memory’, ‘epileptic’ and ‘deafferentation’. Our direct patient reports offer confirmation that some aspects of AVH in psychosis may be captured by these proposed groupings; however, additional factors would need further expansion. Primary research from our qualitative data reinforces the proposal that integration of methods in investigating AVH need stronger qualitative phenomenological focus. Reference Upthegrove, Caldwell, Ives, Oyebode and Wood37 One limitation highlighted in the past literature includes the repeated use of standardised measures to record voice-hearing phenomenology. Reference Stanghellini, Langer, Ambrosini and Cangas10 These include the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale and the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, which measure frequency and disruption, Reference Hedlund and Vieweg38,Reference Kay, Flszbein and Opfer39 and the Psychotic Symptom Rating Scale, which rates location, loudness, distress, controllability and beliefs. Reference Haddock, McCarron, Tarrier and Faragher40 In terms of content and power, the Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire rates benevolence, malevolence, omnipotence, resistance and engagement and the Voice Power Differential Scale rates voice power. Reference Chadwick, Lees and Birchwood41,Reference Birchwood, Meaden, Trower, Gilbert and Plaistow42 However, even in their totality, the novel phenomenological features we report, including the extent of social relationship and emotional control, imposition and multisensory additional features would not be captured.

This study has a number of strengths. Our use of novel, qualitative techniques allowed a more reflexive and direct aspect to AVH experience, and a mobile interview allowed participants to describe their experience in a non-threatening, open setting. By using diaries and photos collected prior to interview, the information we have gleaned was less ‘tainted’ by the researcher's questioning and should, therefore, be a credible representation of the nature of such experiences. Methodologies used were in-depth and aimed at gaining true first-hand accounts in a sample that is large in terms of qualitative research. Photo elicitation is a technique that is particularly useful in mental health research as it affords individuals the opportunity to explore feelings and experiences that may otherwise be difficult to articulate. Reference Kantrowitz-Gordon and Vandermause43 Without the use of a priori dimensions, standard questionnaires or structured interviews, we have been able to capture AVH aspects that participants themselves feel are most important. We were also able to continue interviewing until data saturation (i.e. no new themes emerged), giving confidence that the essence of the experience was captured within our sample.

The study does however have clear limitations. The sample of patients with psychosis, although our intended focus, could reflect a group identified with diagnostic criteria that already include frequent and complex voices, and as such our results only reflect this limited nosology. However, it is clear from literature that the diagnostic outcome from first-episode psychosis is broad, including affective and non-affective psychoses. Reference Heslin, Lomas, Lappin, Donoghue, Reininghaus and Onyejiaka44 Replication of our extensive qualitative methodology in non-psychotic groups is needed before any firm conclusions can be drawn. Although our sample contained participants from both genders, participants were predominantly male and from a White ethnic group. Future research is also needed using this methodology in non-help-seeking populations, and other cultures, to ascertain whether AVH experience is similar in these differing populations.

However, our results should be of clear interest to clinical, translational and biological researchers. Studies aiming to understand the underlying neuropathology of auditory hallucinations have been challenging. Neuroimaging studies looking to distinguish psychotic from non-psychotic AVH produced mixed results, Reference Upthegrove, Caldwell, Ives, Oyebode and Wood37,Reference Diederen, Daalman, de Weijer, Neggers, van Gastel and Blom45 and it may be that some aspects of AVH are common across all voice-hearing populations; however, key distinctions in psychotic voices, if present, have not been captured by studies using standardised rating scales. Our results should provide a starting point for future research aiming to address these questions and suggest that the themes of voice entity and control with their associated codes (see Tables 2 and 3) are important aspects to study and ones that may suggest areas of key clinical relevance.

Table 3 Theme 2: control

Code Number endorsing code Description Quote
Authority 14 Voices as powerful
Voices as ‘strong’ and participants as being too ‘weak’ to stop them
Voice take complete charge over participants’ thoughts and lives
Power associated with unpredictability, giving authority
‘It's the main part of my life at the minute yeah and I am pretty powerless to stop it because of how strong it is’
‘If it's really strong, the pull’
‘I'm always wary of it at the back of my mind, that it might happen’
‘I got no power, no intention, I can't block it at all’
Control of behaviour
   Making demands




   Commenting

   Threatening
13 Giving specific orders to be followed ‘I went to have like a relaxing sit down in the front room and the voices were saying “Get up, get up”’
Dictating to participants what they should be doing ‘You take that or you don't take that, eat this or you don't eat this’
Commands given that prevented opportunity for further thought or discussion ‘The way it would reply is no kind of open-ended questions, it'll always be in a statement manner’
3 Commenting statements about their daily activities ‘Oh she's writing in the diary, oh she's managed to go up the stairs’
‘Bitch is going to start a diary’
8 Physically threatening participants
Threats often accompanied by swearing
Forceful or angry
‘We're going to get you’
‘If you don't take it what we'll do is we'll burn you in hot water, boiled water’
‘“Bitch,” they added the swearing… I felt quite threatened by them’
‘They're really aggressive or angry, shouting’
Control of emotions
   Manipulative, tricking 7 Manipulative nature of voices
Ability to ‘use’ the individual's own voice to trick participants
Voices were trying to mislead or create doubt
‘Male voice can use my own voice against me…’
‘Creating situations, making me think that’
‘Look at what you've done… the impact of you and the decisions you've made’
   Blaming and mocking 9 Blaming or accusatory voice, which would criticise their choices or decisions, mocking, ridiculing, calling names ‘I must have been doing something wrong back in the day’
‘He was like “Ha ha, you deserved it because you brought all this pain and suffering”’
‘“Ha ha, you deserved it”’
‘Idiot… be like a man’
   Helpful/guiding 5 Helpful, offering guidance and support
Protective, able to defend participants in times of need
‘They would tell me that it's not… so bad and tell me to calm down… help’
‘Telling me to avoid certain roads or… maybe get out a bit earlier or do certain things for my own good’
‘I'll make sure you come out on top’
Disruption
   Interfering



   Constant


   Compelling
17 Participants struggle to function; continually being hindered by their voices Interfering, to the extent that their lives had been destroyed ‘I couldn't be out in pub, I didn't want to out in public or anything, I didn't want to be around crowds cause… I'd hear voices through the people’
‘I'm trying to do something normal like have a cup of tea and they're bothering me’
‘I ain't got a life anymore really… I can't do anything’
14 Constant occurrence of voices
Voices as continually being in the background, voices were typically unrelenting
‘All day, every day’
‘It's constantly niggling you’
‘Constantly bombarding you’
16 Voices difficult to ignore
Urged to follow their voices, not necessarily through their own choice, but because they found them so compelling
Attention diverted solely to their voices, the absolute focus of awareness
‘You can't get away from it’
‘What you're meant to be doing is looking at me and answering me, however, you need to be paying attention to what they're saying’
‘Anything that's going on around me just seems to fade away and I'm just concentrating on what the voices are telling me’
‘It eggs me on’
Imposed
   Experienced as being ‘done to’







   Physical imposition and additional sensations
17 Participants passive in the experience
Voices force themselves upon participants who were unable to stop hearing them
‘It's like being possessed, you know. What can you do? What can you really do’
‘It's like I'm being held down’
‘I'm constantly caged in by voices’
‘My body will recognise it’
‘When they choose to talk to me, they choose to talk to me and they take over they take over the whole situation as it is at the time’
‘It's out of my hands’
‘if it's really strong, the pull, I just loose whole control of it’
11 Reported with bodily sensations
From feeling itchy to actual physical pain, participants described how strongly their voices were experienced alongside sensations in other modalities
‘Giving me pain, giving me physical pain basically… I felt from inside organ… pain going on’
‘Makes me feel sick’
‘My whole body, its feeling has been changed’
‘It's not even just a voice um it's… I've had like a push on my shoulder um… I've smelt like things which like you wouldn't expect…’
‘They poke and prod me and put pressure on me’
‘I can feel it yeah. It's like… acupuncture
pressure on my neck and the back of my head and that’
‘When it happens, I itch for ages’

Funding

This study was funded by Healthy Minds Charitable Trust and Birmingham & Solihull Mental Health Foundation Trust.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Waters, F, Allen, P, Aleman, A, Fernyhough, C, Woodward, TS, Badcock, JC, et al. Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia and nonschizophrenia populations: a review and integrated model of cognitive mechanisms. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38: 683–93.Google Scholar
2 Oyebode, F. Sims' Symptoms in the Mind: Textbook of Descriptive Psychopathology. Elsevier Health Sciences, 2014.Google Scholar
3 Anthony, D. The cognitive neuropsychiatry of auditory verbal hallucinations: an overview. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2004; 9: 107–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 Bak, M, Myin-Germeys, I, Hanssen, M, Bijl, R, Vollebergh, W, Delespaul, P, et al. When does experience of psychosis result in a need for care? A prospective general population study. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29: 349–58.Google Scholar
5 Larøi, F, Sommer, IE, Blom, JD, Fernyhough, C, Ffytche, DH, Hugdahl, K, et al. The characteristic features of auditory verbal hallucinations in clinical and nonclinical groups: state-of-the-art overview and future directions. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38: 724–33.Google Scholar
6 Woods, A, Jones, N, Alderson-Day, B, Callard, F, Fernyhough, C. Experiences of hearing voices: analysis of a novel phenomenological survey. Lancet Psychiatry 2015; 2: 323–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Slotema, C, Daalman, K, Blom, J, Diederen, K, Hoek, H, Sommer, I. Auditory verbal hallucinations in patients with borderline personality disorder are similar to those in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2012; 42: 1873–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Looijestijn, J, Diederen, KM, Goekoop, R, Sommer, IE, Daalman, K, Kahn, RS, et al. The auditory dorsal stream plays a crucial role in projecting hallucinated voices into external space. Schizophr Res 2013; 146: 314–9.Google Scholar
9 Howes, OD, Shotbolt, P, Bloomfield, M, Daalman, K, Demjaha, A, Diederen, KMJ, et al. Dopaminergic function in the psychosis spectrum: an [18F]-DOPA imaging study in healthy individuals with auditory hallucinations. Schizophr Bull 2013; 39: 807–14.Google Scholar
10 Stanghellini, G, Langer, ÁI, Ambrosini, A, Cangas, AJ. Quality of hallucinatory experiences: differences between a clinical and a non-clinical sample. World Psychiatry 2012; 11: 110–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11 O'Connor, K, Nelson, B, Lin, A, Wood, SJ, Yung, A, Thompson, A. Are UHR patients who present with hallucinations alone at lower risk of transition to psychosis? Psychiatry Res 2016; 235: 117–96.Google Scholar
12 Broome, MR, Harland, R, Owen, GS, Stringaris, A. The Maudsley Reader in Phenomenological Psychiatry. Cambridge University Press, 2013.Google Scholar
13 Stanghellini, G, Broome, MR. Psychopathology as the basic science of psychiatry. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 205: 169–70.Google Scholar
14 Lataster, T, Valmaggia, L, Lardinois, M, van Os, J, Myin-Germeys, I. Increased stress reactivity: a mechanism specifically associated with the positive symptoms of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med 2013; 43: 1389–400.Google Scholar
15 Woods, A, Jones, N, Bernini, M, Callard, F, Alderson-Day, B, Badcock, JC, et al. Interdisciplinary approaches to the phenomenology of auditory verbal hallucinations. Schizophr Bull. 2014; 40 (suppl 4): S24654.Google Scholar
16 Goldsmith, LP. A discursive approach to narrative accounts of hearing voices and recovery. Psychosis Psychol Soc Integr Approaches 2012; 4: 235–45.Google Scholar
17 Sandhu, A, Ives, J, Birchwood, M, Upthegrove, R. The subjective experience and phenomenology of depression following first episode psychosis: a qualitative study using photo-elicitation. J Affect Disord 2013; 149: 166–74.Google Scholar
18 Upthegrove, R, Birchwood, M, Ross, K, Brunett, K, McCollum, R, Jones, L. The evolution of depression and suicidality in first episode psychosis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2010; 122: 211–8.Google Scholar
19 World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10). WHO, 1991.Google Scholar
20 Erdner, A, Magnusson, A. Photography as a method of data collection: helping people with long-term mental illness to convey their life world. Perspect Psychiatr Care 2011; 47: 145–50.Google Scholar
21 Harper, D. Talking about pictures: a case for photo elicitation. Visual Stud 2002; 17: 1326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22 Harrington, C, Lindy, I. The Use of Reflexive Photography in the Study of the Freshman Year Experience. Taylor and Francis, 1998.Google Scholar
23 Krippendorff, K. Content Analysis: An Introduction to its Methodology. Sage, 2012.Google Scholar
24 Hsieh, H-F, Shannon, SE. Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual Health Res 2005; 15: 1277–88.Google Scholar
25 Nayani, TH, David, AS. The auditory hallucination: a phenomenological survey. Psychol Med 1996; 26: 177–89.Google Scholar
26 Daalman, K, Boks, M, Diederen, K, de Weijer, AD, Blom, JD, Kahn, RS, et al. The same or different? A phenomenological comparison of auditory verbal hallucinations in healthy and psychotic individuals. J Clin Psychiatry 2011; 72: 320–5.Google Scholar
27 Upthegrove, R, Ross, K, Brunet, K, McCollum, R, Jones, L. Depression in first episode psychosis: the role of subordination and shame. Psychiatry Res 2014; 217: 177–84.Google Scholar
28 Birchwood, M, Iqbal, Z, Upthegrove, R. Psychological pathways to depression in schizophrenia: studies in acute psychosis, post psychotic depression and auditory hallucinations. Eur Aarch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005; 255: 202–12.Google ScholarPubMed
29 Bell, V. A community of one: social cognition and auditory verbal hallucinations. PLoS Biol 2013; 11: e1001723.Google Scholar
30 Upthegrove, R, Chard, C, Jones, L, Gordon-Smith, K, Forty, L, Jones, I, et al. Adverse childhood events and psychosis in bipolar affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry 2015; 206: 191–7.Google Scholar
31 McGuire, P, David, A, Murray, R, Frackowiak, R, Frith, C, Wright, I, et al. Abnormal monitoring of inner speech: a physiological basis for auditory hallucinations. Lancet 1995; 346: 596600.Google Scholar
32 de Weijer, AD, Neggers, SF, Diederen, K, Mandl, RC, Kahn, RS, Pol, H, et al. Aberrations in the arcuate fasciculus are associated with auditory verbal hallucinations in psychotic and in non-psychotic individuals. Hum Brain Mapp 2013; 34: 626–34.Google Scholar
33 Allen, P, Modinos, G, Hubl, D, Shields, G, Cachia, A, Jardri, R, et al. Neuroimaging auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: from neuroanatomy to neurochemistry and beyond. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38: 695703.Google Scholar
34 Jaspers, K. General Psychopathology. JHU Press, 1997.Google Scholar
35 Heinks-Maldonado, TH, Mathalon, DH, Houde, JF, Gray, M, Faustman, WO, Ford, JM. Relationship of imprecise corollary discharge in schizophrenia to auditory hallucinations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64: 286–96.Google Scholar
36 McCarthy-Jones, S, Thomas, N, Strauss, C, Dodgson, G, Jones, N, Woods, A, et al. Better than mermaids and stray dogs? Subtyping auditory verbal hallucinations and its implications for research and practice. Schizophr Bull 2014; 40 (suppl 4): S27584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Upthegrove, RBM, Caldwell, K, Ives, J, Oyebode, F, Wood, SJ. How we Understand Hallucinations; a systematic review of current evidence. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2015; 15 Dec, doi: 10.111/acps.12531.Google Scholar
38 Hedlund, JL, Vieweg, BW. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS): a comprehensive review. J Oper Psychiatry 1980; 11: 4865.Google Scholar
39 Kay, SR, Flszbein, A, Opfer, LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13: 261.Google Scholar
40 Haddock, G, McCarron, J, Tarrier, N, Faragher, E. Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS). Psychol Med 1999; 29: 879–89.Google Scholar
41 Chadwick, P, Lees, S, Birchwood, M. The revised Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire (BAVQ-R). Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177: 229–32.Google Scholar
42 Birchwood, M, Meaden, A, Trower, P, Gilbert, P, Plaistow, J. The power and omnipotence of voices: Subordination and entrapment by voices and significant others. Psychol Med 2000; 30: 337–44.Google Scholar
43 Kantrowitz-Gordon, I, Vandermause, R. Metaphors of distress photo-elicitation enhances a discourse analysis of parents' accounts. Qual Health Res 2015; pii: 1049732315575729. (Epub ahead of print) Google Scholar
44 Heslin, M, Lomas, B, Lappin, J, Donoghue, K, Reininghaus, U, Onyejiaka, A, et al. Diagnostic change 10 years after a first episode of psychosis. Psychol Med 2015; 45: 2757–69.Google Scholar
45 Diederen, KM, Daalman, K, de Weijer, AD, Neggers, SF, van Gastel, W, Blom, JD, et al. Auditory hallucinations elicit similar brain activation in psychotic and nonpsychotic individuals. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38: 1074–82.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Clinical and demographic details

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Themes and codes.

Figure 2

Table 2 Theme 1: entity

Figure 3

Table 3 Theme 2: control

Supplementary material: PDF

Upthegrove et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Upthegrove et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 540.6 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.