Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T01:48:57.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stalking risks to celebrities and public figures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 April 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Prominent figures are frequently subjected to unwanted and intrusive attentions. Such stalking behaviour is often driven by psychotic illness, angrily blaming the public figure for delusional persecution (resentful motivation), or based on erotomanic delusions (intimacy seeking motivation), for example. This behaviour can cause psychological harm to both perpetrator and victim, and is unlawful. In the rare instances where a public figure has been attacked, the perpetrator has usually had a history of such stalking behaviour and of severe mental illness. For these reasons, early identification and diversion into appropriate care and treatment will be for the benefit of both parties and will prevent more serious violence in a minority of cases. The importance of the provision of education to improve both reporting rates by victims and an appropriate response from the criminal justice system is highlighted. A multi-agency approach involving the criminal justice system and mental health services is the most effective means of achieving these aims.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

None.

LEARNING OBJECTIVES

  • Learn that severe mental illness, particularly psychosis, is often an important driver of stalking behaviour

  • Learn that delusional disorder is a treatable mental illness

  • Appreciate that prevention rather than prediction is the approach to managing the risks of high-harm low-probability outcomes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2018 

Stalking is the term given to repeated and unwanted intrusive behaviour towards another person which creates distress. This might include correspondence (in all its varied modern forms) and/or approaches. In clinical terms, such behaviour appears to have a bimodal distribution, and a critical two-week threshold is often referred to (Mullen Reference Mullen, James and Meloy2009a). In about half of cases, it is short-lived, lasting less than two weeks, usually only a few days. However, for the other half, it is very likely to last much longer, many months (Purcell Reference Purcell, Pathé and Mullen2004). ‘Pursuing a course of conduct’ that amounts to harassment or stalking constitutes a criminal offence in the UK (under the Protection from Harassment Act 1997).

Celebrities and public figures, because of their high profile and because their role requires them to court public affection and attention, are at particular risk of stalking. On the very rare occasions when a public figure has been attacked, stalking behaviour has generally been a prelude, and hence it is of interest to the specialist units around the world with responsibility for managing these risks – for example, the Fixated Threat Assessment Centre (FTAC) in the UK (James Reference James, Farnham, Wilson, Meloy and Hoffmann2014), the Los Angeles Police Department Threat Management Unit (Dunn Reference Dunn, Meloy and Hoffmann2014), the US Secret Service, the Australian Federal Police Fixated Threat Assessment Team, and other dignitary protection teams. This article provides an overview of the best evidence in this area, together with our experience as threat assessment professionals and clinicians.

Although stalking is a relatively new area for psychiatry, regicides (Régis Reference Régis1890), erotomania and paranoia, all relevant to the stalking of public figures, have a much longer history. There is also increasing evidence that a number of problem behaviours previously thought to be separate phenomena may be better understood as related, with more in common than they have to distinguish them, encompassing stalking, the pursuit of public figures, making threats and abnormally persistent complaining. There is also some overlap with another cluster of phenomena previously thought to be distinct, including public figure assassins, school shooters, workplace shooters and lone-actor terrorists (Hempel Reference Hempel, Levine and Meloy2000; McCauley Reference McCauley, Moskalenko and Van Son2013; Capellan Reference Capellan2015). Such phenomena often have in common the development of a pathological fixation on a person or cause motivated by a grievance, ending in violence. It appears to be a sadly ubiquitous (Hempel Reference Hempel, Levine and Meloy2000), although very rare, human phenomenon that, from time to time, a person will ‘snap’ and run amok (Spores Reference Spores1988), acting with extreme violence and killing many people. The particular narrative (‘school shooting’ or ‘terrorist incident’, say) invoked by the perpetrator or the media as apparent explanation would probably better be replaced by asking the question, ‘Why is it that, in this time and place, this is the way in which extreme distress is manifested?’ – Hacking's (Reference Hacking1998) ‘ecological niche’.

Some concepts from the threat assessment literature

The pooling of these varied areas of inquiry reveals shared themes that are significant in the stalking of celebrities and public figures.

First, pathological fixation (Mullen Reference Mullen, Pathé and Purcell2009b) – the development of an abnormal, highly focused and all-encompassing interest in a person or cause that comes to dominate the life of the stalker, leading them to believe they are entitled to a relationship with the person whom they are pursuing. This fixation distinguishes them from ordinary fans, who may hope for a relationship, but do not have this sense of ‘entitled reciprocity’ (Meloy Reference Meloy, Sheridan, Hoffmann, Meloy, Sheridan and Hoffmann2008).

Second, grievance and the importance of thinking about the motivation for engaging in this unwanted behaviour, which we explore in more detail below.

Third, serious violence as a possible outcome, and specifically targeted violence, i.e. aimed at a specific person and likely to have been planned in advance. This offers opportunities for earlier discovery, prevention and disruption. It also distinguishes it from the sort of violence we may be more used to in psychiatric settings – namely, impulsive violence where a person in a highly aroused state strikes out, in the moment, at whoever happens to be nearby. Sometimes the original target is never reached – perhaps because they are too well protected and a more accessible one is selected instead. In our experience, when there have been adverse outcomes, it has often been the friends and family of the stalker who have been the victim. Threat management professionals themselves need to be mindful that they may become a part of the grievance (Federal Bureau of Investigation 2017). Violent outcomes are, of course, rare, and we will also be interested in preventing the persistence of the stalking behaviour, the psychosocial damage it causes to the stalker and the victim, the risk of an escalation of the behaviour, and the risks of disruption at high-profile events or locations.

Epidemiology

Stalking in the general population is common – in the UK the lifetime prevalence of being stalked is 15% (20.2% for women and 9.8% for men (Office for National Statistics 2016)). The prevalence of stalking of celebrities and public figures is less researched, but probably much higher. For example, a survey of UK members of parliament (MPs) (James Reference James, Sukhwal and Farnham2016a,Reference James and Farnhamb) suggested that 81% had experienced, at the hands of constituents, at least one of the twelve inappropriate behaviours asked about in the study, and the career prevalence of being stalked was 38%. In addition, 18% had been subjected to an attack or an attempted attack. In The Netherlands, 33% of politicians had been stalked during their lifetime and 46% threatened (Malsch Reference Malsch, Visscher and Blaauw2002), in Sweden 74% of MPs reported having experienced harassment, threats or violence (Staatens Offentliga Utredningar 2006) and 28% of Norwegian MPs had experienced ‘genuine stalking’ (Narud Reference Narud and Dahl2015). In terms of celebrities, a German study reported a 79% career prevalence of stalking among television personalities (Hoffmann Reference Hoffmann, Sheridan, Meloy, Sheridan and Hoffmann2008).

Mental disorder, warning behaviours and attacks

The stalkers of public figures and celebrities come to notice either by their unwanted communications or by inappropriate approaches. The Exceptional Case Study (Fein Reference Fein and Vossekuil1999) looked at 83 people who had attacked, or approached to attack, a prominent public figure in the USA between 1949 and 1996: 61% had a history of psychiatric illness, 43% a history of delusional ideas (88% had acted on them) and 11% experienced command hallucinations. For 77% there was a history of verbal or written communication with the target. In a survey of attacks on European politicians between 1990 and 2004 (James Reference James, Mullen and Meloy2007), 10 attackers (42%) were psychotic at the time, and there was evidence of warning behaviours in 11 cases. Looking at 23 attacks on the British royal family between 1778 and 1994 (James Reference James, Mullen and Pathé2008), 48% of the attackers were psychotic, and 10 out of the 23 exhibited warning behaviours. There are similar findings from the Dutch royal family (van der Meer Reference van der Meer, Bootsma and Meloy2012), Canadian justice officials (Eke Reference Eke, Meloy and Brooks2014), the US Congress (Dietz Reference Dietz, Matthews and Martell1991a) and Hollywood celebrities (Dietz Reference Dietz, Matthews and Van Duyne1991b). This is a group with very high rates of severe mental illness, primarily psychosis, and with high rates of ‘warning behaviours’. Taken together these two clinical features offer the opportunity to intervene to prevent adverse outcomes.

In examining the behaviour of public figure stalkers, several important features to look out for emerge from the literature (Box 1), as well as suggesting close attention be paid to ‘warning behaviours’ (Box 2), indicators that the subject may be moving along a hypothesised ‘pathway to violence’ (Calhoun Reference Calhoun and Weston2016). It is unclear how useful this theoretical ‘pathway model’ is in practice. Experience in this field suggests that individuals rarely move smoothly along a path, can skip certain of the hypothesised steps and can proceed to violence without engaging in the other steps. However, evidence of finality in language and leakage to a third party are well-recognised precursors in many of those who do resort to violence.

BOX 1 Assessing the behaviour of public figure stalkers

Important features to look out for include:

  • threats

  • approaching with a weapon (or a bizarre approximation of one)

  • attempting to breach security

  • anger

  • declarations of specific intents

  • suicidal or homicidal language

  • last-resort language

  • delusional beliefs with horrific content (such as being raped or having one's organs removed)

  • erotomania

  • delusions of being a deity (and therefore above the law), of having a special destiny or of kinship

BOX 2 ‘Warning behaviours’ of public figure stalkers

The following behaviours indicate an increased risk of physical assault:

  • adoption of a ‘pseudo-commando’ or ‘warrior’ mentality

  • evidence of new aggression

  • an increase in energy levels

  • the making of threats or leaking information about their plans to a third party

  • evidence of finality in their acts or language

(Meloy Reference Meloy, Hoffmann and Guldimann2012)

Whatever model is adopted, an effective threat management unit needs to capture such ‘knowable’ information, to share it with the right people in a timely fashion and to respond promptly to proactively manage behaviours of concern. This will involve raising awareness of the unit's existence and purpose with a wide range of potential stakeholders, as well as sharing information about the underlying empirical evidence for this work.

Motivational typology

Why would one choose to correspond with or to meet a public figure? Cases cluster into similar motivational groups to those in the wider stalking literature (Box 3), although particular public figures may tend to attract one group more than another. For example, Parliament has the constitutional function of resolving grievances, so it comes as no surprise to learn that MPs often attract those seeking help with pathological grievances. Young, attractive, eligible celebrities or royalty may be a particular focus for erotic interest, and therefore for the pathologies of love, too.

BOX 3 Motivational typology of stalkers of celebrities and public figures

Rejected Former intimates who have been unable to come to terms with the loss of the relationship

Resentful Those who are angrily pursuing a grievance or other idiosyncratic agenda, blaming the public figure or demanding their help

Intimacy seekers Those who believe they are entitled to, or are already in, a close relationship with the public person: this includes those with erotomania, pretenders and amity seekers

Help seekers A more hapless and helpless group seeking assistance from the public figure for a personal problem in their lives

Incompetent suitors Those attempting, in a socially inept way, to initiate a sexual relationship with the public figure

Predatory Those making clandestine pre-attack preparations for a sexual attack: a rare group

Attention seekers Those seeking attention for themselves or for a personal cause, and using the profile of the public figure as a means to this end

Chaotic A common group who are so floridly psychotic that it is impossible to make sense of the underlying motivation

The rejected

These are former intimates of the public figure who have been unable to come to terms with the loss of the relationship. Although the most common group of stalkers, these are rarely the province of public figure or celebrity threat management units, as their origins lie in the personal life of the prominent person rather than in their public role.

The resentful

This is the group that most commonly comes to the attention of public figure threat management units (50% of referrals to FTAC (James Reference James, Farnham and Sukhwal2016c)) and the most concerning in terms of violence risk. They are angrily pursuing a grievance or another idiosyncratic agenda, either blaming the public figure for their persecution, or demanding the help of the public figure in remedying the situation. Clinically, this group contains many people with paranoid illnesses – the victims of ‘gang stalking’ (Sheridan Reference Sheridan and James2015), ‘targeted individuals’ (see below), and others complaining of mind control, secret government experiments or being controlled by ‘apps’, for example. These are not new phenomena – see, for example, Jay's (Reference Jay2012) account of James Tilly Matthews, the victim of ‘mind control’ at the hands of the ‘Air Loom Gang’ in the 18th century. He was committed to Bethlem Hospital in 1797 after causing a disturbance in Parliament, shouting ‘Treason’ at the Home Secretary, having first written to him to complain about conspiracies. Also in the resentful category are the abnormally persistent complainants (Lester Reference Lester, Wilson and Griffin2004), a complex and clinically difficult group. Their complaint has frequently begun with a genuine grievance, but their response has evolved, often over decades, into something highly pathological and out of all proportion to the initial insult. For some the grievance will have assumed a delusional character requiring psychiatric management, although this is often made very difficult by their propensity to incorporate clinical staff into their pattern of complaining and litigating. For others, although they may not be delusional, their behaviour is clearly highly problematic for themselves and others around them. Complaints departments are extremely familiar with such people, even if psychiatrists are not.

The intimacy seekers

The intimacy-seeking group (24% of referrals to FTAC (James Reference James, Farnham and Sukhwal2016c)) believe they are entitled to, or already have, a close personal relationship with the public figure, as an advisor (the amity seeking), a sexual partner (the erotomanic) or because they themselves hold the particular public role (the pretenders). They are very commonly suffering from psychotic illnesses with delusional beliefs about these matters. They sometimes send in biological material for DNA testing as proof of their kinship. They are probably even more common among the stalkers of celebrities.

The help seekers

Help seekers are those writing to or approaching public figures in a more hapless and helpless, rather than angry and entitled, manner. They may raise concerns about their own welfare when they are turning to the public figure for help as a last resort.

The incompetent suitors

Incompetent suitors may contact public figures, perhaps in quite abnormal or persistent ways, in hopes of initiating an intimate relationship. Commonly, those in this group have difficulties understanding social rules and norms, and individuals with autistic spectrum disorders and intellectual disabilities are overrepresented.

The predatory

Predatory stalkers are those making clandestine preparations prior to a sexual attack. This is a rare group, containing high-risk sexual offenders, occasionally seen by celebrity stalking management units. Not only are they very uncommon, it is rare for them to be referred to psychiatric services as, unlike the other motivational groups, they are at pains to keep their preparations secret.

The attention seekers

A group of individuals come to the attention of threat management units owing to unusual behaviour designed to attract attention to themselves for reasons of self-aggrandisement or notoriety. Personality disorders, often of the borderline, emotionally unstable and histrionic types, are probably overrepresented in this group, and the general approach is to attempt to extinguish the unwanted behaviour by avoiding institutional overreaction, and it is often better to try to minimise the involvement of the threat management unit.

The chaotic

Finally, there are the chaotic, those who are so floridly psychotic that it is almost impossible to discern any underlying reason or purpose for their approach or correspondence, often because of profound thought disorder. They made up 16% of referrals to FTAC in the James & Farnham (Reference James, Farnham and Sukhwal2016c) study, although sadly, in our experience, they are increasingly common as mental health services are cut. Examples include a young man who drove from Italy to Buckingham Palace to hand over a 600-page dossier to the Queen, the contents of which were incomprehensible, and an elderly woman who sent pieces of string and wool to the Prime Minister, together with tattered bank statements and identification documents.

Threats

Threats occupy an odd position in this work and have a complex literature. Sending correspondence which is not threatening is associated with a higher likelihood of approaching (Meloy Reference Meloy2014). There is one group of stalkers – ‘howlers’ (Calhoun Reference Calhoun and Weston2016) or ‘shockers’ (Warren Reference Warren, Mullen, McEwan, Meloy and Hoffmann2014) – whose aim is to cause fear and distress in their target and they therefore send very alarming and threatening correspondence, but are unlikely to approach or attack as they have achieved their purpose with their writings. However, making threats is also associated with a high level of mortality in the longer term, both suicide and homicide (MacDonald Reference MacDonald1968; Warren Reference Warren, Mullen and Thomas2008). The absence of threatening communications should not reassure the clinician that the person does not pose a threat, and if threats are made, they should be taken seriously. One modifying factor may be the ‘intimacy effect’. Violence is much more likely in a stalking case when the two parties have formerly been intimate, and in this group, threats of violence are significantly associated with actual violence (Calhoun Reference Calhoun and Weston2016).

Unusual clinical features

In line with the studies, it is our experience that those who approach or send problematic correspondence to public figures have high levels of severe mental illness. There are also some unusual features. There are high rates of encapsulated delusional disorder (including, of course, erotomania), where the person can appear to maintain a high level of functioning and the presence of mental illness can be missed altogether by the inexperienced. We have also encountered many cases of folie à deux and internet-facilitated folie à plusieurs, where people with paranoid illnesses, particularly those featuring some form of intrusive government surveillance, are able to find others with the same delusional beliefs online. In particular, there are communities of people who describe themselves as ‘targeted individuals’ and as victims of ‘gang stalking’ (Sheridan Reference Sheridan and James2015). Their symptoms often include highly concerning horrific delusions, and several recent US mass shootings have been perpetrated by ‘targeted individuals’, apparently as a direct result of their psychotic experiences – Jiverley Wong in the 2009 Binghamton shootings, Aaron Alexis at the Washington Naval Yard in 2013, Myron May at Florida State University in 2014, and Gavin Long in Baton Rouge in 2016, for example. In other words, not all delusional beliefs are equal when it comes to making a clinical assessment of the patient (see ‘threat/control-override’ symptoms and violence risk in psychosis more generally (Link Reference Link, Stueve and Phelan1998)).

Peripatetic patients are also common. People are attracted to particular public figures (especially the Queen, the US President, the Pope and certain Hollywood celebrities) from around the world, and travel internationally in pursuit of delusional quests. Sometimes people travel around the country or the world in an attempt to flee delusional persecution. Sometimes people move to evade mental health services, often with great success. This can present great difficulties for mental health services that are tied to catchment areas and legal jurisdictions.

A word on celebrities

Much of the threat management research has centred on public figures. The response to managing threats is dependent on the government response in different countries. For example, the existence of an embedded National Health Service, with mental health teams arranged according to geographical location, has been fundamental to the ability of FTAC to operate in the UK to assist with the diversion of those needing psychiatric treatment into health care rather than punishment in the criminal justice system. It is no surprise that the proliferation of stalkers of celebrities in Los Angeles was noted following the murder of Rebecca Schaeffer by a stalker in 1989, and that it led to a change in stalking laws in California and the establishment of the LAPD Threat Management Unit. However, for a celebrity sitting outside of this specialist jurisdiction, it is likely that any management of a stalking case would fall to local police. There are differences in presentation and it is unwise to assume that all lessons learnt with public figures will provide a total description of the circumstances facing a celebrity victim of stalking. It is long noted that celebrities are often reluctant to bring stalkers to the attention of the authorities. There may also be difficulties in distinguishing a stalker from an obsessive ‘superfan’. The importance of education for those in the public eye without the safeguards enjoyed by some public figures should be emphasised, although it remains to be seen how this is best achieved.

Social media

Many of the studies undertaken on stalkers of celebrities and public figures pre-date the dawn of social media. Nowadays, maintaining a large following on social networking sites such as Instagram and Twitter is seen as essential to the business of self-promotion. Celebrities often address their fans in a familiar way, promoting the idea of intimacy. The stalkers among their potentially large audience, some of whom may be psychotic, may misinterpret this familiar tone. Social media can be used as a ‘weapon’ in the stalker's armoury (Suzy Lamplugh Trust 2016), and general guidance on the use of social media and cyberstalking would be of benefit to celebrities. Indeed, minor celebrities with no access to advice or security may be particularly vulnerable.

Threat assessment and risk assessment

Threat (unlike risk) assessment is conducted rapidly, in an actively evolving, operational environment, with limited information, as a means of triaging and prioritising cases. For example, imagine two referrals come into the unit at the same time. The first involves a man who has been corresponding angrily with his MP for many months or years about a grievance. He has sent a new letter with a change in the tone of his language. He says he has reached the end of the line, that there will be no more correspondence, and he will be coming to the MP's surgery that evening to end matters once and for all. The second consists of a large volume of sexually explicit correspondence from a woman to a young prince, insisting that they are destined to be together and asking him to collect her from Heathrow airport when her flight arrives from the USA. Both cases will require some intervention, but it is the former that would be given a higher priority – the sender is angry, demanding, using last-resort language and has given a deadline. However, further inquiries reveal that the correspondent in the former case is serving a lengthy prison sentence and is in no position to visit the MP's surgery, whereas the woman with erotomania is currently on a flight from the USA. The prioritisation of the cases would reverse.

The motivational typologies described, together with warning behaviours and risk factors, can be used to assign cases to prioritisation categories. Structured professional judgement tools from the stalking literature can assist (e.g. the Stalking Risk Profile (MacKenzie Reference MacKenzie, McEwan and Pathé2009)). They can also help in identifying a range of treatment targets in individual cases – for example, reminding one to think not only about the treatment of the psychotic illness, but also to address the patient's substance misuse, social isolation and cognitive distortions related to their perceived relationship with the public figure. They are also useful in maintaining focus on managing risks in a variety of domains, not only violence, but also persistence, escalation or disruption. What they cannot do is predict bad outcomes with certainty. Rare events, such as suicides, are mathematically impossible to predict, however good the risk assessment tool (Nielssen Reference Nielssen, Wallace and Large2017), because the positive predictive value is dependent on the prevalence of the condition being measured. In rare conditions, even if the test used does much better than chance or clinical opinion, it will still be wrong much more often that it is right (a high rate of false positives).

Large & Nielssen (Reference Large and Nielssen2011) advocate focusing on the welfare of the patient rather than the current preoccupation with risks of suicide or violence. This view has some merit, as it is not possible to predict what any individual will do. However, the use of risk factors does allow for the identification of the group of fixated loners most at risk, and from which dangerous behaviour is most likely to arise. Intervening and treating the risk factors in this entire group will prevent adverse outcomes without needing to know which individuals would have gone on to engage in the behaviour in question. If one's response is a reactive intervention (to perceived ‘risk’), one is always in a state of chasing the behaviour or risk being exhibited, hoping to minimise the potential violence or communicated threat. In taking a preventive approach, we endeavour to assist as many people as possible into care, prioritising those in higher-risk groups when they are ill, thereby doing them a health good and preventing adverse outcomes.

Interventions

Threat management units have no additional powers: the focus is on making referrals to the relevant agencies responsible for managing the person, sharing information to enable them to make the best decisions they can and to coordinate their responses. The efficacy of an intervention will also be dependent on the context of the country, its government, laws, health system and sanctions. Given the high prevalence of psychosis among stalkers, the most common interaction in the UK is between the threat management unit and the local health service (the person's family doctor or mental health team). There is no barrier to information sharing in these circumstances – health professionals from two teams involved in the care of a patient (the threat management team and the local team) are sharing information to improve the patient's management (General Medical Council 2017). In general, the direction of information flow is from the threat management team to the local clinical team, providing them with additional information about their patient's behaviour and mental state from their interactions with the public figure.

Issues arising at FTAC when effecting an intervention in the UK

The interface between a threat management team and a mental health service, like all interfaces in psychiatry, often throws up interesting material. We have encountered forms of institutional acting out, including the ‘opt-in letter’ (Wilson Reference Wilson, Farnham and Henley2013) and the ‘consenting service’, two exquisite service designs for excluding individuals with anosognosia as a result of psychosis. This can require clinical escalation to obtain the appropriate response to referrals – expecting someone with grandiose messianic delusions to make an appointment if they'd like to discuss their mental health is plainly an absurd response from a mental health service.

There are genuine clinical complexities, though, in this work. How should one deal with a person with a chronic untreated psychotic illness who is refusing treatment when compulsion under mental health legislation does not appear to be a proportionate response? What do we mean when we say a person in a chronically angry and paranoid mental state is not a risk to themselves or others? They are clearly at much higher relative risk of violence than someone (including themselves) not in that mental state (Coid Reference Coid, Ullrich and Kallis2013). Perhaps we mean that the violence is not imminent? In which case, we are assuming (or hoping) we will find out about any violence shortly before it happens so that we can intervene at that point. Is this reasonable or wise? Perhaps we mean the absolute risk is low, but then the absolute risk of serious violence will always be low. ‘Playing the odds’ is no substitute for clinical judgement. We know that psychiatric thinking about this area, even in experts, is muddled (Large Reference Large, Ryan and Callaghan2012).

These difficulties can often be fruitfully explored in individual cases via professionals' meetings, involving the relevant agencies (perhaps the local mental health team, family doctor, threat management team and local police). Psychotic illnesses require treatment in the usual way with antipsychotic medication, psychological therapy and family support. The importance of occupational therapy cannot be overstated, although is often difficult to obtain, in helping people replace the hole left in their lives when they choose to stop their pursuit of the public figure. Delusional disorders, despite received clinical wisdom, are just as responsive to treatment as other psychotic illnesses. Manschreck & Khan (Reference Manschreck and Khan2006), in their review of 134 published case reports of delusional disorder, report that 89.6% either recovered or improved in response to treatment (mostly with antipsychotic medication). The therapeutic pessimism associated with such cases is not warranted.

In some cases, the criminal justice system may need to be involved, because there is no mental disorder at all, or as an additional means of providing mental healthcare (perhaps via the criminal sections of mental health legislation or by adding mental health treatment to a community order) or to provide the person with a clear message that their behaviour is unwanted and unlawful. The criminal offences most likely to be involved are those of harassment, stalking or malicious communications. A threat management team, often being jointly staffed by mental health clinicians and police officers, may be in a unique position to help oversee and coordinate multi-agency responses like this.

Threat management teams also offer advice and training in relation to this unusual area of practice, giving presentations to police forces and mental health services, sharing best practice and being involved with the planning of large events (Pathé Reference Pathé, Haworth and Lowry2015). Hoffmann & Sheridan (Reference Hoffmann, Sheridan, Meloy, Sheridan and Hoffmann2008) observed that historically there was little sympathy for celebrity victims of stalking, that in some way this behaviour ‘goes with the territory’ and they should endure it. However, the police need to take such reports seriously and to utilise multi-agency forms to achieve best results.

Conclusions

The stalking of public figures and celebrities is common and often motivated by mental illness. There are highly specialised threat management teams working in this area around the world, often combining law enforcement officers and mental health clinicians. However, the mainstay of management in these cases is the familiar model of criminal justice liaison and diversion, and mental health clinicians working in local services are likely to come across cases of this sort either within their existing case-load or via a referral from threat management teams. It is helpful to think about the patient's motivation in stalking the public figure, to maintain therapeutic optimism about the treatment of mental disorder in such cases and to take advantage of the expertise of threat management teams, who may be useful allies in facilitating better multi-agency communication and intervention. Understanding the ‘who, what and how’ of sharing ‘knowable’ information develops a shared risk relationship between the mental health provider, the family, community and threat management team. This helps develop a case management team approach, which intersects and accesses the many resources, both clinical and legal, that are available. Information is critical to properly develop such an approach. Privacy laws, based on interpretation and institutional policy, may block this flow of information. In most cases, relevant information can be shared between the healthcare provider and law enforcement agency. Mental health clinicians in the threat management team are able to communicate with those in the treating team. At minimum, law enforcement should be satisfied with one-way communication between themselves and the treating staff, ensuring that the reporting is as complete and accurate as possible to enable the best clinical decision-making. This will include a coordinated discharge plan and follow-up management, which integrates the work of the mental healthcare provider and the threat management professionals. This is especially important with individuals who are treatment resistant or who have relapsing–remitting conditions and are at risk of repeating their stalking behaviour in future episodes of illness. The concerns in such cases are that the unwanted behaviours may persist, cause psychosocial harm to the stalker and the victim, may escalate, may cause disruption at public events or, rarely, may result in an attack. The treatment of underlying mental disorder will reduce these risks.

MCQs

Select the single best option for each question stem

  1. 1 Stalking or harassment is best described as:

    1. a following a person without them knowing

    2. b making threats

    3. c engaging in repeated, unwanted intrusive behaviours of any kind

    4. d standing outside a public figure's house

    5. e threatening to kill a public figure.

  2. 2 The motivations encountered most commonly by public figure threat management teams is:

    1. a rejected motivation

    2. b resentful motivation

    3. c chaotic motivation

    4. d intimacy-seeking motivation

    5. e predatory motivation.

  3. 3 Regarding those who have attacked public figures:

    1. a they are likely to have a history of serious mental illness and to have exhibited warning behaviour

    2. b they are unlikely to have a psychotic illness

    3. c their delusional beliefs are unlikely to be longstanding

    4. d the motivation is likely to have been political

    5. e they are likely to be psychopathic.

  4. 4 Regarding delusional disorder:

    1. a it rarely responds to treatment

    2. b it only responds to certain antipsychotic medications

    3. c treatment is likely to result in improvement in most cases

    4. d it is always easy to diagnose

    5. e chronic delusions about mind control are harmless.

  5. 5 Regarding the risk assessment of serious violence:

    1. a modern risk assessment tools perform no better than chance

    2. b the positive predictive value is independent of the prevalence of serious violence

    3. c risk assessment tools are useful in predicting serious violence

    4. d when the prevalence of serious violence is very low, the false-positive rate will be low

    5. e when the prevalence of serious violence is very low, the false-positive rate will be high.

MCQ answers

1 c 2 b 3 a 4 c 5 e

References

Calhoun, FS, Weston, SW (2016) Threat Assessment and Management Strategies: Identifying the Howlers and Hunters (2nd edn). CRC Press.Google Scholar
Capellan, JA (2015) Lone wolf terrorist or deranged shooter? A study of ideological active shooter events in the United States, 1970–2014. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, 38: 395413.Google Scholar
Coid, JW, Ullrich, S, Kallis, C (2013) The relationship between delusions and violence: findings from the East London First Episode Psychosis Study. JAMA Psychiatry, 70: 465–71.Google Scholar
Dietz, PE, Matthews, DB, Martell, DA, et al. (1991 a) Threatening and otherwise inappropriate letters to members of the United States Congress. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 36: 1445–68.Google Scholar
Dietz, PE, Matthews, DB, Van Duyne, C, et al. (1991 b) Threatening and otherwise inappropriate letters to Hollywood celebrities. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 36: 185209.Google Scholar
Dunn, J (2014) The Los Angeles Police Department Threat Management Unit. In International Handbook of Threat Assessment (eds Meloy, JR, Hoffmann, J): 285–98. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eke, AW, Meloy, JR, Brooks, K, et al. (2014) Threats, approach behavior, and violent recidivism among offenders who harass Canadian justice officials. Journal of Threat Assessment and Management, 1: 188202.Google Scholar
Federal Bureau of Investigation (2017) Making Prevention a Reality: Identifying, Assessing, and Managing the Threat of Targeted Attacks. FBI.Google Scholar
Fein, RA, Vossekuil, B (1999) Assassination in the United States: an operational study of recent assassins, attackers, and near-lethal approachers. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 44: 321–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
General Medical Council (2017) Confidentiality: Good Practice in Handling Patient Information. GMC.Google Scholar
Hacking, I (1998) Mad Travellers: Reflections on the Reality of Transient Mental Illnesses. Free Association Books.Google Scholar
Hempel, AG, Levine, RE, Meloy, JR, et al. (2000) A cross-cultural review of sudden mass assault by a single individual in the oriental and occidental cultures. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 45: 582–8.Google Scholar
Hoffmann, J, Sheridan, L (2008) Celebrities as victims of stalking. In Stalking, Threatening, and Attacking Public Figures (eds Meloy, JR, Sheridan, L, Hoffmann, J): 195–214. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
James, DV, Mullen, PE, Meloy, JR, et al. (2007) The role of mental disorder in attacks on European politicians 1990–2004. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 116: 334–44.Google Scholar
James, DV, Mullen, PE, Pathé, MT, et al. (2008) Attacks on the British royal family: the role of psychotic illness. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 36: 5967.Google Scholar
James, DV, Farnham, FR, Wilson, SP (2014) The Fixated Threat Assessment Centre: implementing a joint policing and psychiatric approach to risk assessment and management in public figure threat cases. In International Handbook of Threat Assessment (eds Meloy, JR, Hoffmann, J): 299–320. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
James, DV, Farnham, FR, Sukhwal, S, et al. (2016 a) Aggressive/intrusive behaviours, harassment and stalking of members of the United Kingdom parliament: a prevalence study and cross-national comparison. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 27: 177–97.Google Scholar
James, DV, Sukhwal, S, Farnham, FR, et al. (2016 b) Harassment and stalking of members of the United Kingdom Parliament: associations and consequences. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 27: 309–30.Google Scholar
James, DV, Farnham, FR (2016 c) Outcome and efficacy of interventions by a public figure threat assessment and management unit: a mirrored study of concerning behaviors and police contacts before and after intervention. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 34: 660–80.Google Scholar
Jay, M (2012) The Influencing Machine: James Tilly Matthews and the Air Loom. Strange Attractor Press.Google Scholar
Large, MM, Nielssen, OB (2011) Probability and loss: two sides of the risk assessment coin. The Psychiatrist, 35: 413–8.Google Scholar
Large, M, Ryan, CJ, Callaghan, S (2012) Hindsight bias and the overestimation of suicide risk in expert testimony. The Psychiatrist, 36: 236–7.Google Scholar
Lester, G, Wilson, B, Griffin, L, et al. (2004) Unusually persistent complainants. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184: 352–6.Google Scholar
Link, BG, Stueve, A, Phelan, J (1998) Psychotic symptoms and violent behaviors: probing the components of ‘threat/control-override’ symptoms. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33: S5560.Google Scholar
MacDonald, JM (1968) Homicidal Threats. CC Thomas.Google Scholar
MacKenzie, RD, McEwan, TE, Pathé, MT, et al. (2009) The Stalking Risk Profile: Guidelines for the Assessment and Management of Stalkers. Stalkinc. & Centre for Forensic Behavioural Science.Google Scholar
Malsch, M, Visscher, M, Blaauw, E (2002) Stalking van bekende personen [Stalking of Well-known People]. Boom.Google Scholar
Manschreck, TC, Khan, NL (2006) Recent advances in the treatment of delusional disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 51: 114–9.Google Scholar
McCauley, C, Moskalenko, S, Van Son, B (2013) Characteristics of lone-wolf violent offenders: a comparison of assassins and school attackers. Perspectives on Terrorism, 7: 424.Google Scholar
Meloy, JR, Sheridan, L, Hoffmann, J (2008) Public figure stalking, threats, and attacks: the state of the science. In Stalking, Threatening, and Attacking Public Figures (eds Meloy, JR, Sheridan, L, Hoffmann, J): 3–34. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meloy, JR, Hoffmann, J, Guldimann, A, et al. (2012) The role of warning behaviors in threat assessment: an exploration and suggested typology. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 30: 256–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meloy, JR (2014) Approaching and attacking public figures: a contemporary analysis of communications and behavior. Journal of Threat Assessment and Management, 1: 243–61.Google Scholar
Mullen, PE, Pathé, M, Purcell, R (2009 a) Stalkers and their Victims (2nd Edn). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mullen, PE, James, DV, Meloy, JR, et al. (2009 b) The fixated and the pursuit of public figures. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 20: 3347.Google Scholar
Narud, K, Dahl, AV (2015) Stalking experiences reported by Norwegian members of parliament compared to a general population sample. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 26: 116–31.Google Scholar
Nielssen, O, Wallace, D, Large, M (2017) Pokorny's complaint: the insoluble problem of the overwhelming number of false positives generated by suicide risk assessment. BJPsych Bulletin, 41: 1820.Google Scholar
Office for National Statistics (2016) Compendium: Intimate Personal Violence and Partner Abuse. ONS.Google Scholar
Pathé, MT, Haworth, DJ, Lowry, TJ, et al. (2015) A model for managing the mentally ill fixated person at major events. Australia & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49: 610–5.Google Scholar
Purcell, R, Pathé, M, Mullen, PE (2004) Editorial: when do repeated intrusions become stalking? Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 15: 571–83.Google Scholar
Régis, E (1890) Les régicides dans l'histoire et dans le présent: etude médico-psychologique [Regicides in History and in the Present: A Medico-Psychological Study]. Maloine.Google Scholar
Sheridan, LP, James, DV (2015) Complaints of group-stalking (‘gang-stalking’): an exploratory study of their nature and impact on complainants. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 26: 601–23.Google Scholar
Spores, JC (1988) Running Amok: An Historical Inquiry. Ohio University.Google Scholar
Staatens Offentliga Utredningar [Government Official Reports] (2006) Jakten på makten. Betänkande av Kommittén om hot och våld mot förtreondevalda [The Quest for Power. Report of the Committee on Threats and Violence against Elected Officials]. Fritzes.Google Scholar
Suzy Lamplugh Trust (2016) The Stalker in Your Pocket (https://www.suzylamplugh.org/past-national-stalking-awareness-weeks). Accessed 18 January 2018.Google Scholar
van der Meer, BB, Bootsma, L, Meloy, R (2012) Disturbing communications and problematic approaches to the Dutch Royal Family. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 23: 571–89.Google Scholar
Warren, LJ, Mullen, PE, Thomas, SDM, et al. (2008) Threats to kill: a follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 38: 599605.Google Scholar
Warren, LJ, Mullen, PE, McEwan, TE (2014) Explicit threats of violence. In International Handbook of Threat Assessment (eds Meloy, JR, Hoffmann, J): 18–38. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, S, Farnham, F, Henley, S (2013) Opt-in letters. The Psychiatrist, 37: 340.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.