In 1923 Martha May Eliot launched a 3-year prospective trial in New Haven, Connecticut, to determine whether rickets could be prevented by giving infants cod liver oil and teaching mothers to ‘sunbath’ babies even in the winter (Eliot Reference Eliot1925). Her pragmatic community trial was successful because nurses were able to convince mothers of the value of cod liver oil and they even demonstrated how the baby's mouth was to be held open when administering it. Only 4% in the intervention group, but more than 20% in the control group, developed moderate rickets. This seminal trial not only changed public health practice but also demonstrated that vitamin D requirements differ depending on the infant and that nutritional interventions can best be combined with lifestyle changes (Hunt Reference Hunt1995).
The Lally & Gaughran review
In this edition of BJPsych Advances, Lally & Gaughran review studies on vitamin D in schizophrenia and depression (Lally Reference Lally and Gaughran2019). Several studies reported a cross-sectional association between vitamin D and depression and, to a lesser extent, schizophrenia. However, the authors rightly focus on rigorous longitudinal studies and clinical trials to answer the question whether there is a place for vitamin D supplementation in treatment or prevention of depression and schizophrenia.
Their careful review provides important background information and, although not a systematic review, summarises the seminal studies well. The authors note the paucity of longitudinal data. Further, most of the few prospective studies could not confirm the cross-sectional results. The trials in depression and the single randomised study of schizophrenia were also negative. Hence, evidence is lacking that vitamin D insufficiency is a potential cause of depression or schizophrenia rather than a consequence. The authors point out that the depression trials are limited as they are underpowered, of small sample size and have heterogeneous study populations. They ask again how we should adapt the general population advice for vitamin D use in patients with depression or schizophrenia; they conclude that we do not know and that ‘a presumptive diagnosis of insufficiency could be made, based on risk factors’. Others would argue that the evidence strongly suggests there is no reason to treat these patients differently than other groups at high risk of vitamin D deficiency (McGrath Reference McGrath2017).
Advice for researchers
No advice for clinicians can be given, but what advice should we give clinical or population researchers on the basis of Lally & Gaughran's review? In view of the poverty of research this is a pressing question and I will try to make some suggestions.
First, the research reflects insufficiently that vitamin D has been implicated in an indefinite number of acute and chronic conditions. Low vitamin D levels seem a frailty indicator or a biomarker of a poor health status at times. Depression and schizophrenia certainly occur often as a direct or indirect consequence of disorders that have been related to vitamin D. This means that observational studies must control much more carefully for confounding by disease and, importantly, by subclinical traits. This should resolve some of the discrepancies between studies. Against this background, the studies reviewed by Lally & Gaughran that specifically look at vitamin D and bone mineral density only in psychosis are probably not the way forward; it is unclear why this particular relation should differ in psychiatric patients. Rather, carefully controlled longitudinal studies of multiple morbidities accounting for competing risks are needed.
Second, Lally & Gaughran mention that people with more pigmented skin have a higher rate of psychosis and lower vitamin D levels when living in northern or southern countries than those with less pigmented skin. In particular, persons from Black African or Caribbean ethnicity are affected. Vitamin D treatment or prevention studies in darker-skinned populations are called for (Dealberto Reference Dealberto2007; McGrath Reference McGrath2011).
Third, reverse causality, as Lally & Gaughran point out, is a likely explanation for the discrepancy between cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Why does nobody test this explanation? With repeated measures of vitamin D, it would be easy to examine whether depression or schizophrenia lead to changes in vitamin D levels owing, for example, to less outdoor activity.
Finally, and most importantly, we need another Martha May Eliot to rock the boat of the small cross-sectional clinical vitamin D studies currently performed in psychiatry. We need another pragmatic and prospective community trial of vitamin D supplementation combined with lifestyle advice. Like the mothers of young children, persons with low levels of vitamin D at high risk of depression and psychosis may need to be taught how to effectively exercise, sunbathe and eat healthily.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.