Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T16:50:15.430Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sublexical modulation of simultaneous language activation in bilingual visual word recognition: The role of syllabic units

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2014

MARKUS CONRAD*
Affiliation:
Universidad de la Laguna, Tenerife, Spain Freie Universität, Berlin, Germany
CARLOS J. ÁLVAREZ
Affiliation:
Universidad de la Laguna, Tenerife, Spain
OLIVIA AFONSO
Affiliation:
Universidad de la Laguna, Tenerife, Spain
ARTHUR M. JACOBS
Affiliation:
Freie Universität, Berlin, Germany
*
Address for correspondence: Markus Conrad, Dpto. Psicología Cognitiva, Social y Organizacional, Facultad de Psicología, Campus de Guajara, Universidad de La Laguna 38205-S/C de Tenerife - Canary Islands, Spain Email: [email protected]

Abstract

We addressed the question of whether syllabic units of the presented language would activate words containing these syllables in the nonpresented language. In two lexical decision experiments using Spanish and German words presented to two groups of late Spanish–German and German–Spanish bilinguals and to two monolingual control groups, target words’ syllable-frequency in the nonpresented language was manipulated. Inhibitory effects of syllable-frequency in the nonpresented language were found only when Spanish–German bilinguals read German L2 words– suggesting that L2 sublexical syllabic units activated L1 syllabic neighbors’ representations that would interfere with L2 target processing. On the contrary, no inhibitory effects but rather a facilitation tendency due to syllable-frequency from the nonpresented German language was obtained for both groups of bilinguals reading Spanish words. This dissociation concerning the spread of activation from sublexical units to lexical representations from bilinguals’ two languages is discussed in terms of structural differences between the two languages.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M. & de Vega, M. (2000). Syllable frequency effect in visual word recognition: Evidence of a sequential-type processing. Psicológica, 21 (3), 341374.Google Scholar
Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., & Perea, M. (2004). Are syllables phonological units in visual word recognition? Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 427452.Google Scholar
Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., & Taft, M. (2001). Syllables and morphemes: Contrasting frequency effects in Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 545555.Google Scholar
Álvarez, C. J., de Vega, M., & Carreiras, M. (1998). La Sílaba como unidad de activación léxica en la lectura de palabras trisílabas. Psicothema, 10, 371386.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM). Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, 1995.Google Scholar
Barber, H., Vergara, M. y Carreiras, M. (2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport, 15 (3), 545548.Google Scholar
Beauvillain, C. (1992). Orthographic and lexical constraints in bilingual word recognition. In Harris, R.J. (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals, pp. 221235. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Bijeljac-Babic, R., Biardeau, A., & Grainger, J. (1997). Masked orthographic priming in bilingual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25, 447457.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (1998). Word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence against the existence of two separate lexicons. Psychologica Belgica, 38, 163175.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (2003), Bilingual visual word recognition: Evidence from masked phonological priming In Kinoshita, S. & Lupker, S.J. (Eds.), Masked priming: The state-of-the-art. Hove UK: Psychology Press. Masked priming: The state-of-the-art, pp. 323–343. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Van Wijnendaele, I. (2003), The importance of phonological coding in visual word recognition: Further evidence from second-language processing, Psychologica Belgica, 43, 249258.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Van Dyck, G. & Van de Poel, M. (1999). Visual word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence from masked phonological priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 137148.Google Scholar
Carrasco-Ortiz, H., Midgley, K. J., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2012). Are phonological representations in bilinguals language specific? An ERP study on interlingual homophones. Psychophysiology, 49 (4), 531543.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Álvarez, C. J., & de Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766780.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., & Perea, M. (2004). Naming pseudowords in Spanish: Effects of syllable frequency. Brain and Language, 90, 393400.Google Scholar
Chetail, F., Colin, C., & Content, A. (2012). Electrophysiological markers of syllable frequency during written word recognition in French. Neuropsychologia, 50 (14), 34293439.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Tamm, S., Carreiras, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2010). Simulating syllable frequency effects within an interactive activation framework. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22 (5), 861893.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Carreiras, M., Tamm, S., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Syllables and bigrams: orthographic redundancy and syllabic units affect visual word recognition at different processing levels. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 461479.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Carreiras, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2008). Contrasting effects of token and type syllable frequency in lexical decision. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23 (2), 296326.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2007). Phonology as the source of syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition: Evidence from French. Memory & Cognition, 35 (5), 974983.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Stenneken, P., & Jacobs, A. M. (2006). Associated or dissociated effects of syllable frequency in lexical decision and naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13 (2), 339345.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2004). Replicating syllable-frequency effects in Spanish in German: One more challenge to computational models of visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19 (3), 369390.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., Delmaar, P., & Lupker, S. J. (2000). The processing of interlexical homographs in translation recognition and lexical decision: Support for non-selective access to bilingual memory. Quaterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 397428.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., Borgwaldt, S., Bos, M., & Van den Eijnden, E. (2002). Lexical decision and word naming in bilinguals: Language effects and task effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 91124.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (1998). The BIA model and bilingual Word recognition. In Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. M. (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition, pp. 189225. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Maywah NJ.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: from identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 219224.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Grainger, J., & Van Heuven, W.J.B. (1999). Recognition of cognates and interlingual homographs: The neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 496518.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Miwa, K., Brummelhuis, B., Sappelli, M., and Baayen, R.H. (2010) How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 284–30.Google Scholar
Doctor, E. A., & Klein, D. (1992). Phonological processing in bilingual word recognition. In Harris, R.J. (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals, pp. 237252. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Doignon, N., & Zagar, D. (2005). Illusory conjunctions in French: The nature of sublexical units in visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20 (3), 443464.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Masked translation priming effects with highly proficient simultaneous bilinguals. Experimental Psychology, 57 (2), 98107.Google Scholar
Duyck, W., Drieghe, D., Diependaele, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). The size of the cross-lingual masked phonological priming effect does not depend on second language proficiency. Experimental Psychology, 51, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Forster, K. I., & Frost, R. (1997). Translation priming with different scripts: Masked priming with cognates and noncognates in Hebrew–English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23, 11221139.Google Scholar
Grainger, J. (1993). Visual word recognition in bilinguals. In Schreuder, R. & Weltens, B. (Eds.), The bilingual lexicon, pp. 1125. Amsterdam–Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grainger, J., & Dijkstra, T. (1992). On the representation and use of language information in bilinguals. In Harris, R.J. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in bilinguals, pp.207220. Amsterdam: Elsevier Google Scholar
Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 22, 696713.Google Scholar
Haigh, C. A., & Jared, D. (2007). The activation of phonological representations by bilinguals while reading silently: Evidence from interlingual homophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 33, 623644.Google ScholarPubMed
Hutzler, F., Conrad, M. y Jacobs, A. M. (2005). Effects of syllable frequency in lexical decision and naming: An eye movement study. Brain and Language, 92 (2), 138152.Google Scholar
Hutzler, F., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2005). Effects of syllable-frequency in lexical decision and naming: An eye-movement study. Brain and Language, 92 (2), 138152.Google Scholar
Jacobs, A. M., Nuerk, H. C., Graf, R., Braun, M, & Nazir, T. A. (2008). The initial capitalization superiority effect in German: evidence for a perceptual frequency variant of the orthographic cue hypothesis of visual word recognition. Psychological Research, 72 (6), 657665.Google Scholar
Jared, D., & Kroll, J. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 231.Google Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., & Dijkstra, T. (2004). Recognizing cognates and interlingual homographs: Effects of code similarity in language-specific and generalized lexical decision. Memory & Cognition, 32, 533550.Google Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., Dijkstra, T., Schriefers, H., Baayen, R. H., Grainger, J., & Zwitserlood, P. (2008). Native language influences on word recognition in a second language: A megastudy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1231.Google Scholar
López-Zamora, M., Luque, J. L., Álvarez, C. J., & Cobos, P. L. (2013). Individual differences in categorical perception are related to sub-lexical/phonological processing in reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 63, 239252.Google Scholar
Lukatela, G., Savić, M., Gligorijević, B., Ognjenović, P., Turvey, M. T. (1978) Bi–alphabetical lexical decision. Language and Speech, 21, 142–65.Google Scholar
Maïonchi-Pino, N., Magnan, A., & Ecalle, J. (2010). Syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition: Developmental approach in French children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 7082.Google Scholar
Mathey, S., & Zagar, D. (2002). Lexical similarity in visual word recognition: The effect of syllabic neighborhood in French. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior, Brain and Cognition, 8, 107121.Google Scholar
Mathey, S., Zagar, D., Doignon, N., & Seigneuric, A. (2006). The nature of the syllabic neighbourhood effect in French. Acta Psychologica, 123, 372393.Google Scholar
Midgley, K. J., Holcomb, P. J., van Heuven, W. J. B., & Grainger, J. (2008). An Electrophysiological investigation of cross-language effects of orthographic neighborhood. Brain Research, 1246, 123135.Google Scholar
Midgley, K. J., Holcomb, P. J. & Grainger, J. (2010). Effects of Cognate Status on Second Language Learners and Proficient Bilinguals Investigated with Event-Related Potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 16341647.Google Scholar
Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (1998). Effects of syllable frequency and syllable neighborhood frequency in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 134144.Google Scholar
Peressotti, F., Cubelli, R., Job, R. (2003). On recognizing proper names: The orthographic cue hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 47 (1), 87116.Google Scholar
Ruschemeyer, S., Nojack, A., & Limbach, M. (2008). A mouse with a roof? Effects of phonological neighbors on processing of words in sentences in a non-native language. Brain and Language, 104 (2), 113121.Google Scholar
Sebastián, N., Martí, M. A., Carreiras, M. F., & Cuetos, F. (2000). LEXESP, léxico informatizado del Español. Barcelona: Ediciones de la Universitat de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. (1987). Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy? In Coltheart, M. (Ed.), Attention and performance: The psychology of reading (Vol 7). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96 (4), 523568.Google Scholar
Stenneken, P., Conrad, M., Goldenberg, G. & Jacobs, A. M. (2003). Visual Processing of Sublexical Units in Dyslexia. Brain and Language, 87, 137138.Google Scholar
Stenneken, P., Conrad, M., Hutzler, F., Braun, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2005). Frequency effects with visual words and syllables in a dyslexic reader. Behavioural Neurology, 16 (2–3, SPECIAL ISSUE), 103117.Google Scholar
Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 11401154.Google Scholar
Taft, M., & Forster, K. I. (1976). Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 607620.Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G. & Dijkstra, T. (2002). Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 780789.Google Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J. B., Dijkstra, T. (2010). Language comprehension in the bilingual brain: fMRI and ERP support for psycholinguistic models. Brain Research Reviews, 64, 104122.Google Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J. B., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J. (1998). Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 458483.Google Scholar
Van Heuven, W., Schriefers, H., Dijkstra, A., & Hagoort, P. (2008). Language conflict in the bilingual brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 27062716.Google Scholar
Van Wijnendaele, I., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Visual word recognition in bilingual: Phonological priming from the second to the first language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 616627.Google Scholar
Voga, M., & Grainger, J. (2007). Cognate status and cross-script translation priming. Memory & Cognition, 35, 938952.Google Scholar