Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T04:11:54.470Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Structural priming in bilinguals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2017

ROGER P. G. VAN GOMPEL*
Affiliation:
University of Dundee
MANABU ARAI
Affiliation:
Seijo University
*
Address for correspondence: Roger van Gompel, School of Social Sciences, Perth Road, University of Dundee, Dundee, DD1 4HN, Scotland, UK[email protected]

Abstract

In this review, we examine how structural priming has been used to investigate the representation of first and second language syntactic structures in bilinguals. Most experiments suggest that structures that are identical in the first and second language have a single, shared mental representation. The results from structures that are similar but not fully identical are less clear, but they may be explained by assuming that first and second language representations are merely connected rather than fully shared. Some research has also used structural priming to investigate the representation of cognate words. We will also consider whether cross-linguistic structural priming taps into long-term implicit learning effects. Finally, we discuss recent research that has investigated how second language syntactic representations develop as learners’ proficiency increases.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*We would like to thank Martin Pickering and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on a previous version of this article. Roger van Gompel acknowledges support from ESRC award ES/P001866/1.

References

Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 (5), 931–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.931Google Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2009). Persistence of emphasis in language production: A cross-linguistic approach. Cognition, 112 (2), 300317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2012). Effects of phonological feedback on the selection of syntax: Evidence from between-language syntactic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (3), 503516. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2013). From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals. Cognition, 127 (3), 287306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.005Google Scholar
Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18 (3), 355387. http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90004-6Google Scholar
Bock, K. (1989). Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cognition, 31 (2), 163186. http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(89)90022-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: transient activation or implicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129 (2), 177192. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.129.2.177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K., & Loebell, H. (1990). Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 139. http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90035-ICrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to structural relations: bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150–71. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.150Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J. (in press). An experimental approach to linguistic representation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., McLean, J. F., & Stewart, A. J. (2006). The role of local and global syntactic structure in language production: Evidence from syntactic priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21 (7–8), 9741010. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600824609Google Scholar
Cai, Z. G., Pickering, M. J., Yan, H., & Branigan, H. P. (2011). Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65 (4), 431445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.05.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Baumann, M., Pappert, S., & Fitz, H. (2015). Do lemmas speak German? A verb position effect in German structural priming. Cognitive Science, 39 (5), 11131130. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113 (2), 234272. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, B., Jia, Y., Wang, Z., Dunlap, S., & Shin, J.-A. (2013). Is word-order similarity necessary for cross-linguistic structural priming? Second Language Research, 29, 375389. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658313491962Google Scholar
Cleland, A. A., & Pickering, M. J. (2003). The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 49 (2), 214230. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00060-3Google Scholar
Costa, A., Caramazza, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2000). The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26 (5), 12831296. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1283Google Scholar
Desmet, T., & Declercq, M. (2006). Cross-linguistic priming of syntactic hierarchical configuration information. Journal of Memory and Language, 54 (4), 610632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández, E. M., De Souza, R. A., & Carando, A. (2017). Bilingual innovations: Experimental evidence offers clues regarding the psycholinguistics of language change. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20 (2), 251268. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000924CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleischer, Z., Pickering, M. J., & McLean, J. F. (2012). Shared information structure: Evidence from cross-linguistic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (3), 568579. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000551Google Scholar
Flett, S., Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2013). Are non-native structural preferences affected by native language preferences? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16 (4), 751760. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Bernolet, S. (2017). The development of shared syntax in second language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20 (2), 219234. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000164Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Beerts, S., Loncke, M., Desmet, T., & Bernolet, S. (2016). Cross-linguistic structural priming in multilinguals: Further evidence for shared syntax. Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 1430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.003Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D. (2008). Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 (2), 214238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.003Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Psychological Science, 15 (6), 409414. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00693.xGoogle Scholar
Heydel, M., & Murray, W. S. (2000). Conceptual effects in sentence priming: A crosslinguistic perspective. In de Vincenzi, M. & Lombardo, V. (Eds.), Cross-linguistic perspectives on language processing (pp. 227254). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). Cognate effects in picture naming: Does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition, 106 (1), 501511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.02.001Google Scholar
Hsin, L., Legendre, G., & Omaki, A. (2013). Priming cross-linguistic interference in Spanish–English bilingual children. In Baiz, S., Goldman, N., & Hawkes, R.: Proceedings of the 37th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 165177). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Jacob, G., Katsika, K., Family, N., & Allen, S. E. M. (2017). The role of constituent order and level of embedding in cross-linguistic structural priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20 (2), 269282. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000717Google Scholar
Kantola, L., & Van Gompel, R. P. G. (2011). Between- and within-language priming is the same: evidence for shared bilingual syntactic representations. Memory & Cognition, 39 (2), 276–90. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0016-5Google Scholar
Kidd, E., Chan, A., & Chiu, J. (2015). Cross-linguistic influence in simultaneous Cantonese–English bilingual children's comprehension of relative clauses. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (3), 438452. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000649CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Tennant, E., & Nitschke, S. (2015). Shared abstract representation of linguistic structure in bilingual sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22 (4), 10621067. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0775-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kootstra, G. J., & Doedens, W. J. (2016). How multiple sources of experience influence bilingual syntactic choice: Immediate and cumulative cross-language effects of structural priming, verb bias, and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (4), 710732. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000420Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33 (2), 149174. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loebell, H., & Bock, K. (2003). Structural priming across languages. Linguistics, 41 (5), 791824. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.2003.026Google Scholar
Luka, B. J., & Choi, H. (2012). Dynamic grammar in adults: Incidental learning of natural syntactic structures extends over 48 h. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 (2), 345360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meijer, P. J. A., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2003). Building syntactic structures in speaking: A bilingual exploration. Experimental Psychology, 50 (3), 184195. https://doi.org/10.1027//1618-3169.50.3.184Google Scholar
Nitschke, S., Kidd, E., & Serratrice, L. (2010). First language transfer and long-term structural priming in comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25 (1), 94114. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960902872793CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nitschke, S., Serratrice, L., & Kidd, E. (2014). The effect of linguistic nativeness on structural priming in comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29 (5), 525542. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.766355Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39 (4), 633651. http://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2592Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134 (3), 427459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.427CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runnqvist, E., Gollan, T. H., Costa, A., & Ferreira, V. S. (2013). A disadvantage in bilingual sentence production modulated by syntactic frequency and similarity across languages. Cognition, 129 (2), 256263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.008Google Scholar
Salamoura, A., & Williams, J. N. (2006). Lexical activation of cross-language syntactic priming. Bilingualism, 9 (3), 299307. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002641Google Scholar
Salamoura, A., & Williams, J. N. (2007). Processing verb argument structure across languages: Evidence for shared representations in the bilingual lexicon. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28 (4), 627660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070348Google Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56 (2), 153171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shin, J. A., & Christianson, K. (2009). Syntactic processing in Korean-English bilingual production: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Cognition, 112 (1), 175180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.03.011Google Scholar
Shin, J. A., & Christianson, K. (2012). Structural priming and second language learning. Language Learning, 62 (3), 931964. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00657.xGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., Gámez, P. B., Gómez, L. E., Bowers, E., & Shimpi, P. (2010). Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 37 (5), 10471064. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990213Google Scholar
Weber, K., & Indefrey, P. (2009). Syntactic priming in German-English bilinguals during sentence comprehension. NeuroImage, 46 (4), 11641172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.040Google Scholar