Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T07:21:11.510Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Priming and persistence in bilinguals: What codeswitching tells us about lexical priming in sentential contexts*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2021

Michael A. Johns*
Affiliation:
Applied Research Laboratory for Intelligence and Security, University of Maryland, College Park, United States of America
Laura Rodrigo
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish, Italian and Portuguese, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, United States of America
Rosa E. Guzzardo Tamargo
Affiliation:
Department of Hispanic Studies, University of Puerto Rico at Río Piedras, San Juan, United States of America
Aliza Winneg
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish, Italian and Portuguese, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, United States of America
Paola E. Dussias
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish, Italian and Portuguese, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, United States of America
*
Address for correspondence: Michael A. Johns, 7005 52nd Avenue, College Park, MD, USA, 20708, The Applied Research Laboratory for Intelligence and Security, The University of Maryland. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Most studies on lexical priming have examined single words presented in isolation, despite language users rarely encountering words in such cases. The present study builds upon this by examining both within-language identity priming and across-language translation priming in sentential contexts. Highly proficient Spanish–English bilinguals read sentence-question pairs, where the sentence contained the prime and the question contained the target. At earlier stages of processing, we find evidence only of within-language identity priming; at later stages of processing, however, across-language translation priming surfaces, and becomes as strong as within-language identity priming. Increasing the time between the prime sentence and target question results in strengthened priming at the latest stages of processing. These results replicate previous findings at the single-word level but do so within sentential contexts, which has implications both for accounts of priming via automatic spreading activation as well as for accounts of persistence attested in spontaneous speech corpora.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Assadollahi, R and Pulvermüller, F (2003) Early influences of word length and frequency: a group study using MEG. Neuroreport 14, 11831187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basnight-Brown, DM and Altarriba, J (2007) Differences in semantic and translation priming across languages: The role of language direction and language dominance. Journal of Memory and Cognition 35, 953965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D, Kliegl, R, Vasishth, S and Baayen, H (2015) Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967.Google Scholar
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.Google Scholar
Baus, C, Costa, A and Carreiras, M (2013) On the effects of second language immersion on first language production. Acta psychologica 142, 402409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beatty-Martínez, AL, Navarro-Torres, CA, Dussias, PE, Bajo, MR, Guzzardo Tamargo, RE and Kroll, JF (2019) Interaction Context Mediates the Consequences of Bilingualism for Language and Cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46, 10221047.Google Scholar
Becker, CA (1980) Semantic context effects in visual word recognition: An analysis of semantic strategies. Journal of Memory and Cognition 8, 493512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bijeljac-Babic, R, Biardeau, A and Grainger, J (1997) Masked orthographic priming in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Cognition 25, 447457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K and Griffin, ZM (2000) The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129, 177192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodner, GE and Stalinski, SM (2008) Masked repetition priming and proportion effects under cognitive load. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 62, 127131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M, Armstrong, BC, Perea, M and Frost, R (2014) The what, when, where, and how of visual word recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18, 9098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M, Vergara, M and Barber, H (2005) Early event-related potential effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 18031817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, F, Dell, GS and Bock, K (2006) Becoming Syntactic. Psychological Review 113, 234272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cop, U, Dirix, N, Van Assche, E, Drieghe, D and Duyck, W (2017) Reading a book in one or two languages? An eye movement study of cognate facilitation in L1 and L2 reading. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 747769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dambacher, M, Kliegl, R, Hofmann, M and Jacobs, AM (2006) Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading. Brain Research 1084, 89103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S, Naccache, L, Le Clec'H, G, Koechlin, E, Mueller, M, Dehaene-Lambertz, G, van de Moortele, P and Le Bihan, D (1998) Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature 395, 597600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchon, A, Perea, M, Sebastián-Gallés, N, Martí, A and Carreiras, M (2013) EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word properties. Behavior Research Methods 45, 12461258.Google ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, JA, Dimitropoulou, M, Uribe-Etxebarria, O, Laka, I and Carreiras, M (2010a) Electrophysiological correlates of the masked translation priming effect with highly proficient simultaneous bilinguals. Brain Research 1359, 142154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, JA, Perea, M and Carreiras, M (2010b) Masked translation priming effects with highly proficient simultaneous bilinguals. Experimental Psychology 57, 98107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elston-Güttler, KE, Gunter, TC and Kotz, SA (2005) Zooming into L2: Global language context and adjustment affect processing of interlingual homographs in sentences. Cognitive Brain Research 25, 5770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrand, L and Grainger, J (1994) Effects of orthography are independent of phonology in masked form priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 47, 365382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geyer, A, Holcomb, PJ, Midgley, KJ and Grainger, J (2011) Processing words in two languages: An event-related brain potential study of proficient bilinguals. Journal of Neurolinguistics 24, 338351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, TH, Weissberger, G, Runnqvist, E, Montoya, RI and Cera, CM (2012) Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A multi-lingual naming test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15, 594615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J and Holcomb, PJ (2009) Watching the word go by: On the time-course of component processes in visual word recognition. Language and Linguistics Compass 3, 128156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gries, ST (2005) Syntactic Priming: A Corpus-based Approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34, 365399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gries, ST (2011) Studying syntactic priming in corpora: implications of different levels of granularity. In Schönefeld, D (Ed.) Converging evidence: Methodological and theoretical issues for linguistic research (pp. 143165). Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, ST and Kootstra, GJ (2016) Structural priming within and across languages: a corpus-based perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 235250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M, Indefrey, P and Muysken, P (2009) Research techniques for the study of code-switching. In Bullock, BE and Toribio, JA (Eds.) The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Code-Switching (pp. 2139). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, RJ, Bernolet, S, Schoonbaert, S, Speybroeck, S and Vanderelst, D (2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language 58, 214238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, RJ, Pickering, MJ and Veltkamp, E (2004) Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish–English bilinguals. Psychological Science 15, 409414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauk, O, Davis, MH, Ford, M, Pulvermüller, F and Marslen-Wilson, WD (2006) The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. NeuroImage 30, 13831400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holcomb, PJ and Grainger, J (2006) On the Time Course of Visual Word Recognition: An Event-related Potential Investigation using Masked Repetition Priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 16311643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inhoff, AW (1984) Two stages of word processing during eye fixations in the reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23, 612624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inhoff, AW and Radach, R (1998) Definition and computation of oculomotor measures in the study of cognitive processes. In Underwood, G (Ed.) Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 2953). Kidlington, Oxford, United Kingdom: Elsevier Science Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, MA, Valdés Kroff, JR and Dussias, PE (2019) Mixing things up: How blocking and mixing affect the processing of codemixed sentences. International Journal of Bilingualism 23(2), 584611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keuleers, E and Brysbaert, M (2010) Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods 42, 627633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A, Brockhoff, PB and Christensen, RHB (2017) lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software 82, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levshina, N (2015) How to do linguistics with R: Data exploration and statistical analysis. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leys, C, Ley, C, Klein, O, Bernard, P and Licata, L (2013) Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49, 764766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libben, MR and Titone, DA (2009) Bilingual lexical access in context: evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35, 381390.Google ScholarPubMed
Linck, JA, Kroll, JF and Sunderman, G (2009) Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science 20, 15071515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V, Blumenfeld, HK and Kaushanskaya, M (2007) The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP–Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 940967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meade, G, Midgley, KJ and Holcomb, PJ (2018) An ERP Investigation of L2–L1 Translation Priming in Adult Learners. Frontiers in Psychology 9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Misra, M, Guo, T, Bobb, SC and Kroll, JF (2012) When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language 67, 224237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myslín, M and Levy, R (2015) Code-switching and predictability of meaning in discourse. Language 91, 871905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myslín, M and Levy, R (2016) Comprehension priming as rational expectation for repetition: Evidence from syntactic processing. Cognition 147, 2956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neely, JH (2012) Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In Besner, D & Humphreys, GW (Eds.) Basic Processes in Reading (pp. 272344). New York City, New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Neely, JH and Keefe, DE (1989) Semantic context effects on visual word processing: A hybrid prospective-retrospective processing theory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 24, 207248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neely, JH, Keefe, DE and Ross, K (1989) Semantic priming in the lexical decision task: Roles of prospective prime-generated expectancies and retrospective semantic matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15, 10031019.Google ScholarPubMed
Perea, M, Marcet, A, Lozano, A and Gómez, P (2018) Is masked priming modulated by memory load? A test of the automaticity of masked identity priming in lexical decision. Journal of Memory and Cognition 46, 11271135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, MJ and Branigan, HP (1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language 39, 633651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, MJ and Branigan, HP (1999) Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3, 136141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, MJ and Garrod, S (2004) The interactive-alignment model: Developments and refinements. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27, 212225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2019) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL: https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Rayner, K (1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124, 372422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K and Pollatsek, A (1989) The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K, Chace, KH, Slattery, TJ and Ashby, J (2006) Eye movements as reflections of comprehension processes in reading. Scientific Studies of Reading 10, 241255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitter, D, Keller, F and Moore, JD (2011) A computational cognitive model of syntactic priming. Cognitive Science 35, 587637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodd, JM, Curtin, BL, Kirsch, H, Millar, A and Davis, MH (2013) Long-term priming of the meanings of ambiguous words. Journal of Memory and Language 68, 180198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossell, SL, Price, CJ and Nobre, CA (2003) The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs. Neuropsychologia 41, 550564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-Casas, R, Ferré, P, García-Albea, JE and Guasch, M (2006) The nature of semantic priming: Effects of the degree of semantic similarity between primes and targets in Spanish. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 18, 161184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S, Holcomb, PJ, Grainger, J and Hartsuiker, RJ (2011) Testing asymmetries in noncognate translation priming: Evidence from RTs and ERPs. Psychophysiology 48, 7481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, Y, Walters, J and Prior, A (2019) Target accessibility contributes to asymmetric priming in translation and cross-language semantic priming in unbalanced bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22, 157176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D, Libben, M, Mercier, J, Whitford, V and Pivneva, I (2011) Bilingual lexical access during L1 sentence reading: The effects of L2 knowledge, semantic constraint, and L1–L2 intermixing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37, 14121431.Google ScholarPubMed
Torres Cacoullos, R and Travis, , E, C. (2016) Two languages, one effect: Structural priming in spontaneous code-switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19, 733753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Travis, CE, Torres Cacoullos, R and Kidd, E (2017) Cross-language priming: A view from bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 283298.Google Scholar
Traxler, MJ, Williams, RS, Blozis, SE and Morris, RK (2005) Working memory, animacy, and verb class in the processing of relative clauses. Journal of Memory and Language 53, 204224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E, Duyck, W and Hartsuiker, RJ (2012) Bilingual word recognition in a sentence context. Frontiers in Psychology 3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00174.Google Scholar
van Heuven, WJ, Dijkstra, T, Grainger, J and Schriefers, H (2001) Shared neighborhood effects in masked orthographic priming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8, 96101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vergara-Martínez, M, Gómez, P, Jiménez, M and Perea, M (2015) Lexical enhancement during prime-target integration: ERP evidence from matched-case identity priming. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 15, 492504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voeten, C (2019) buildmer: Stepwise Elimination and Term Reordering for Mixed-Effects Regression. R package version 1.3. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=buildmer.Google Scholar
Was, C, Woltz, D and Hirsch, D (2019) Memory processes underlying long-term semantic priming. Memory & Cognition 47, 313325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wen, Y and van Heuven, WJB (2017) Non-cognate translation priming in masked priming lexical decision experiments: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 24, 879886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: PDF

Johns et al. supplementary material

Johns et al. supplementary material 1

Download Johns et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 58.5 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Johns et al. supplementary material

Johns et al. supplementary material 2

Download Johns et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 61.5 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Johns et al. supplementary material

Johns et al. supplementary material 3

Download Johns et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 21 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Johns et al. supplementary material

Johns et al. supplementary material 4

Download Johns et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 20.1 KB