Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:44:03.910Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictors of language proficiency in school-age Spanish–English bilingual children with and without developmental language disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 November 2021

Kerry Danahy Ebert*
Affiliation:
Department of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, University of Minnesota-Twin Cities, Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA
Madeline Reilly
Affiliation:
Department of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, University of Minnesota-Twin Cities, Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Kerry Danahy Ebert, Department of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, 115 Shevlin Hall, 164 Pillsbury Dr. SE, Minneapolis, MN 55455, [email protected]

Abstract

Children with developmental language disorder (DLD) have impairments in their language-learning abilities that may influence interactions with environmental opportunities to learn two languages. This study explores relationships between proficiency in L1 and L2 and a set of environmental and personal variables within a group of school-age Spanish–English bilingual children with DLD and a group of typically-developing peers. Within each group, current usage in the home, length of L2 exposure, gender, maternal education, analytical reasoning, and number of L1 conversational partners were used to predict proficiency in each language. Results showed that home language environment, particularly home L2 usage, strongly predicted L1 proficiency but had less influence on the L2. Female gender predicted L1 skills in both groups, whereas analytical reasoning predicted both L1 and L2 but only for children with DLD. This study expands the limited literature on how children with DLD interact with their environment to learn two languages.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altman, N and Krzywinski, M (2016) Analyzing outliers: Influential or nuisance? Nature Methods 13, 281282. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3812CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreou, G and Lemoni, G (2020) Narrative skills of monolingual and bilingual pre-school and primary school children with developmental language disorder (DLD): A systematic review. Open Journal of Modern Linguistics 10(05), 429458.10.4236/ojml.2020.105026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, LM, Peña, ED, Summers, CL, Boerger, KM, Resendiz, MD, Greene, K, Bohman, TM and Gillam, RB (2012) The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism 15(3), 616629. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000090CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedore, LM, Peña, ED, Griffin, ZM and Hixon, JG (2016) Effects of age of English exposure, current input/output, and grade on bilingual language performance. Journal of Child Language 43(3), 687706.10.1017/S0305000915000811CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, DV, Snowling, MJ, Thompson, PA, Greenhalgh, T, & the CATALISE-2 Consortium. (2017) Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 58(10), 10681080. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12721CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E and Paradis, J (2015) Sources of individual differences in the acquisition of tense inflection by English second language learners with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 36(4), 953976. https://doi:10.1017/S014271641300057XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohman, TM, Bedore, LM, Peña, ED, Mendez-Perez, A and Gillam, RB (2010) What you hear and what you say: Language performance in Spanish–English bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 13, 325344. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050903342019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, L, Sherbenou, RJ and Johnsen, SK (2010) Test of Nonverbal Intelligence–Fourth Edition (TONI-4). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G and Durkin, K (2007) Phonological short-term memory, language and literacy: Developmental relationships in early adolescence in young people with SLI. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48(2), 147156. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01703.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Cat, C (2021) Socioeconomic status as a proxy for input quality in bilingual children?. Applied Psycholinguistics 42(2), 301324. doi: 10.1017/S014271642000079XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duursma, E, Romero-Contreras, S, Szuber, A, Proctor, P, Snow, C, August, D and Calderón, M (2007) The role of home literacy and language environment on bilinguals' English and Spanish vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics 28(1), 171-190. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0142716406070093CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebert, KD, Rak, D, Slawny, CM and Fogg, L (2019) Attention in bilingual children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62(4), 979992.10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics (2019) America's children: Key national indicators of well-being, 2019. U.S. Government Printing Office. https://www.childstats.gov/pdf/ac2019/ac_19.pdfGoogle Scholar
Gollan, TH, Starr, J and Ferreira, VS (2015) More than use it or lose it: The number-of-speakers effect on heritage language proficiency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22(1), 147155.10.3758/s13423-014-0649-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Govindarajan, K and Paradis, J (2019) Narrative abilities of bilingual children with and without developmental language disorder (SLI): Differentiation and the role of age and input factors. Journal of Communication Disorders 77, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2018.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, CS, Lawrence, F, Rodriguez, B, Davison, MD and Miccio, AW (2011) Changes in language usage of Puerto Rican mothers and their children: Do gender and timing of exposure to English matter? Applied Psycholinguistics 32(2), 275.10.1017/S014271641000041XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, CS, Komaroff, E, Rodriguez, BL, Lopez, LM, Scarpino, SE and Goldstein, B (2012) Predicting Spanish–English bilingual children's language abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 55(5), 12511264. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0016)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E and Core, C (2013) Input and language development in bilingually developing children, Seminars in Speech and Language 34, 215226.Google ScholarPubMed
Kan, PF, Miller, A, Cheung, S and Brickman, A (2020) The distributed L1 and L2 language-learning environments of dual language learners across home and school settings. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 51(4), 10071023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohnert, K, Ebert, KD and Pham, GT (2021) Language disorders in bilingual children and adults (3rd Ed). Plural Publishing.Google Scholar
Lauro, J, Core, C and Hoff, E (2020) Explaining individual differences in trajectories of simultaneous bilingual development: Contributions of child and environmental factors. Child Development 91(6), 20632082.10.1111/cdev.13409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, LB, Ellis Weismer, S, Miller, CA, Francis, DJ, Tomblin, JB and Kail, RV (2007) Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50(2), 408-428. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/029)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, JW, Evans, JL, Fargo, JD, Schwartz, S and Gillam, RB (2019) Structural relationship between cognitive processing and syntactic sentence comprehension in children with and without developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 61(12), 29502976. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norbury, CF, Gooch, D, Wray, C, Baird, G, Charman, T, Simonoff, E, Vamvakas, G and Pickles, A (2016) The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57(11), 12471257. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12573CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J (2010) Alberta language environment questionnaire. University of Alberta Child English as a Second Language Resource Centre. https://www.ualberta.ca/linguistics/media-library/chesl/documents/aleq-26mar2010.pdfGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J (2011) Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1, 213237.10.1075/lab.1.3.01parCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J, Emmerzael, K and Sorenson Duncan, T (2010) Assessment of English language learners: Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication Disorders 43(6), 474497.10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J and Jia, R (2017) Bilingual children's long-term outcomes in English as a second language: Language environment factors shape individual differences in catching up with monolinguals. Developmental Science 20(1), e12433. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peña, ED, Bedore, LM, Lugo-Neris, MJ and Albudoor, N (2020) Identifying developmental language disorder in school age bilinguals: Semantics, grammar, and narratives. Language Assessment Quarterly, 118.Google Scholar
Pham, G and Tipton, T (2018) Internal and external factors that support children's minority first language and English. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 49(3), 595606. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Portes, A and Hao, L (2002) The price of uniformity: Language, family and personality adjustment in the immigrant second generation. Ethnic and Racial Studies 25(6), 889912.10.1080/0141987022000009368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prevoo, MJ, Malda, M, Mesman, J, Emmen, RA, Yeniad, N, van Ijzendoorn, MH and Linting, M (2014) Predicting ethnic minority children's vocabulary from socioeconomic status, maternal language and home reading input: Different pathways for host and ethnic language. Journal of Child Language 41(5), 963984. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000299CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rojas, R and Iglesias, A (2013) The language growth of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Child Development 84(2), 630646.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01871.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rojas, R, Iglesias, A, Bunta, F, Goldstein, B, Goldenberg, C and Reese, L (2016) Interlocutor differential effects on the expressive language skills of Spanish-speaking English learners. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 18(2), 166177. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2015.1081290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheele, AF, Leseman, PP and Mayo, AY (2010) The home language environment of monolingual and bilingual children and their language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics 31(1), 117140. doi:10.1017/S0142716409990191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semel, E, Wiig, EH and Secord, WA (2003) Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th edn.). The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Smolander, S, Laasonen, M, Arkkila, E, Lahti-Nuuttila, P and Kunnari, S (2021) L2 vocabulary acquisition of early sequentially bilingual children with TD and DLD affected differently by exposure and age of onset. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 56, 7289. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorenson Duncan, T and Paradis, J (2020) How does maternal education influence the linguistic environment supporting bilingual language development in child second language learners of English?. International Journal of Bilingualism 24(1), 46-61. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006918768366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, JB, Records, NL, Buckwalter, P, Zhang, X, Smith, E and O'Brien, M (1997) Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 40(6), 12451260.10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unsworth, S (2016) Quantity and quality of language input in bilingual language development. In Nicoladis, E and Montanari, S (eds), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Factors moderating language proficiency pp. 103121.Google Scholar
Wiig, EH, Secord, WA and Semel, E (2006) Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed., Spanish). The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar