Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T05:17:12.613Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonetic equivalence in the acquisition of /l/ by Spanish–English bilingual children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 June 2012

JESSICA A. BARLOW*
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
PAIGE E. BRANSON
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
IGNATIUS S. B. NIP
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
*
Address for correspondence: Jessica A. Barlow, School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, San Diego, CA 92812-1518, USA[email protected]

Abstract

Spanish [l] is characterized as clear, and is associated with a high second formant (F2) frequency and a large difference between F2 and the first formant (F1) frequencies. In contrast, English [l] is darker (with a lower F2 and a relatively smaller F2–F1 difference) and also exhibits contextual variation due to an allophonic velarization rule that further darkens [l] postvocalically. We aimed to determine if Spanish–English bilingual children evidence these differences productively, in a manner comparable to that of monolinguals, or if they produce an [l] that is intermediate to that of Spanish and English monolinguals. We acoustically analyzed [l] productions of seven Spanish–English bilingual, seven Spanish monolingual, and seven English monolingual children. Results showed that the bilinguals had similar prevocalic F2 and F2–F1 values for [l] in both languages, comparable to those of Spanish monolinguals, but significantly higher than those of English monolinguals. The bilinguals also produced English (but not Spanish) [l] with significantly lower postvocalic F2 and F2–F1 values. We assume that the bilinguals have a merged phonetic category for prevocalic [l] but not postvocalic [l], and further, that they maintain separate grammars, allowing the allophonic velarization rule to apply in English but not Spanish.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Aspects of this research were supported in part by a grant to the first author from the American Speech-Language-Hearing Foundation. Results presented herein were part of a larger study detailed in the second author's undergraduate honors thesis (Branson, 2011). Thanks to the following individuals who provided assistance with aspects of this research: Peter Torre III, Leah Fabiano-Smith, Sonja Pruitt-Lord, Henrike Blumenfeld, as well as past and present members of the Phonological Typologies Lab at San Diego State University, especially Alanna Call, Leah Lubetkin, Mirna Enriquez, Adam Jacobson, and Jacqueline Mendez. Thanks also to three anonymous reviewers who provided very helpful feedback and suggestions.

References

Alameda, J. R., & Cuetos, F. (1995). Diccionario de frecuencias de las unidades lingüísticas del castellano. Oviedo, Spain: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad de Oviedo.Google Scholar
Alarcos Llorach, E. (1991). Fonología española. Madrid: Biblioteca Románica Hispánica.Google Scholar
Anderson, R., & Smith, B. L. (1987). Phonological development of two-year-old monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 14, 5778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Azambuja, E. J. (1998). A aquisição das líquidas laterais do português. MA thesis, Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Brazil.Google Scholar
Baertsch, K. S., & Davis, S. (2003). The split margin approach to syllable structure. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 32, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, W., & Trofimovich, P. (2005). Interaction of native- and second-language vowel system(s) in early and late bilinguals. Language and Speech, 48, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baković, E. (1998). Spanish codas and overapplication. In Schwegler, A., Tranel, B. & Uribe-Etxebarria, M. (eds.), Romance linguistics: Theoretical perspectives, pp. 1323. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, J. A. (2002). Error patterns and transfer in Spanish–English bilingual phonological development. Boston University Conference on Language Development, 26, 6071.Google Scholar
Barlow, J. A. (2003a). Assessment of English phonology. San Diego, CA: Phonological Typologies Laboratory, School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University.Google Scholar
Barlow, J. A. (2003b). Assessment of Spanish phonology – revised. San Diego, CA: Phonological Typologies Laboratory, School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University.Google Scholar
Barlow, J. A. (2003c). The stop–spirant alternation in Spanish: Converging evidence for a fortition account. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 22, 5186.Google Scholar
Barlow, J. A. (2007). Grandfather effects: A longitudinal case study of the phonological acquisition of intervocalic consonants in English. Language Acquisition, 14, 121164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. M. (1999). The acquisition of Spanish. In Taylor, O. L. & Leonard, L. B. (eds.), Language acquisition across North America: Cross-cultural and cross-linguistic perspectives, pp. 157208. San Diego, CA: Singular.Google Scholar
Bernhardt, B., & Stemberger, J. P. (2002). Intervocalic consonants in the speech of English-speaking Canadian children with phonological disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 16, 199214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, C. (1995). A direct realist perspective on cross-language speech perception. In Strange, W. (ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience, pp. 171206. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Blevins, J. (1995). The syllable in phonological theory. In Goldsmith, J. A. (ed.), The handbook of phonological theory, pp. 206244. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2008). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.0.26) [Computer program]. http://www.praat.org (retrieved June 16, 2008).Google Scholar
Borowsky, T. (1986). Topics in the lexical phonology of English. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech, 46, 217243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branson, P. E. (2011). Acoustic properties of /l/ as produced by English monolingual, Spanish monolingual, and Spanish–English bilingual children: A retrospective study. Undergraduate honors thesis, San Diego State University.Google Scholar
Brown, C. A. (1998). The role of the L1 grammar in the L2 acquisition of segmental structure. Second Language Research, 14, 136193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullock, B. E., Toribio, A. J., Davis, K. A., & Botero, C. G. (2004). Phonetic convergence in bilingual Puerto Rican Spanish. Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 23, 113125.Google Scholar
Carreira, M. M. (1996). Spanish clusters: Coronals, /s/, and syllable structure conditions. In Parodi, C., Quicoli, C., Saltarelli, M. & Zubizarreta, M. L. (eds.), Aspects of Romance linguistics: Selected papers from the Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages XXIV, pp. 123134. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Carterette, E. C., & Jones, M. H. (1975). Informal speech: Alphabetic and phonemic texts with statistical analysis and tables. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Cataño, L., Barlow, J. A., & Moyna, M. I. (2009). Phonetic inventory complexity in the phonological acquisition of Spanish: A retrospective, typological study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 23, 446472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N., & Keyser, S. J. (1983). CV phonology: A generative theory of the syllable. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Colina, S. (1997). Identity constraints and Spanish resyllabification. Lingua, 103, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, V. J. (2002). Background to the L2 user. In Cook, V. J. (ed.), Portraits of the L2 user, pp. 128. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M., & Clark, A. (1996). Early bilingual acquisition of the voicing contrast in English and Spanish. Journal of Phonetics, 24, 351365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A. (1992). Variation in developing and fully developed phonologies. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications, pp. 191210. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A., Chin, S. B., Elbert, M., & Powell, T. W. (1990). Some constraints on functionally disordered phonologies: Phonetic inventories and phonotactics. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Döpke, S. (2000). Generation of and retraction from cross-linguistically motivated structures in bilingual first language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 209226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckman, F. R., & Iverson, G. K. (1997). Structure preservation in interlanguage phonology. In Hannahs, S. J. & Young-Scholten, M. (eds.), Focus on phonological acquisition, pp. 183208. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Escudero, P. (2007). Second language phonology: The role of perception. In Pennington, M. C. (ed.), Phonology in context, pp. 109134. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 551585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L. [C.], & Barlow, J. A. (2010). Interaction in bilingual phonological acquisition: Evidence from phonetic inventories. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. [C.], & Bunta, F. (2012). Voice onset time of voiceless bilabial and velar stops in 3-year-old bilingual children and their age-matched monolingual peers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26, 148163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano, L. C., & Goldstein, B. A. (2005). Phonological cross-linguistic effects in bilingual Spanish–English speaking children. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3, 5663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L. [C.], & Goldstein, B. A. (2010a). Early-, middle-, and late-developing sounds in monolingual and bilingual children: An exploratory investigation. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 6677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. [C.], & Goldstein, B. A. (2010b). Phonological acquisition in bilingual Spanish–English speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 160178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantini, A. (1985). Language acquisition of a bilingual child: A sociolinguistic perspective. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1980). Phonetic approximation in second language acquisition. Language Learning, 30, 117133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1987). The production of ‘new’ and ‘similar’ phones in a foreign language: Evidence for the effect of equivalence classification. Journal of Phonetics, 15, 4765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1991). Age of learning affects the authenticity of voice onset time (VOT) in stop consonants produced in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 395411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings and problems. In Strange, W. (ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and methodological issues, pp. 233277. Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (2007). Language contact in bilingualism: Phonetic system interactions. In Cole, J. & Hualde, J. I. (eds.), Laboratory Phonology, 9, pp. 353382. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., & MacKay, I. R. A. (1995). Effects of age of second-language learning on the production of English consonants. Speech Communication, 16, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Port, R. (1981). Cross-language phonetic interference: Arabic to English. Language and Speech, 24, 125146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Schirru, C., & MacKay, I. R. A. (2003). Interaction between the native and second language phonetic subsystems. Speech Communication, 40, 467491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, A. V., & Dodd, B. (2001). Phonologically disordered German-speaking children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 291307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frazer, T. C. (1996). Chicano English and Spanish interference in the Midwestern United States. American Speech, 71, 7285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawlitzek-Maiwald, I., & Tracy, R. (1996). Bilingual bootstrapping. Linguistics, 34, 901926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F. (1989). Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 16, 161179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J. (1995). Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22, 611631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F., Paradis, J., & Crago, M. B. (2004). Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Gierut, J. A. (2007). Phonological complexity and language learnability. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. E., & Wright, K. L. (2010). English speech acquisition in 3- to 5-year-old children learning Russian and English. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 429444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goad, H., & Brannen, K. (2000). Syllabification at the right edge of words: Parallels between child and adult grammars. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics, 15, 126.Google Scholar
Göksel, A., & Kerslake, C. (2005). Turkish: A comprehensive grammar. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B. [A.], & Washington, P. S. (2001). An initial investigation of phonological patterns in typically developing 4-year-old Spanish–English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. A. (ed.) (2004). Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish–English speakers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B. A., Fabiano, L. [C.], & Washington, P. S. (2005). Phonological skills in predominantly English-speaking, predominantly Spanish-speaking, and Spanish–English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 201218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. A., & Iglesias, A. (1996). Phonological patterns in normally developing Spanish-speaking 3- and 4-year-olds of Puerto Rican descent. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 8290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, J. R., & Nip, I. S. B. (2010). Some organization principles in early speech development. In Maassen, B. & Van Lieshout, P. H. H. M. (eds.), Speech motor control: New developments in basic and applied research, pp. 171188. Cary, NC: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Kreiter, J. (2003). Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 267288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamers, J. F., & Blanc, M. H. A. (2000). Bilinguality and bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. W. (1969). Spanish phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Harris, J. W. (1983). Syllable structure and stress in Spanish. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Harris, J. W. (1993). Integrity of prosodic constituents and the domain of syllabification rules in Spanish and Catalan. In Hale, K. & Keyser, S. J. (eds.), The view from Building 20: Essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger, pp. 177193. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. P. (2000). Gradient well-formedness in Optimality Theory. In Dekkers, J., van der Leeuw, F. & van de Weijer, J. (eds.), Optimality Theory: Phonology, syntax, and acquisition, pp. 88120. New York: Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, B. F., & Mulford, R. (1982). The acquisition of a second language phonology: Interaction of transfer and developmental factors. Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 313328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandorena, C. L. M. (1990). A aquisição da fonologia do português: Estabelecimento de padrões com base em traços distintivos. Ph.D. dissertation, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39 (1), 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huffman, M. K. (1997). Phonetic variation in intervocalic onset /l/'s in English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 115141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiménez, B. C. (1987). Acquisition of Spanish consonants in children aged 3–5:7. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 18, 357363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, C. E., & Wilson, I. L. (2002). Phonetic evidence for early language differentiation: Research issues and some preliminary data. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 271289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, M. J. (2004, June 17). Summary: Interdental /l/ part 1. Message posted to http://linguistlist.org/issues/15/15-1836.html.Google Scholar
Kaye, J. (1990). ‘Coda’ licensing. Phonology, 7, 301330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. [M.] (2002). Developing vowel systems as a window to bilingual phonology. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 315334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2004). Voice onset time in bilingual German–Spanish children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 7188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. M., Trujillo, C., & Lleó, C. (2001). Bilingual phonological acquisition: An analysis of syllable structure and VOT. In Cantone, K. F. & Hinzelin, M.-O. (eds.), Proceedings of the Colloquium on Structure, Acquisition, and Change of Grammars: Phonological and Syntactic Aspects, pp. 3854. Hamburg: Universität Hamburg.Google Scholar
Kenstowicz, M. J. (1994). Phonology in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Khattab, G. (2002). /l/ production in English–Arabic bilingual speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 335353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, R. D. (1967). Functional load and sound change. Language, 43, 831852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a “perceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 50, 93107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladefoged, P. (2000). A course in phonetics. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Lado, R. (1957). Linguistics across cultures. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Lee, S. A. S., & Iverson, G. K. (2012). Stop consonant productions of Korean–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 275287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1964). Some acoustic characteristics of selected English consonants. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Research Center in Anthropology, Folklore, and Linguistics.Google Scholar
Levi, S. V. (2001). Glides, laterals, and Turkish vowel harmony. Chicago Linguistic Society, 37, 379393.Google Scholar
Lleó, C. (2002). The role of markedness in the acquisition of complex prosodic structures by German–Spanish bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 291313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C. (2003). Prosodic licensing of codas in the acquisition of Spanish. Probus, 15, 257281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., Kuchenbrandt, M., Kehoe, M., & Trujillo, C. (2003). Syllable final consonants in Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In Müller, N. (ed.), (In)vulnerable domains in multilingualism, pp. 191220. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Llisterri, J., & Martínez-Daudén, G. (1991). Laternal consonant production in bilingual speakers learning a third language. Actes du XIIème Congrès International des Sciences Phonétiques, pp. 318321. Aix-en-Provence: Université de Provence, Service des Publications.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mack, M. (1982). Voicing-dependent vowel duration in English and French: Monolingual and bilingual production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71, 173178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, M. (1984). Early bilinguals: How monolingual-like are they? Neurolinguistics, 13, 161173.Google Scholar
MacLeod, A. A. N., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2010). What is the impact of age of second language acquisition on the production of consonants and vowels among childhood bilinguals? International Journal of Bilingualism, 14, 400421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2004). A unified model of language acquisition. In Kroll, J. F. & De Groot, A. M. B. (eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches, pp. 4967. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Major, R. C. (2001). Foreign accent. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinet, A. (1955). Économie des changements phonétiques. Bern: Francke.Google Scholar
McDonald, M. G. (1989). The influence of Spanish phonology on the English spoken by United States Hispanics. In Bjarkman, P. C. & Hammond, R. M. (eds.), American Spanish pronunciation: Theoretical and applied perspectives, pp. 215236. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1978). Second-language acquisition in childhood. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (1989). Early differentiation of languages in bilingual children. In Hyltenstam, K. & Obler, L. K. (eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Aspects of acquisition, maturity and loss, pp. 1340. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2001). The simultaneous acquisition of two languages: Early differentiation and subsequent development of grammars. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F. (eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition, pp. 1142. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menke, M. (2010). Examination of the Spanish vowels produced by Spanish–English bilingual children. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 28, 98135.Google Scholar
Mines, M., Hanson, B., & Shoup, J. (1978). Frequency of occurrence of phonemes in conversational English. Language and Speech, 21, 221241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuñez-Cedeño, R. (2007). The acquisition of Spanish codas: A frequency/sonority approach. Hispania, 90, 147163.Google Scholar
Oh, S. (2002). Cross-language blending of /l/ gestures by Korean–English bilingual children. Boston University Conference on Language Development, 26, 473484.Google Scholar
Padilla, A. M., & Liebman, E. (1975). Language acquisition in the bilingual child. Bilingual Review, 2, 3455.Google Scholar
Padilla, A. [M.], & Lindholm, K. (1984). Child bilingualism: The same old issues revisited. In Martinez, J. & Mendoza, R. (eds.), Chicano psychology, pp. 369408. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, C., & Prunet, J. F. (eds.) (1991). Phonetics and phonology (vol. 2): The special status of coronals: Internal and external evidence. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2001). Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems? International Journal of Bilingualism, 5, 1938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B., Fernandez, S., Ledeweg, V., & Oller, K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 4158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Rappazzo, C. (2003). Comparison of Spanish, English, and bilingual children's performance across semantic tasks. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1995). Language acquisition. In Gleitman, L. R. & Liberman, M. (eds.), An invitation to cognitive science (vol. 1): Language, pp. 135182. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Port, R. F., & Mitleb, F. M. (1983). Segmental features and implementation of English by Arabic speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 219229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, M. (2010). Gestural characterization of a phonological class: The liquids. Ph.D. dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Pye, C., Ingram, D., & List, H. (1987). A comparison of initial consonant acquisition in English and Quiché. In Nelson, K. E. & Van Kleeck, A. (eds.), Children's language, pp. 175190. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Quilis, A., Esgueva, M., Gutiérrez Araus, M. L., & Cantarero, M. (1979). Características acústicas de las consonantes laterales españoles. Lingüística Española Actual, 1, 233343.Google Scholar
Quilis, A., & Esgueva Martínez, M. A. (1980). Frecuencia de fonemas en el español hablado. Lingüística Española Actual, 2, 125.Google Scholar
Rangel, G. A. (1998). Uma análise auto-segmental da fonologia normal: Estudo longitudinal de 3 crianças de 1:6 a 3:0. MA thesis, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre.Google Scholar
Recasens, D. (2004). Darkness in [l] as a scalar phonetic property: Implications for phonology and articulatory control. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18, 593603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Recasens, D., & Espinosa, A. (2005). Articulatory, positional and coarticulatory characteristics for clear /l/ and dark /l/: Evidence from two Catalan dialects. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 35, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo, M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 13981411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riney, T. J., & Okamura, K. (1999). Does bilingualism affect the first language? ICU Language Research Bulletin, 14, 101113.Google Scholar
Roelofs, A., & Verhoef, K. (2006). Modeling the control of phonological encoding in bilingual speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 167176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rojo, G. (1991). Frecuencia de fonemas en el español actual. In Brea, M., & Fernández Rei, F. (eds.), Homenaxe ó profesor Constantino García, pp. 451467. Santiago de Compostela: Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Servicio de Publicación e Intercambio Científico.Google Scholar
Romaine, S. (1992). Bilingualism. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Santa Ana, O. (1996). Sonority and syllable structure in Chicano English. Language Variation and Change, 8, 6389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnitzer, M. L., & Krasinski, E. (1994). The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child. Journal of Child Language, 21, 565584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selkirk, E. O. (1982). The syllable. In van der Hulst, H. & Smith, N. (eds.), The structure of phonological representations, pp. 337383. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Shea, C. E., & Curtin, S. (2011). Experience, representations and the production of second language allophones. Second Language Research, 27 (2), 229250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shriberg, L., Austin, D., Lewis, B. A., McSweeny, J., & Wilson, D. (1997). The speech disorders classification systems (SDCS): Extensions and lifespan reference data. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 723740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonet, M. (2010). Dark and clear laterals in Catalan and Spanish: Interaction of phonetic categories in early bilinguals. Journal of Phonetics, 38, 663678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smit, A. B. (1993). Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa–Nebraska Articulation Norms Project: Consonant singletons. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 533547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
So, L. K. H., & Leung, C. S. S. (2006). Phonological development of Cantonese–Putonghua bilingual children. In Hua, Z. & Dodd, B. (eds.), Phonological development and disorders in children: A multilingual perspective, pp. 413427. Buffalo, NY: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stemberger, J. P. (1996). Syllable structure in English, with emphasis on codas. In Bernhardt, B., Gilbert, J. & Ingram, D. (eds.), Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition, pp. 6275. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Baum, S. (2006). Production of coronal stops by simultaneous bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 97114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornburgh, D. F., & Ryalls, J. H. (1998). Voice onset time in Spanish–English bilinguals: Early versus late learners of English. Journal of Communication Disorders, 31, 215229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Topbaş, S. (2004/2005). Türkçe Sesletim-Sesbilgisi Testi [Turkish Articulation and Phonology Test]. Ankara: Milli Eğitim Yayınevi 4. Akşam Sanat Okulu.Google Scholar
Topbaş, S. (2006). Does the speech of Turkish-speaking phonologically disordered children differ from that of children speaking other languages? Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 20, 509522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhoeven, L. (1994). Transfer in bilingual development: The linguistic interdependence hypothesis revisited. Language Learning, 44, 381415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vokic, G. (2010). L1 allophones in L2 speech production: The case of English learners of Spanish. Hispania, 93, 430452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vorperian, H. K., Kent, R. D., Lindstrom, M. J., Kalina, C. M., Gentry, L. R., & Yandell, B. S. (2005). Development of vocal tract length during early childhood: A magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117, 338350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vorperian, H. K., Wang, S., Chung, M. K., Schimek, E. M., Durtschi, R. B., Kent, R. D., Ziegert, A. J., & Gentry, L. R. (2009). Anatomic development of the oral and pharyngeal portions of the vocal tract: an imaging study. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 16661678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, I. (1991). Phonological processing in two languages. In Bialystok, E. (ed.), Language processing in bilingual children, pp. 2548. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, I. (2007). Simultaneous bilinguals and Flege's speech learning model. In Trouvain, J. & Barry, W. J. (eds.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 2007), pp. 15331536. Saarbrücken: Pirrot. [http://www.icphs2007.de/conference/Papers/1239/1239.pdf, retrieved June 3, 2011].Google Scholar
Weinreich, U. (1953). Languages in contact. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1982). A principle theory for language acquisition. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art, pp. 288315. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (1984). Proportional occurrences of allophones in the Puerto Rican dialect of Spanish. Master's thesis, Temple University.Google Scholar
Yavaş, M. S., & Goldstein, B. A. (1998). Phonological assessment and treatment of bilingual speakers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 4960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yavaş, M. S., & Goldstein, B. A. (2006). Aspects of bilingual phonology: The case of Spanish–English bilingual children. In Hua, Z. & Dodd, B. (eds.), Phonological development and disorders in children: A multilingual perspective, pp. 265285. Buffalo, NY: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young-Scholten, M. (2004). Prosodic constraints on allophonic distribution International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 6777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zampini, M. L. (1994). The role of native language transfer and task formality in the acquisition of Spanish spirantization. Hispania, 77, 470481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zampini, M. L. (1996). Voiced stop spirantization in the ESL speech of native speakers of Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 335354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamuner, T. S., Gerken, L., & Hammond, M. (2004). Phonotactic probabilities in young children's production of coda consonants. Journal of Child Language, 31, 515536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar