Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T07:31:30.542Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of socio-economic status on cognitive control in non-literate bilingual speakers*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 September 2016

VISHNU KK NAIR*
Affiliation:
ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders and Discipline of Speech Pathology and Audiology, Flinders University
BRITTA BIEDERMANN
Affiliation:
ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders and School of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Curtin University
LYNDSEY NICKELS
Affiliation:
ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders and Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University
*
Address for correspondence: Vishnu KK Nair, Discipline of Speech Pathology and Audiology, Level 7, Flinders Medical Centre, Flinders University, SA 5001, Australia. [email protected]

Abstract

Previous research has suggested that the advantages for cognitive control abilities in bilinguals are attenuated when socio-economic status (SES) is controlled (e.g., Morton & Harper, 2007). This study examined the effect of SES on cognitive control in illiterate monolingual and bilingual individuals who lived in adverse social conditions. We tested monolinguals and bilinguals using Simon and Attentional Network task while controlling for two potential confounding factors: SES and literacy. Bilinguals were faster for both trials with and without conflict demonstrating overall faster response times (global advantage) compared to monolinguals on both tasks. However, no bilingual advantage was found for conflict resolution on the Simon task and attentional networks on the Attentional Network task. The overall bilingual effects provide evidence for a bilingual advantage even among individuals without literacy skills and of very low SES. This indicates a strong link between bilingualism and cognitive control over and above effects of SES.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

During the preparation of this paper, Vishnu KK Nair was supported by an International Macquarie University Research Excellence Scholarship (iMQRES - 2011105), ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University. Lyndsey Nickels was supported by an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship (FT 120100102), and Britta Biedermann by an Australian Research Council Postdoctoral Fellowship and Discovery Project (DP 11010079). We would like to thank Albert Costa and Mireia Hernández for their help with experiments, Jeff Mathew and staff members of Marthoma College of Special Education, Kasargod, Kerala for their assistance in data collection, and two anonymous reviewers for constructive comments on an earlier draft.

References

Abutalebi, J., & Clahsen, H. (2015). Bilingualism, cognition, and aging. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (01), 12.Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Parris, B. A., & Weekes, B. S. (2015). Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia, 69, 201210.Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20 (3), 242275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashaie, S., & Obler, L. (2014). Effect of age, education, and bilingualism on confrontation naming in older illiterate and low-educated populations. Behavioural Neurology, 2014.Google Scholar
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 (05), 857894.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Claiming evidence from non-evidence: A reply to Morton and Harper. Developmental Science, 12 (4), 499501.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a model for bilingualism: Commentary on Baum and Titone. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 (05), 899902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19 (2), 290.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 859.Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29 (3), 696.Google Scholar
Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51 (1), 5.Google Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130 (3), 278288.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11 (2), 282298.Google Scholar
Chengappa, S., Shivashankar, N., Nair, V. K., Nayak, D., & Arvind, H. (2011). Test of Language Proficiency. Language and Brain Organization in Normative Multilingualism. Indian Department of Science and Technology (DST).Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106 (1), 5986.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113 (2), 135149.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23 (11), 13641371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., & Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330 (6009), 13591364.Google Scholar
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16 (1), 143149.Google Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14 (3), 340347.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35 (1), 116124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2014). Bilingual children show advantages in nonverbal auditory executive function task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18 (6), 717729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 (02), 6781.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 515530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18 (4), 625658.Google Scholar
Huettig, F., & Mishra, R. K. (2014). How literacy acquisition affects the illiterate mind–a critical examination of theories and evidence. Language and Linguistics Compass, 8 (10), 401427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23 (1), 70.Google Scholar
Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2 (2), 174207.Google Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 605621.Google Scholar
Martin, M.M., & Bialystok, E. (2003). The development of two kinds of inhibition in monolingual and bilingual children: Simon vs. Stroop. Poster presented at the meeting of the Cognitive Development Society, Park City, Utah.Google Scholar
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75 (5), 13731386.Google Scholar
Mindt, M. R., Arentoft, A., Germano, K. K., D'Aquila, E., Scheiner, D., Pizzirusso, M., & Gollan, T. H. (2008). Neuropsychological, cognitive, and theoretical considerations for evaluation of bilingual individuals. Neuropsychology Review, 18 (3), 255268.Google Scholar
Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Pandey, A., & Huettig, F. (2012). Spoken language-mediated anticipatory eye movements are modulated by reading ability: Evidence from Indian low and high literates. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5 (1), 110.Google Scholar
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10 (6), 719726.Google Scholar
Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8 (1), 7487.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66 (2), 232258.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32 (1), 325.Google Scholar
Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (02), 253262.Google Scholar
Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M. (2010). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 1 (15), 2637.Google Scholar
Simon, J. R., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the ‘Stroop effect'vs. the ‘Simon effect’. Acta Psychologica, 73 (2), 159170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, J. R., & Small, A. M. Jr (1969). Processing auditory information: Interference from an irrelevant cue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53 (5), 433.Google Scholar
Simon, J. R., & Wolf, J. D. (1963). Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6 (1), 99105.Google Scholar
Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28 (6), 1345.Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems. Cognitive Theory, 3, 200239.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (01), 324.Google Scholar
Venkatesan, S. (2011). Socio Economic Status Scale: NIMH revised version. AIISH, Mysore.Google Scholar