Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T20:22:56.309Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi–English bilinguals*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 March 2012

NIHARIKA SINGH
Affiliation:
Centre of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences, Allahabad University, Allahabad, India
RAMESH KUMAR MISHRA*
Affiliation:
Centre of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences, Allahabad University, Allahabad, India
*
Address for correspondence: Ramesh Kumar Mishra, Centre of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences (CBCS), University of Allahabad, Allahabad 211002, UPIndia[email protected]

Abstract

Though many previous studies have reported enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals, few have investigated if such control is modulated by language proficiency. Here, we examined the inhibitory control of high and low proficient Hindi–English bilinguals on an oculomotor Stroop task. Subjects were asked to make a saccade as fast as possible towards the appropriate colour patch among competitors and distractors suppressing an eye movement evoked by the meaning of the word. High proficient bilinguals quickly oriented their attention towards the correct colour patch while effectively controlling the Stroop interference compared with low proficient subjects, on both colour and direction words. High proficient bilinguals also had fewer saccadic errors and demonstrated overall faster saccadic latency on all trial types. The results provide strong evidence for enhanced oculomotor control in proficient bilinguals compared with the less proficient ones.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Niharika Singh was supported by a Cognitive Science Initiative grant awarded to Ramesh Mishra by the Department of Science and Technology, Government of India. We thank the Editor and both reviewers, whose constructive comments have helped improve the manuscript in many ways.

References

Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 242275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2008). Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80, 207245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2009). Discourse-mediation of the mapping between language and visual world: Eye movements and mental representation. Cognition, 111, 5571.Google Scholar
Anderson, T. J., Jenkins, I. H., Brooks, D. J., Hawken, M. B., Frackowiak, R. S., & Kennard, C. (1994). Cortical control of saccades and fixation in man: A PET study. Brain, 117, 10731084.Google Scholar
Balkenuis, C., & Johansson, B. (2007). Anticipatory models in gaze control: A developmental model. Cognitive Processing, 8, 167174.Google Scholar
Barnes, G. R., & Donelan, S. F. (1999). The remembered pursuit task: Evidence for segregation of timing and velocity storage in predictive oculomotor saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 129, 5767.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636644.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilinguals on cognitive control in Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24, 1049.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859873.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified anti-saccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 13411354.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109, 93100.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied Psycholinguistisc, 19, 6985.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325339.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan, the rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103119.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112, 494500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2010). Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118, 245257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron, 39, 713726.Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., Wildenberg, W. V. D., & Paolier, D. (2008). How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 302312.Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., Linenweber, M. R., Petersen, S. E., Raichle, M. E., Van Essen, D. C., & Shulman, G. L. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron, 21, 761773.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113, 135149.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 5986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2009). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19, 12011206.Google Scholar
Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. F. (2010). Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioural and Brain Functions, 6 (5), 12 pages. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-6-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodríguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Ávila, C. (2010). Bridging language and attention: Brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. Neuroimage, 53, 12721278.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (2011). Bilingual worlds. In Cook, V. & Bassetti, B. (eds.), Language and bilingual cognition, pp. 229240. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hallett, P. E. (1978). Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instruction. Vision Research, 18, 12791296.Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M. (1992). Visual attention and eye movement control during reading and picture viewing. In Rayner, K. (ed.), Eye movements and visual cognition, pp. 126283. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L. J., Vivas, A. B., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2010). The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 315325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodgson, T. L., Parris, B. A, Gregory, N. J., & Jarvis, T. (2009). The saccadic Stroop effect: Evidence for involuntary programming of eye movements by linguistic cues. Vision Research, 49, 569574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 787795.Google Scholar
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kao, G. W., & Morrow, M. J. (1994). The relationship of anticipatory smooth eye movement to smooth pursuit initiation. Vision Research, 34, 30273036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovács, Á. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 65566560.Google Scholar
Kowler, E. (1990). The role of visual and cognitive processes in the control of eye movement. In Kowler, E. (ed.), Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes, pp.170. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Law, I., Svarer, C., Holm, S., & Paulson, O. B. (1997). The activation pattern in normal humans during suppression, imagination and performance of saccadic eye movements. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 161, 419434CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LinckJ, A. J, A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Loosing access to native language while whole immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second language learning. Psychological Science, 20, 15071515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E. (2011). Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 588595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114 (1), 2941.Google Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 8193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirman Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 475494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R. K. (2009). Interaction of language and visual attention: Evidence from production and comprehension. Progress in Brain Research, 176, 277292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mishra, R. K., & Marmolejo-Ramos, F. (2010). On the mental representations originating during the interaction between language and vision. Cognitive Processing, 11, 295305.Google Scholar
Munoz, D. P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look away: The anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye-movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 218228.Google Scholar
Namazi, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 597616.Google Scholar
Raemaekers, M., Jansma, J. M., Cahn, W., Van der Geest, J. N., van der Linden, J. A., Kahn, R. S., & Ramsey, N. F. (2002). Neuronal substrate of the saccadic inhibition deficit in schizophrenia investigated with 3-dimensional event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study using tract-based spatial statistics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 313320.Google Scholar
Ray, S., Schall, J. D., & Murthy, A. (2004). Programming of double-step saccade sequences: Modulation by cognitive control. Vision Research, 44, 27072718.Google Scholar
Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M. (2011). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 126137.Google Scholar
Schall, J. D. (1995). Neural basis of saccade target selection. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 6, 6385.Google ScholarPubMed
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2 (183), 8 pages. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183.Google Scholar
Srivastava, A. K. (1990). Multilingualism and school education in India: Special features, problems and prospects. In Pattanayak, D. P. (ed.), Multilingualism in India, pp. 3753. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Tao, L., Marzecova, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2 (123), 19 pages. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theeuwees, J., & Godijn, R. (2004). Inhibition-of-return and oculomotor interference. Vision Research, 44, 14851492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theeuwees, J., Kramer, A., Hahn, S., & Irwin, D. (1998). Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science, 9, 379385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vega, C., & Fernanez, M. (2011). Errors on the WCST correlate with language proficiency scores in Spanish–English bilinguals. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, 158164.Google Scholar
Wijnen, J. G., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2007). Response inhibition in motor and oculomotor conflict tasks: Different mechanisms, different dynamics? Brain and Cognition, 63, 260270.Google Scholar