Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T06:59:12.590Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2008

IOULIA KOVELMAN
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT
STEPHANIE A. BAKER
Affiliation:
Department of Education, Dartmouth College
LAURA-ANN PETITTO*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto
*
Address for correspondence: Laura-Ann Petitto, University of Toronto Scarborough, Department of Psychology, 1265 Military Trail, Toronto, ON, MIC 1A4Canada[email protected]

Abstract

How does age of first bilingual language exposure affect reading development in children learning to read in both of their languages? Is there a reading advantage for monolingual English children who are educated in bilingual schools? We studied children (grades 2–3, ages 7–9) in bilingual Spanish–English schools who were either from Spanish-speaking homes (new to English) or English-speaking homes (new to Spanish), as compared with English-speaking children in monolingual English schools. An early age of first bilingual language exposure had a positive effect on reading, phonological awareness, and language competence in both languages: early bilinguals (age of first exposure 0–3 years) outperformed other bilingual groups (age of first exposure 3–6 years). Remarkably, schooling in two languages afforded children from monolingual English homes an advantage in phoneme awareness skills. Early bilingual exposure is best for dual language reading development, and it may afford such a powerful positive impact on reading and language development that it may possibly ameliorate the negative effect of low SES on literacy. Further, age of first bilingual exposure provides a new tool for evaluating whether a young bilingual has a reading problem versus whether he or she is a typically-developing dual-language learner.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We extend our warm thanks to the members of the Petitto Lab, particularly Matthew Dubins, Katherine White, Elizabeth Norton and Paola Peacock-Villada, as well as the schools and the families who participated in this study. We are grateful to the three anonymous reviewers and the editor, Dr. Ping Li, for all their insightful comments that helped us improve our paper. Funding was provided by grants to L. A. Petitto from the National Institute of Health (R01HD045822-01A2 “Neuroimaging and behavioral studies of bilingual reading”) and (R21 HD 050558-02 Infants' Neural Basis for Language Using New Near Infrared Spectroscopy) and The Spencer Foundation. For more information on related research see http://www.utsc.utoronto.ca/~petitto/ and http://www.utsc.utoronto.ca/~petitto/lab//index.html.

References

Adams, M. J. (1994). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
August, D. & Hakuta, K. (eds.), (1997). Improving schooling for language-minority children: A research agenda. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I. & Petitto, L. A. (2007). Teaching reading in two languages: Should we teach both languages at the same time or in sequence? Abstract accepted as a poster presentation at the European Second Language Association Conference, Newcastle, UK.Google Scholar
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Billingsley, R. & Nagy, W. (2001). Processes underlying timing and fluency of reading: Efficiency, automaticity, coordination, and morphological awareness. In Wolf, M. (ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain, pp. 383414. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1991). Letters, sounds, and symbols: Changes in children's understanding of written language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 7589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. & Hakuta, K. (eds.) (1999). Confounded age: Linguistic and cognitive factors in age differences for second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G. & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read: Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9 (1), 4361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Majumder, S. & Martin, M. M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24 (1), 2744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Shenfield, T. & Codd, J. (2000). Languages, scripts, and the environment: Factors in developing concepts of print. Developmental Psychology, 36 (1), 6676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birdsong, D. & Molis, M. (2001). On the evidence for maturational constraints in second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44 (2), 235249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40 (2), 197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Oxford: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, D. J. (1964). The analysis of word sounds by young children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 34 (2), 158170.Google Scholar
Bruck, M. & Genesee, F. (1995). Phonological awareness in young second language learners. Journal of Child Language, 22 (2), 307324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldas, S. J. & Bankston, C. I. (1997). Effect of school population socioeconomic status on individual academic achievement. The Journal of Educational Research, 90, 269277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capute, A. J., Palmer, F. B., Shapiro, B. K., Wachtel, R. C., Schmidt, S. & Ross, A. (1986). Clinical linguistic and auditory milestone scale: Prediction of cognition in infancy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 28 (6), 762771.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X. & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 331361.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X. & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Estimating risk for future reading difficulties in kindergarten children: A research-based model and its clinical implications. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 3850.Google Scholar
Charron, F. & Petitto, L. A. (1991). Les premiers signes acquis par des enfants sourds en langue des signes québécoise (LSQ): Comparaison avec les premiers mots. Revue Québécoise de Linguistique Théorique et Appliquée, 10 (1), 71122.Google Scholar
Chee, M. W. L., Soon, C. S. & Lee, H. L. (2001). Relative language proficiency modulates BOLD signal change when bilinguals perform semantic judgments. NeuroImage, 13 (6), 11551163.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Cole, S. R. & Lightfoot, C. (2005). The development of children (5th edn.). New York: Worth Publishers.Google Scholar
Colvin, M. K., Dunbar, K. & Grafman, J. (2001). The effects of frontal lobe lesions on goal achievement in the water jug task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13 (8), 11291147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
CORE, Consortium on Reading Excellence. (1999). Assessing reading. Novato, CA: Arena Press.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (ed.) (1991). Interdependence of first- and second-language proficiency in bilingual children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M. & Damasio, A. R. (1994). The return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science, 264 (5162), 11021105.Google Scholar
Deuchar, M. & Quay, S. (1999). Language choice in the earliest utterances: A case study with methodological implications. Journal of Child Language, 26 (2), 461475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. (eds.), The frontal lobes, pp. 466503. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Clark-Chiarelli, N. & Wolf, A. (2004). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness in low-income Spanish and English bilingual preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (3), 323347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Droop, M. & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability in first- and second-language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38 (1), 78103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dulay, H. C. & Burt, M. K. (1974). Natural sequences in child second language acquisition. Language Learning, 24 (1), 3753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durgunoglu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E. & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85 (3), 453465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, A. W. & Lambon-Ralph, M. A. (2000). Age of acquisition effects in adult lexical processing reflect loss of plasticity in maturing systems: Insights from connectionist networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26 (5), 11031123.Google ScholarPubMed
Engen, L. & Hoien, T. (2002). Phonological skills and reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 15 (7–8), 613631.Google Scholar
Eviatar, Z. & Ibrahim, R. (2000). Bilingual is as bilingual does: Metalinguistic abilities of Arabic-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 451471.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., MacKay, I. R. A. & Meador, D. (1999). Native Italian speakers' perception and production of English vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106 (5), 29732987.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J. & MacKay, I. R. A. (1995). Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97 (5), 31253134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H. & Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41 (1), 78104.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Steinhauer, K. & Pfiefer, E. (2002). Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99 (8), 529534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frost, J., Madsbjerg, S., Niedersoe, J., Olofsson, A. K. & Sörensen, P. M. I. (2005). Semantic and phonological skills in predicting reading development: From 3–16 years of age. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research & Practice, 11 (2), 7992.Google Scholar
Gaab, N., Tallal, P., Kim, H., Lakshminarayanan, K., Archie, J. J., Glover, G. H. & Gabrieli, J. D. (2005). Neural correlates of rapid spectrotemporal processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 8288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F. (1989). Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 16, 161179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F. & Gándara, P. (1999). Bilingual education programs: A cross-national perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 55 (4), 665685.Google Scholar
Glennen, S. (2002). Language development and delay in internationally adopted infants and toddlers: A review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11 (4), 333339.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1981). Fragile and resilient properties of language learning. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds.), Language acquisition: State of the art, pp. 5177. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2004). Neuroscience and education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74 (1), 114.Google Scholar
Guion, S. G. (2005). Knowledge of English word stress patterns in early and late Korean–English bilinguals. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27 (4), 503–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HakutaK., Goto Butler, Y. & Witt, D. K., Goto Butler, Y. & Witt, D. (2000). How long does it take English learners to attain proficiency? The University of California Linguistic Minority Research Institute Policy Report, 2000-1.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E. & Wiley, E. (2003). Critical evidence: A test of the critical-period hypothesis for second-language acquisition. Psychological Science, 14 (1), 3138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakuta, K. & Garcia, E. E. (1989). Bilingualism and education. American Psychologist, 44 (2), 374379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, T., Wexler, K. & Holcomb, P. (2000). An ERP investigation of binding and coreference. Brain and Language, 75 (3), 313346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernandez, A. E. & Li, P. (2007). Age of acquisition: Its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 1334 (4), 113.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2004). Language development. Belmont, CA: Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
Holm, A. & Dodd, B. (2001). Comparison of cross-language generalization following speech therapy. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 53 (3), 166172.Google Scholar
Holowka, S., Brosseau-Lapré, F. & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Semantic and conceptual knowledge underlying bilingual babies' first signs and words. Language Learning, 52 (2), 205262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S. & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21 (1), 6099.Google Scholar
Kimberg, M. & Serdyukov, P. (2004). A balanced relationship of languages in a bilingual society. NABE Journal of Research and Practice, 2 (1), 214224.Google Scholar
Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A. & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: An fMRI investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20 (1), 153–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovelman, I. & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Bilingual babies' maturational and linguistic milestones as a function of their age of first exposure to two languages. Poster presented at the conference for the Society for Neuroscience, Orlando, FL.Google Scholar
Kovelman, I. & Petitto, L. A. (2003). Stages of language development in bilingual children exposed to their other language at different ages. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the International Symposium on Bilingualism, Phoenix, AZ.Google Scholar
Leafstedt, J. M. & Gerber, M. M. (2005). Crossover of phonological processing skills: A study of Spanish-speaking students in two instructional settings. Remedial and Special Education, 26 (4), 226235.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. Oxford: Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, H., Bates, E. & Li, P. (1992). Sentence interpretation in bilingual speakers of English and Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13 (4), 451484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, M. G. & Tashakkori, A. (2004). Effects of a two-way bilingual program on the literacy development of students in kindergarten and first grade. Bilingual Research Journal, 28 (1), 19–34.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. & Eichen, E. B. (1991). The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 30 (4), 486512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. & Fischer, S. D. (1989). Looking through phonological shape to lexical meaning: The bottleneck of non-native sign language processing. Memory & Cognition, 17 (6), 740754.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd edn.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2000). Grammaticality judgments in a second language: Influences of age of acquisition and native language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21 (3), 395423.Google Scholar
Mills, J. (2001). Being bilingual: Perspectives of third generation Asian children on language, culture and identity. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 4 (6), 383402.Google Scholar
Muter, V. & Diethelm, K. (2001). The contribution of phonological skills and letter knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. Language Learning, 51 (2), 187219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (1–2), 1136.Google Scholar
Neville, H. J., Coffey, S. A., Lawson, D. S., Fischer, A., Emmorey, K. & Bellugi, U. (1997). Neural systems mediating American Sign Language: Effects of sensory experience and age of acquisition. Brain and Language, 57 (3), 285308.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14 (1), 1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Asada, T., Aruga, M., Hirakata, M., Nishikawa, M., Katoh, A. & Imabayashi, E. (2001). Functional anatomy of musical perception in musicians. Cerebral Cortex, 11 (8), 754760.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (2002). Language and literacy development in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2005). Grammatical morphology in children learning English as a second language: Implications of similarities with Specific Language Impairment. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools, 36 (3), 172187.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z. (1998). Assessing lexical development in bilingual babies and toddlers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 2 (3), 347372.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V. & Oller, K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18 (1), 4158.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C. & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning: A Journal of Applied Linguistics, 43, 93120.Google Scholar
Pena, M., Maki, A., Kovacic, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F. & Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100 (20), 1170211705.Google Scholar
Perani, D., Abutalebi, J., Paulesu, E., Brambati, S., Scifo, P. & Cappa, S. F. (2003). The role of age of acquisition and language usage in early, high-proficient bilinguals: An fMRI study during verbal fluency. Human Brain Mapping, 19 (3), 170182.Google Scholar
Petersson, K. M., Reis, A., Askelof, S., Castro-Caldas, A. & Ingvar, M. (2000). Language processing modulated by literacy: A network analysis of verbal repetition in literate and illiterate subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (3), 364382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitto, L. A. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27 (1), 152.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. (2005). How the brain begets language: On the neural tissue underlying human language acquisition. In McGilvray, J. (ed.), The Cambridge companion to Chomsky, pp. 84101. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. (2007). Cortical images of early language and phonetic development using Near Infrared Spectroscopy. In Battro, A. M., Fischer, K. W. & Léna, P. J. (eds.), The educated brain, pp. 213232. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. & Dunbar, K. N. (in press). New findings from educational neuroscience on bilingual brains, scientific brains, and the educated mind. In Fischer, K. & Katzir, T. (eds.), Building usable knowledge in mind, brain and education. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. & Holowka, S. (2002). Evaluating attributions of delay and confusion in young bilinguals: Special insights from infants acquiring a signed and a spoken language. Journal of Sign Language Studies, 3 (1), 433.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Katerelos, M., Levy, B. G., Gauna, K., Tetreault, K. & Ferraro, V. (2001). Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28 (2), 453496.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. & Kovelman, I. (2003). The Bilingual Paradox: How signing–speaking bilingual children help us resolve bilingual issues and teach us about the brain's mechanisms underlying all language acquisition. Learning Languages, 8, 518.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Zatorre, R. J., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E. J., Dostie, D. & Evans, A. C. (2000). Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: Implications for the neural basis of human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97 (25), 1396113966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poeppel, D. & Wexler, K. (1993). The full competence hypothesis of clause structure in early German. Language, 69 (1), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D. & Snow, C. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children reading in English: Toward a model of comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97 (2), 246256.Google Scholar
Reading Success Network. (1997). Taking a reading: A researcher's guide to reading assessment. Los Angeles: Los Angeles County Office of Education.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. L. & Turner, L. J. (1989). Linguistic awareness skills in grade one children in a French immersion setting. Reading and Writing, 1, 7386.Google Scholar
Sanders, L. D., Neville, H. J. & Woldorff, M. G. (2002). Speech segmentation of native and non-native speakers: The use of lexical, syntactic, and stress-pattern cues. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45 (3), 519530.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In Neuman, S. & Dickinson, D. (eds.), Handbook for research in early literacy, pp. 97110. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Senghas, R. J. & Kegl, J. (1994). Social considerations in the emergence of Idioma de Signos Nicaraguense. Signpost/International Sign Linguistics Quarterly, 7 (1), 4046.Google Scholar
Shapiro, B. K., Palmer, F. B., Antell, S., Bilker, S., Ross, A. & Capute, A. J. (1990). Precursors of reading delay: Neurodevelopmental milestones. Pediatrics, 85, 416420.Google Scholar
Shin, S. J. & Milroy, L. (1999). Bilingual language acquisition by Korean schoolchildren in New York City. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2 (2), 147167.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. & Ryan, L. (2004). Language acquisition: The age factor. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Slavin, R. E. & Cheung, A. (2003). Effective reading programs for English language learners. Baltimore, MD: Center for Research on the Education of Students Placed at Risk, The Johns Hopkins University.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Galaburda, A. M. (2002). Second language learners and understanding the brain. In Galaburda, A. M., Kosslyn, S. M. & Christen, Y. (eds.), The languages of the brain, pp. 151165. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. & Hoefnagel-Hoehle, M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Child Development, 49, 11141128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M. & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100 (5), 28602865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, I. (1991). Foreign accents revisited: The English pronunciation of Russian immigrants. Language Learning, 41, 177204.Google Scholar
van Steensel, R. (2006). Relations between socio-cultural factors, the home literacy environment and children's literacy development in the first years of primary education. Journal of Research in Reading, 29 (4), 367382.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L. (2000). Components in early second language reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4 (4), 313330.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. & McCune, L. (1994). When is a word a word? Journal of Child Language, 21, 517542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber-Fox, C. M. & Neville, H. J. (1999). Functional neural subsystems are differentially affected by delays in second language immersion: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilinguals. In Birdsong, D. (ed.), Second language acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis, pp. 2338. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Weber-Fox, C. & Neville, H. J. (2001). Sensitive periods differentiate processing of open- and closed-class words: An ERP study of bilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44 (6), 13381353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1992). The organization and reorganization of human speech perception. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 15, 377402.Google Scholar
White, L. & Genesee, F. (1996). How native is near-native? The issue of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 12, 238265.Google Scholar
Wolfe, C. D. & Bell, M. A. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Developmental Psychobiology, 44, 6883.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5 (3), 211239.Google Scholar
Wolpert, L., Beddington, R., Brockes, J., Jessell, T., Lawrence, P. & Meyerowitz, E. (1998). Principles of development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wong Fillmore, L. (1976). The second time around: Cognitive and social strategies in second language acquisition. Ph.D. dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. (1991). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery – Revised. Chicago: Riverside Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Yopp, H. K. (1995). A test for assessing phonemic awareness in young children. The Reading Teacher, 49, 2029.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C. & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131 (1), 329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed