Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T00:47:45.609Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states and cross-linguistic transfer*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2013

KATY BORODKIN*
Affiliation:
Bar-Ilan University
MIRIAM FAUST
Affiliation:
Bar-Ilan University
*
Address for correspondence: Katy Borodkin, Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, 52900, Israel[email protected]

Abstract

This study examined cross-linguistic transfer in oral language skills in a sample of 50 native Hebrew speakers who learned English as a second language. The ability to retrieve phonological forms of words in naming, as manifested by the tendency to experience tip-of-the-tongue states, was correlated across languages. We also found within and across language correlations between this ability and grammatical accuracy, lexical diversity, and syntactic complexity in second language narratives. These findings are consistent with the transfer across languages in oral language skills and provide insights into the processes linking phonological and higher level encoding in production of connected speech.

Type
Research Notes
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported in part by grant number 602/06 of the Israel Science Foundation. We wish to thank the three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. We also wish to thank the students who participated in the study.

References

Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2002). Reading, syntactic, orthographic, and working memory skills of bilingual Arabic–English speaking Canadian children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 661678.Google Scholar
Bock, K. (1987). An effect of the accessibility of word forms on sentence structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Levelt, W. J. (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding. In Gernsbacher, M. A. (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, pp. 945984. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Borodkin, K., & Faust, M. (2012). Word retrieval in developmental language impairments: Application of the Tip-of-the-Tongue paradigm. In Faust, M. (ed.), The handbook of the neuropsychology of language, pp. 963982. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borodkin, K., & Faust, M. (2013). Native language phonological skills in low-proficiency second language learners. Ms., Bar-Ilan University.Google Scholar
Borodkin, K., & Faust, M. (in press). Naming abilities in low-proficiency second language learners. Journal of Learning Disabilities, doi:10.1177/0022219412453769. Published online by Sage, August 27, 2012.Google Scholar
Brown, R., & McNeill, D. (1966). The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 325337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., & Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 542579.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In California State Department of Education (ed.), Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework, pp. 349. Los Angeles, CA: Evaluation, Dissemination, and Assessment Center, California State University.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1991). Interdependence of first and second-language proficiency in bilingual children. In Bialystok, E. (ed.), Language processing in bilingual children, pp. 7089. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283321.Google Scholar
Donaldson, J. (2001). Room on the broom. London: Macmillan Children's Books.Google Scholar
Durán, P., Malvern, D., Richards, B., & Chipere, N. (2004). Developmental trends in lexical diversity. Applied Linguistics, 25, 220242.Google Scholar
Durgunoglu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453465.Google Scholar
Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8, 453485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faust, M., Dimitrovsky, L., & Davidi, S. (1997). Naming difficulties in language-disabled children: Preliminary findings with the application of the Tip-of-the-Tongue paradigm. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 10261036.Google Scholar
Faust, M., Dimitrovsky, L., & Schacht, T. (2003). Naming difficulties in children with dyslexia: Application of the tip of the tongue paradigm. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 203216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faust, M., & Sharfstein-Friedman, S. (2003). Naming difficulties in adolescents with dyslexia: Application of the tip-of-the-tongue paradigm. Brain and Cognition, 53, 211217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, P., Tonkyn, A., & Wigglesworth, G. (2000). Measuring spoken language: A unit for all reasons. Applied Linguistics, 21, 354375.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Brown, A. S. (2006). From tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) data to theoretical implications in two steps: When more TOTs means better retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 462483.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Bonanni, M. P. (2005). Proper names get stuck on bilingual and monolingual speakers’ tip of the tongue equally often. Neuropsychology, 19, 278287.Google Scholar
Iwashita, N., Brown, A., McNamara, T., & O'Hagan, S. (2008). Assessed levels of second language speaking proficiency: How distinct? Applied Linguistics, 29, 2449.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., Furth, K., & Hilliard, C. (2012). Phonological overlap affects lexical selection during sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 14391449.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K., Kan, P. F., & Conboy, B. T. (2010). Lexical and grammatical associations in sequential bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53, 684698.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Michael, E., Tokowicz, N., & Dufour, R. (2002). The development of lexical fluency in a second language. Second Language Research, 18, 137171.Google Scholar
Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American Engish. Providence, RI: Brown Unviersity Press.Google Scholar
Kulot, D. (2003). Das kleine krokodil und die große liebe. Stuttgart-Wien: K. Thienemann Verlag (Thienemann Verlag GmbH).Google Scholar
Lee, M.-W., & Gibbons, J. (2007). Rhythmic alternation and the optional complementiser in English: New evidence of phonological influence on grammatical encoding. Cognition, 105, 446456.Google Scholar
Lennon, P. (1990). Investigating fluency in EFL: A quantitative approach. Language Learning, 40, 387417.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 138.Google Scholar
Lindsey, K. A., Manis, F. R., & Bailey, C. E. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 482494.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1995). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (2nd edn.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk (3rd edn.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2002). Investigating accommodation in language proficiency interviews using a new measure of lexical diversity. Language Testing, 19, 85104.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Martínez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P. S. (2004). The language-specific nature of grammatical development: Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7, 212224.Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Lervåg, A. (2011). Cross-linguistic transfer of oral language, decoding, phonological awareness and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis of the correlational evidence. Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 114135.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2003). Syntactic complexity measures and their relationship to L2 proficiency: A research synthesis of college-level L2 writing. Applied Linguistics, 24, 492518.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. (2002). Narrative competence among monolingual and bilingual school children in Miami. In Oller, K. & Eilers, R. (eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children, pp. 135174. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language processing and second language development: Processability Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Polio, C. G. (1997). Measures of linguistic accuracy in second language writing research. Language Learning, 47, 101143.Google Scholar
Proctor, C. P., August, D., Carlo, M. S., & Snow, C. (2006). The intriguing role of Spanish language vocabulary knowledge in predicting English reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 159169.Google Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1993). Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Richards, B. (1987). Type/token ratios: What do they really tell us? Journal of Child Language, 14, 201209.Google Scholar
Richards, J. C. (1983). Listening comprehension: Approach, design, procedure. TESOL Quarterly, 17, 219240.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User's Guide (Version 1.2). Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Rosston, K., Gerber, M., & Solari, E. (2008). Influence of oral language and phonological awareness on children's bilingual reading. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 413429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tavakoli, P., & Foster, P. (2008). Task design and second language performance: The effect of narrative type on learner output. Language Learning, 58, 439473.Google Scholar
Wolfe-Quintero, K., Inagaki, S., & Kim, H. Y. (1998). Second language development in writing: Measures of fluency, accuracy and complexity. Honolulu, HI: Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center, University of Hawai'i at Mãnoa.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

BORODKIN, K. Supplementary Material

Appendix

Download BORODKIN, K. Supplementary Material(PDF)
PDF 96.4 KB