Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T20:29:23.324Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comparison of Spanish morphology in monolingual and Spanish–English bilingual children with and without language impairment*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2012

GARETH P. MORGAN*
Affiliation:
The Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk, The University of Texas at Austin
M. ADELAIDA RESTREPO
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
ALEJANDRA AUZA
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Querétaro
*
Addressed for correspondence: Gareth P. Morgan, 151 Essex Street, Haverhill, MA 01832, USA[email protected]

Abstract

This study compares Spanish morphosyntax error types and magnitude in monolingual Spanish and Spanish–English bilingual children with typical language development (TD) and language impairment (LI). Performance across groups was compared using cloze tasks that targeted articles, clitics, subjunctives, and derivational morphemes in 57 children. Significant differences were observed between bilingual TD and LI groups on all tasks; however, no differences were observed between bilinguals with TD and monolinguals with LI except on a sum-score across all tasks. There were no observed differences between bilinguals and monolinguals with TD; however, 60% of bilinguals with TD were misclassified as LI when using a cut score derived from monolingual-only data. Results support evidence that Spanish morphosyntax is vulnerable to error in monolingual and bilingual Spanish–English children with LI. However, the grammatical deficit seems clinically relevant only when children are compared to the same language peer group (i.e., bilinguals compared to bilinguals).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

The research in this article was supported by grants from the Institute of Education Sciences: Spanish Screener for Language Impairments in Children (PI: Restrepo, IES# R324A080024) and Post-doctoral Training Grant in Special Education (PI: Vaughn, IES# R324B080008).

References

Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological Testing (7th edn.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. T. (1996). Assessing the grammar of Spanish-speaking children: A comparison of two procedures. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 333344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. T. (1999a). Impact of first language loss on grammar in a bilingual child. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21, 416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. T. (1999b). Noun phrase gender agreement in language attrition: Preliminary results. Bilingual Research Journal, 23 (4), 318337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. T. (2001). Lexical morphology and verb use in child first language loss: A preliminary case study investigation. International Journal of Bilingualism, 5 (4), 377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. T. (2012). First language loss in Spanish-speaking children: Patterns of loss and implications for clinical practice. In Goldstein (ed.), pp. 193–212.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. T., & Marquez, A. (2009). The article paradigm in Spanish-speaking children with SLI in language contact situations. In Grinstead (ed.), pp. 29–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. T., & Souto, S. (2005). The use of articles by monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 621647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auza, A. (2009). Uso de artículos y sus funciones semánticas en niños con y sin trastorno del lenguaje. Ph.D. dissertation, Universidad Autónoma de Querétaro.Google Scholar
Auza, A., & Hernández, A. (2005). El procesamiento dinámico en la adquisición de la morfología derivativa. In Mayor, M. A., Zubiauz, B. & Díez, E. (eds.), Estudios sobre la adquisición del Lenguaje (Colección Aquilafuente 90), pp. 189203. Salamanca: Ediciones Universidad de Salamanca.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (1998). Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41 (5), 11851192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44 (4), 905924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2005). Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26 (2), 195225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D. (2008). Assessment of bilingual children for identification of language impairment: Current findings and implications for practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11 (1), 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R. (1983). Mood selection among Spanish-speaking children, ages 4 to 12. Bilingual Review, 10 (1), 2132.Google Scholar
Bortolini, U., Caselli, M. C., Deevy, P., & Leonard, L. B. (2002). Specific language impairment in Italian: The first steps in the search for a clinical marker. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37 (2), 7793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L., & Serra, M. (1997). Grammatical morphology deficits of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Amsterdam Series in Child Language Development, 6 (69), 3345.Google Scholar
Crago, M., & Paradis, J. (2003). Two of a kind? Commonalities and variation in languages and language learners. In Levy, Y. & Schaeffer, J. (eds.), Language competence across populations: Towards a definition of specific language impairment, pp. 97110. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dawson, J. I., Stout, C. E., & Eyer, J. A. (2003). Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test: Third Edition. DeKalb, IL: Janelle Publications.Google Scholar
De Jong, J. (2010). Notes on the nature of bilingual specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 273277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C., & Horner, E. (2011). Bilingual language assessment: A meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 10771088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eng, N., & O'Connor, B. (2000). Acquisition of definite articles + noun agreement of Spanish–English bilingual children with specific language impairment. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21, 114124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2004). Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B. A. (ed.) (2012). Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish–English speakers. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Green, S. B., Lissitz, R. W., & Muliak, S. A. (1977). Limitations of coefficient alpha as an index of test unidimensionality. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 37, 827838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grinstead, J. (ed.) (2009). Hispanic child languages: Typical and impaired development. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guiberson, M. M., Barret, K. C., Jancosek, E. G., & Itano, C. (2006). Language maintenance and loss in preschool-age children of Mexican immigrants: Longitudinal study. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 28 (1), 417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2002). Narratives in two languages: Assessing performance of bilingual children. Linguistics and Education, 13 (2), 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2004). Narrative development and disorders in bilingual children. In Goldstein (eds.), pp. 235–256.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Restrepo, M. A., Bedore, L. M., Peña, E., & Anderson, R. T. (2000). Language sample analysis in Spanish-speaking children: Methodological considerations. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 8898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Restrepo, M. A., & Simon-Cereijido, G. (2006). Evaluating the discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49 (6), 1209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Simon-Cereijido, G. (2007). The discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Latino English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 968981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, C. S., Lawrence, F. R., & Miccio, A. W. (2008). Exposure to English before and after entry into Head Start: Bilingual children's receptive language growth in Spanish and English. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11 (1), 3056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernández-Pina, F. (1984). Teorías psicosociolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua materna. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Jacobson, P. F. (2012). The effects of language impairment on the use of direct object pronouns and verb inflections in heritage Spanish speakers: A look at attrition, incomplete acquisition and maintenance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (1), 2238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, P. F., & Schwartz, R. G. (2002). Morphology in incipient bilingual Spanish-speaking preschool children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23 (1), 2341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapantzoglou, M., Restrepo, M. A., & Thompson, M. S. (2012). Dynamic assessment of word learning skills: Identifying language impairment in bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 43, 8196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (2004). Kaufman assessment battery for children (2nd edn.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K. (2008). Language disorders in bilingual children and adults. San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing.Google Scholar
López Ornat, S., Fernández, A., Gallo, P., & Mariscal, S. (1994). La adquisición de la lengua española. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Merino, B. J. (1983). Language development in normal and language handicapped Spanish-speaking children. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 5 (4), 379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merino, B. J. (1992). Acquisition of syntactic and phonological features in Spanish. In Butler, K. G. (eds.), Hispanic children and adults with communication disorders: Assessment and intervention, 5798. Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Publishers.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). The acquisition of Spanish: Morphosyntactic development in monolingual and bilingual L1 acquisition and adult L2 acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K. (2007). Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11 (3), 301328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, G. P., Restrepo, M. A., & Auza, A. (2009). Variability in the grammatical profiles of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. In Grinstead (ed.), pp. 283–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Numeroff, L. J., & Bond, F. (1985). If you give a mouse a cookie. New York: Laura Geringer Book.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2010). The interface between bilingual language development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 227252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2000). Tense and temporality: Similarities and differences between language impaired and second language children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 834848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2004). Comparing L2 and SLI grammars in French: Focus on DP. In Prévost, P. & Paradis, J. (eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories, pp. 89108. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., & Genesee, F. (2005/2006). Domain-specific versus domain general theories of the deficit in SLI: Object pronoun acquisition by French–English bilingual children. Language Acquisition, 13, 3362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E., Crago, M., & Genesee, F. (2010). Bilingual children's acquisition of the past tense: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 37, 125.Google Scholar
Peña, E. (2001). Dynamic assessment of diverse children: A tutorial. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 212224.Google Scholar
Peña, E., Iglesias, A., & Lidz, C. (2001). Reducing test bias through dynamic assessment of children's word learning ability. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 138154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (1998). The acquisition of mood selection in Spanish relative clauses. Journal of Child Language, 25 (3), 585604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (2001). Subjunctive mood in Spanish child relatives: At the interface of linguistic and cognitive development. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language (vol. II), pp. 6993. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla, A. P., & Brunner, J. (2011). General and specific effects of lexicon in grammar: Determiner and object pronoun omissions in child Spanish. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55 (2), 2012.Google ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Pirvulescu, M., & Roberge, Y. (2009). Bilingualism as a window into the language faculty: The acquisition of objects in French-speaking children in bilingual and monolingual contexts. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12 (1), 97112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perona, K., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (2005). Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test: Third Edition. Language Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 103115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool language tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo, M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41 (6), 1998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Restrepo, M. A., Castilla, A. P., Schwanenflugel, P. J., Neuharth-Pritchett, S., Hamilton, C. E., & Arboleda, A. (2010). Effects of a supplemental Spanish oral language program on sentence length, complexity, and grammaticality in Spanish-speaking children attending English-only preschools. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Restrepo, M. A., & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2001). Article use in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 28 (2), 433452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Restrepo, M. A., & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2012). Grammatical impairments in Spanish–English speaking children. In Goldstein (ed.), pp. 213–232.Google Scholar
Restrepo, M. A., & Kruth, K. (2000). Grammatical characteristics of a Spanish–English bilingual child with specific language impairment. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21 (2), 66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo, M. A., & Silverman, S. W. (2001). Validity of the Spanish preschool language scale-3 for use with bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10 (4), 382393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Test of Early Grammatical Impairment: Examiner's manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Roseberry, C. A., & Connell, P. J. (1991). The use of an invented language rule in the differentiation of normal and language-impaired Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 34, 594603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sackett, D. L., Haynes, R. B., Guyatt, G. H., & Tugwell, P. (1991). Clinical epidemiology: A basic science for clinical medicine (2nd edn.). Boston, MA: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Sánchez-Naranjo, J., & Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (2010). In the wrong mood at the right time: Children's acquisition of the Spanish subjunctive in temporal clauses. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 55 (2), 227255.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1991). Spanish language attrition in a contact situation with English. In Seliger, H. W. & Vago, R. (eds.), First language attrition: Structural and theoretical perspectives, pp. 151171. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1994). Language contact and change. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (2003). Linguistic consequences of reduced input in bilingual first language acquisition. In Montrul, S. & Ordoñez, F. (eds.), Linguistic theory and language development in Hispanic languages, pp. 375397. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Simon-Cereijido, G., & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2007). Spontaneous language markers of Spanish language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 317339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E. H., Semel, E., & Secord, W. A. (2006). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – 4 Spanish (SCELF–4). Needham Heights, MA: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., & Hwang, M. (1999). Derivational suffix productivity for students with and without language-learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42 (1), 220230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed