Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T18:57:58.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rumination and the Enhancement of Fear: Some Laboratory Findings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2009

Graham C. L. Davey
Affiliation:
The University of Sussex, Brighton

Abstract

This paper describes some of the ways in which ruminative rehearsal and repetitive processing of phobic events may maintain or enhance anxious responding. It focuses on laboratory studies of repetitive rehearsal of potential phobic outcomes and repetitive processing of interoceptive cues. Rehearsal of threatening outcomes whilst in anxious mood has been shown to result in a short term enhancement and resistance to extinction of phobic responses. Increased attention to and discrimination of bodily cues (interoception) has also been shown to result in enhanced phobic responding and increased resistance to extinction. This latter finding appears to result from interoception (1) inflating the threatening nature of the outcome, and (2) leading the individual to reject contradictory information from other sources.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acierno, R.E., Herson, M. and Van Hasselt, V.B. (1993). Interventions for panic disorder: a critical review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review 13, 561578.Google Scholar
Arntz, A., Lavy, E., van den Berg, G. and van Rijsoort, S. (1993). Negative beliefs of spider phobics: a psychometric evaluation of the spider phobia beliefs questionnaire. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy 15, 257277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, D.H. and Cerny, J.A. (1988). Psychological Treatment of Panic. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Barlow, D. (1988). Anxiety and its Disorders. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Borkovec, T.D. (1994). The nature, functions, and origins of worry. In Davey, G.C.L. and Tallis, F. (Eds). Worrying: Perspectives on Theory, Assessment and Treatment. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Borkovec, T.D. and Inz, J. (1990). The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: a predominance of thought activity. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 153158.Google Scholar
Borkovec, T.D. and Lyonfields, J.D. (1993). Worry: thought suppression of emotional processing. In Krohne, H.W. (Ed). Vigilance and Avoidance. Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Bower, G.H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist 36, 129148.Google Scholar
Brown, T.A., Antony, M.M. and Barlow, D.H. (1992). Psychometric properties of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire in a clinical anxiety disorder sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy 30, 3337.Google Scholar
Campbell, D., Sanderson, R.E. and Laverty, S.G. (1964). Characteristics of a conditioned response in human subjects during extinction trials following a simple traumatic conditioning trial. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 68, 627639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, D.M. and Wells, A. (1994). A cognitive model of social phobias. In Heimberg, R.G., Liebowitz, M., Hope, D. and Schneier, F. (Eds). Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Davey, G.C.L. and Matchett, G. (1994a). UCS rehearsal and the retention and enhancement of differential “fear” conditioning: the effects of trait and state anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 708718.Google Scholar
Davey, G.C.L. and Matchett, G. (1994b). The effects of response feedback on conditioned responding during extinction: implications for the role of interoception in anxiety-based disorders. Submitted.Google Scholar
Davey, G.C.L. (1987). An integration of human and animal models of Pavlovian conditioning: associations, cognitions and attributions. In Davey, G.C.L. (Ed). Cognitive Processes and Pavlovian Conditioning in Humans. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Davey, G.C.L. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning in humans: UCS revaluation and self-observation of conditioned responding. Medical Science Research 16, 957961.Google Scholar
Davey, G.C.L. (1992). Classical conditioning and the acquisition of human fears and phobias: a review and synthesis of the literature. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy 14, 2966.Google Scholar
Davey, G.C.L. (1994). Pathological worrying as exacerbated problem solving. In Davey, G.C.L. and Tallis, F. (Eds). Worrying: Perspectives on Theory, Assessment and Treatment. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Davey, G.C.L., de Jong, P.J. and Tallis, F. (1993). UCS inflation in the aetiology of a variety of anxiety disorders; some case histories. Behaviour Research and Therapy 31, 495498.Google Scholar
Ehlers, A. (1991). Cognitive factors in panic attacks: symptom probability and sensitivity. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 5, 157173.Google Scholar
Ehlers, A. (1993). Interoception and panic disorder. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy 15, 321.Google Scholar
Ehlers, A. and Breuer, P. (1992). Increased cardiac awareness in panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 101, 371382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H.J. (1979). The conditioning model of neurosis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2, 155159.Google Scholar
Eysenck, M.W. and Mogg, K. (1992). Clinical anxiety, trait anxiety, and memory bias. In Christianson, S-A. (Ed). The Handbook of Emotion and Memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Graf, P. and Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23, 553568.Google Scholar
Harvey, J.M., Richards, J.C., Dziadosz, T. and Swindell, A. (1993). Misinterpretation of ambiguous stimuli in panic disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research 17, 235248.Google Scholar
Jones, T. and Davey, G.C.L. (1990). The effects of cued UCS rehearsal on the retention of differential “fear” conditioning: an experimental analogue of the “worry” process. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 159164.Google Scholar
Lang, P.J. (1985). The cognitive psychophysiology of emotion: fear and anxiety. In Tuma, A.H. and Maser, D. (Eds). Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lang, P.J., Kozak, M.J., Miller, G.A., Levin, D.N. and MacLean, A. (1980). Emotional imagery: conceptual structure and pattern of somato-visceral response. Psychophysiology 17, 179192.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R.S., Averill, J.R. and Opton, E.M. (1970). Feelings and Emotions: The Loyola Symposium. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lovibond, P.F. and Rapee, R.M. (1994). The representation of feared outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, in press.Google Scholar
Marks, I.M. (1987). Fears, Phobias and Rituals. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Matchett, G. and Davey, G.C.L. (1994). UCS rehearsal during anxious mood: enhancement but not incubation of “fear” responding. Submitted.Google Scholar
Mathews, A and MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy 23, 563569.Google Scholar
Mathews, A. and MacLeod, C. (1994). Emotional processing biases. Annual Review of Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Biehaviour Research and Therapy 28, 455468.Google Scholar
McNally, R.J. and Louro, C.E. (1994). Fear of flying in agoraphobia and simple phobia: distinguishing features. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, in press.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1985). Emotional effects of false automatic feedback. Psychological Bulletin 98, 471494.Google Scholar
Rachman, S., Lopatka, C. and Levitt, K. (1987). Panic: the links between cognitions and bodily symptoms—I. Behaviour Research and Therapy 25, 411423.Google Scholar
Rapee, R.M. (1985). A distinction between panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder: clinical presentation. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 19, 227232.Google Scholar
Rice, C., Matchett, G. and Davey, G.C.L. (1994). The reporting of autobiographical memories during the rehearsal of aversive stimuli: the effect of anxious mood state. Submitted.Google Scholar
Richards, M. and Martin, I. (1990). Eysenck's incubation of fear hypothesis: an experimental test. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 373384.Google Scholar
Russell, C. and Davey, G.C.L. (1991). The effects of false response feedback on human “fear” conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy 29, 191196.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, P.M. and Clark, D.M. (1991). Cognititve therapy for panic disorder. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 5, 215226.Google Scholar
Sandin, B. and Chorot, P. (1989). The incubation theory of fear/anxiety: experi-mental investigation in a human laboratory model of Pavlovian conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy 27, 918.Google Scholar
Tallis, F., Davey, G.C.L. and Capuzzo, N. (1994). The phenomenology of nonpathological worry: a preliminary investigation. In Davey, G.C.L. and Tallis, F. (Eds). Worrying: Perspectives on Theory, Assessment and Treatment. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Valins, S. (1966). Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4, 400408.Google Scholar
Wells, A. (1990). Panic disorder in association with relaxation induced anxiety: an attentional training approach to treatment. Behavior Therapy 21, 273280.Google Scholar
Wells, A. (1994). Attention and the control of worry. In Davey, G.C.L. and Tallis, F. (Eds). Worrying: Perspectives on Theory, Assessment and Treatment. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.