Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T20:48:31.313Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The map of cognitive processes in boredom: multiple mediation models

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2020

Ion Milea*
Affiliation:
Doctoral School ‘Evidence Based Psychological Assessment and Interventions’, Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania The International Institute for the Advanced Studies of Psychotherapy and Applied Mental Health, Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania
Roxana A.I. Cardoş
Affiliation:
The International Institute for the Advanced Studies of Psychotherapy and Applied Mental Health, Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Babeş-Bolyai University, Republicii Street 37, 400015, Cluj-Napoca, Romania
Daniel David
Affiliation:
The International Institute for the Advanced Studies of Psychotherapy and Applied Mental Health, Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Babeş-Bolyai University, Republicii Street 37, 400015, Cluj-Napoca, Romania Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Background:

Trait boredom is associated with several internalizing and externalizing problems. Addressing existing research gaps in the field, the present study investigated the map of cognitive processes for boredom, based on the rational emotive behaviour therapy model (REBT).

Aims:

The general aim of the study was to investigate the organization of irrational and rational evaluative cognitions related to boredom, and the association between boredom and depression symptoms and state/trait anxiety.

Methods:

The 233 participants (84% women) completed online scales of evaluative cognitions, trait boredom, trait/state anxiety and depression. Multiple mediation models via the SPSS extension PROCESS were employed.

Results:

The REBT psychopathology and psychological health models were partially confirmed, as the evaluative primary cognitions predicted positively and significantly the secondary ones in both cases. Low frustration tolerance (LFT) and global evaluations (GE), and frustration tolerance (FT), respectively, had significant effects. We found a positive significant association between boredom proneness and the negative dysfunctional emotions investigated.

Conclusions:

Both results offer further support for the hierarchy of cognitions and the distinction between the level of irrationality and rationality in REBT. This is the first attempt to assess a cognitive map of boredom, underlining the importance of (L)FT in relation to boredom. The significance of GE in boredom suggests that people might see themselves responsible, or even blame themselves, others or life itself while bored. The associations of boredom with anxiety and depression are relevant, as its role in those contexts is not yet fully understood.

Type
Main
Copyright
© British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Al-Saggaf, Y., MacCulloch, R., & Wiener, K. (2019). Trait boredom is a predictor of phubbing frequency. Journal of Technology in Behavioral Science, 4, 245252. doi: 10.1007/s41347-018-0080-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azizi, N. (2009). Manufacturing productivity improvement: a study of human boredom, job rotation and scheduling. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Ottawa.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York, USA: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX, USA: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bench, S. W., & Lench, H. C. (2013). On the function of boredom. Behavioral Sciences, 3, 459472. doi: 10.3390/bs3030459 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caserta, D. A., Dowd, E. T., David, D., & Ellis, A. (2010). Rational and irrational beliefs in primary prevention and mental health. Rational and Irrational Beliefs, 173194.Google Scholar
Chan, C. S., van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., Poon, C. Y., Tam, K. Y., Wong, V. U., & Cheung, S. K. (2018). Situational meaninglessness and state boredom: cross-sectional and experience-sampling findings. Motivation and Emotion, 42, 555565. doi: 10.1007/s11031-018-9693-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chin, A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K. S., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Bored in the USA: experience sampling and boredom in everyday life. Emotion, 17, 110. doi: 10.1037/emo0000232 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chou, W. J., Chang, Y. P., & Yen, C. F. (2018). Boredom proneness and its correlation with internet addiction and internet activities in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The Kaohsiung Journal of Medical Sciences, 34, 467474. doi: 10.1016/j.kjms.2018.01.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Mello, S., & Graesser, A. (2010). Modeling cognitive-affective dynamics with Hidden Markov Models. In Catrambone, R. & Ohlsson, S. (eds), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Cognitive Science Society (pp. 27212726). Austin: Cognitive Science Society. escholarship.org/uc/item/06b952g5 Google Scholar
D’Mello, S., Taylor, R.S., Graesser, A. (2007). Monitoring affective trajectories during complex learning. In Proceedings of the 29th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. escholarship.org/uc/item/6p18v65q Google Scholar
Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Regan, K., & Kuhlman, M. M. (2004). Boredom proneness in anger and aggression: effects of impulsiveness and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 16151627. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K., & Kuhlman, M. M. (2005). Driving anger, sensation seeking, impulsiveness, and boredom proneness in the prediction of unsafe driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37, 341348. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2004.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darden, D. K. (1999). Boredom: a socially disvalued emotion. Sociological Spectrum, 19, 1337. doi: 10.1080/027321799280280 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, D. (2003). Rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT): the view of a cognitive psychologist. In Dryden, W. (ed), Rational Emotive Behaviour Therapy: Theoretical Developments (pp. 130159). New York, USA: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
David, D. O., DiGiuseppe, R., Dobrean, A., Păsărelu, C. R., & Balazsi, R. (2019). The measurement of irrationality and rationality. In Advances in REBT (pp. 79100). Springer, Cham.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, D., Lynn, S. J., & Ellis, A. (eds) (2009). Rational and Irrational Beliefs: Research, Theory, and Clinical Practice. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, D., & Szentagotai, A. (2006). Cognitions in cognitive-behavioral psychotherapies; toward an integrative model. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 284298. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.09.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiGiuseppe, R., Gorman, B., & Raptis, J. (2020). The factor structure of the Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2: implications for rational emotive behavior therapy. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 132. doi: 10.1007/s10942-020-00349-0 Google Scholar
Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind: defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 482495. doi: 10.1177/1745691612456044 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: a conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. New York, USA: L. Stuart.Google Scholar
Ellis, A. (1995). Changing rational-emotive therapy (RET) to rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT). Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 13, 8589. doi: 10.1007/BF02354453 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, A., & Dryden, W. (1997). The Practice of Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy. New York, USA: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Elpidorou, A. (2014). The bright side of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1245. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01245 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elpidorou, A. (2017a). The moral dimensions of boredom: a call for research. Review of General Psychology, 21, 155. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000098 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elpidorou, A. (2017b). The bored mind is a guiding mind: toward a regulatory theory of boredom. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 130. doi: 10.1007/s11097-017-9515-1 Google Scholar
Elpidorou, A. (2018). The good of boredom. Philosophical Psychology, 31, 323351. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2017.1346240 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., & Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of life meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 307340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness – the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 417. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, C. D. (1993). Boredom at work: a neglected concept. Human Relations, 46, 395417. doi: 10.1177/001872679304600305 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., & Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: an emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30, 647666. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U. E., Pekrun, R., & Lipnevich, A. A. (2014). Types of boredom: an experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 401419. doi: 10.1007/s11031-013-9385-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green-Demers, I. (1997). When boredom gives birth to motivation: interrelations between interest-enhancing strategies, interest and self-determination. Dissertation, University of Ottawa. Ottawa, Canada: National Library of Canada.Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. Retrieved from: http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf Google Scholar
Havermans, R. C., Vancleef, L., Kalamatianos, A., & Nederkoorn, C. (2015). Eating and inflicting pain out of boredom. Appetite, 85, 5257. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyland, P., Shevlin, M., Adamson, G., & Boduszek, D. (2014). The organization of irrational beliefs in posttraumatic stress symptomology: testing the predictions of REBT theory using structural equation modelling. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70, 4859. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyland, P., Shevlin, M., Adamson, G., & Boduszek, D. (2013). Modeling the structure of the Attitudes and Belief Scale 2 using CFA and bifactor approaches: toward the development of an abbreviated version. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 43, 6071. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2013.777467 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyland, P., Fox, R., Treacy, G., Maguire, P., Boduszek, D., & Vallières, F. (2017). Assessing the factorial validity of the attitudes and belief scale 2-abbreviated version: a call for the development a gold standard method of measuring rational and irrational beliefs. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 35, 111124. doi: 10.1007/s10942-016-0243-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyland, P., McGinty, G., Karatzias, T., Murphy, J., Vallières, F., & McHugh Power, J. (2019). Can the REBT theory explain loneliness? Theoretical and clinical applications. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 48, 3951 doi: 10.1080/16506073.2018.1475505 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isacescu, J., Struk, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 17411748. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1259995 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jang, E. H., Park, B. J., Park, M. S., Kim, S. H., & Sohn, J. H. (2015). Analysis of physiological signals for recognition of boredom, pain, and surprise emotions. Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 1, 112. doi: 10.1186/s40101-015-0063-5 Google Scholar
LePera, N. (2011). Relationships between boredom proneness, mindfulness, anxiety, depression, and substance use. The New School Psychology Bulletin, 8, 1525.Google Scholar
Loukidou, L., Loan-Clarke, J., & Daniels, K. (2009). Boredom in the workplace: more than monotonous tasks. International Journal of Management Reviews, 11, 381405. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2370.2009.00267.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martz, M. E., Schulenberg, J. E., Patrick, M. E., & Kloska, D. D. (2016). ‘I am so bored!’: prevalence rates and sociodemographic and contextual correlates of high boredom among American adolescents. Youth & Society, 50, 688710. doi: 10.1177/0044118X15626624 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mastro, D. E., Eastin, M. S., & Tamborini, R. (2002). Internet search behaviors and mood alterations: a selective exposure approach. Media Psychology, 4, 157172. doi: 10.1207/S153278XMEP0402_03 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merrifield, C., & Danckert, J. (2014). Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 481491. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3755-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikulas, W. L., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The essence of boredom. The Psychological Record, 43, 312.Google Scholar
Moynihan, A., Igou, E., & van Tilburg, W. A. (2017). Boredom increases impulsiveness: a meaning-regulation perspective. Social Psychology, 48, 293309. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000317 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). The concept of flow. In Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology (pp. 239263). Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
Nederkoorn, C., Vancleef, L., Wilkenhöner, A., Claes, L., & Havermans, R. C. (2016). Self-inflicted pain out of boredom. Psychiatry Research, 237, 127132. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.063 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with boredom in school: an experience sampling perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 4959. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oltean, H. R., Hyland, P., Vallières, F., & David, D. O. (2017). An empirical assessment of REBT models of psychopathology and psychological health in the prediction of anxiety and depression symptoms. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 45, 600615. doi: 10.1017/S1352465817000133 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oltean, H. R., Hyland, P., Vallières, F., & David, D. O. (2019). Rational beliefs, happiness and optimism: an empirical assessment of REBT’s model of psychological health. International Journal of Psychology, 54, 495500. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12492 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 531549. doi: 10.1037/a0019243 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piotrowski, C. (2013). Boredom research: an analysis of topical domain and historical trends. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 40, 50.Google Scholar
Popa, C., & Predatu, R. (2019). The effect of an integrative CBT/REBT intervention in improving emotional functioning and emotional stability in Romanian medical students. Journal of Evidence-Based Psychotherapies, 19, 5971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seo, J., Laine, T. H., & Sohn, K. A. (2019). Machine learning approaches for boredom classification using EEG. Journal of Ambient Intelligence and Humanized Computing, 1-16. doi: 10.1007/s12652-019-01196-3 Google Scholar
Smith, R. P. (1981). Boredom: a review. Human Factors, 23, 329340. doi: 10.1177/001872088102300308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA, USA: Consulting Psychologists Press Google Scholar
Spörrle, M., Welpe, I. M., & Foersterling, F. (2006). Cognitions as determinants of (mal) adaptive emotions and emotionally intelligent behavior in an organizational context. Psicothema, 18, 165171.Google Scholar
Steinberger, F., Moeller, A, & Schroeter, R. (2016). The antecedents, experience, and coping strategies of driver boredom in young adult males. Journal of Safety Research, 59, 6982. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2016.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundberg, N. D., Latkin, C. A., Farmer, R. F., & Saoud, J. (1991). Boredom in young adults: gender and cultural comparisons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22, 209223. doi: 10.1177/0022022191222003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svendsen, L. (2005). A Philosophy of Boredom, trans. Irons, John. London, UK: Reaktion.Google Scholar
Todman, M. (2003). Boredom and psychotic disorders: cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 66, 146167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tze, V. M., Daniels, L. M., & Klassen, R. M. (2016). Evaluating the relationship between boredom and academic outcomes: a meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 119144. doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9301-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2012). On boredom: lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 181194. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2016). Can boredom help? Increased prosocial intentions in response to boredom. Self and Identity, 16, 8296. doi: 0.1080/15298868.2016.1218925CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2017). Boredom begs to differ: differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion, 17, 309, 140. doi: 10.1037/emo0000233 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., & Sedikides, C. (2013). In search of meaningfulness: nostalgia as an antidote to boredom. Emotion, 13, 450, 136. doi: 10.1037/a0030442 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vodanovich, S. J. (2003). Psychometric measures of boredom: a review of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 137, 569595. doi: 10.1080/00223980309600636 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vodanovich, S. J., & Watt, J. D. (2016). Self-report measures of boredom: an updated review of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 150, 196228. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2015.1074531 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogel-Walcutt, J. J., Fiorella, L., Carper, T., & Schatz, S. (2012). The definition, assessment, and mitigation of state boredom within educational settings: a comprehensive review. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 89111. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9182-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watt, J. D., & Hargis, M. B. (2010). Boredom proneness: its relationship with subjective underemployment, perceived organizational support, and job performance. Journal of Business and Psychology, 25, 163174. doi: 10.1007/s10869-009-9138-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinerman, J., & Kenner, C. (2016). Boredom: that which shall not be named. Journal of Developmental Education, 40, 1823.Google Scholar
Windle, R. C., & Windle, M. (2018). Adolescent precursors of young adult drinking motives. Addictive Behaviors, 82, 151157. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.03.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yazzie-Mintz, E. (2007). Voices of students on engagement: a report on the 2006 high school survey of student engagement. Center for Evaluation and Education Policy, Indiana University, pp. 1–12. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED495758.pdf Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.