Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T05:35:28.444Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intensive Cognitive Therapy for PTSD: A Feasibility Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 June 2010

Anke Ehlers*
Affiliation:
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and MaudsleyNHS Trust and King's College London, UK
David M. Clark
Affiliation:
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and MaudsleyNHS Trust and King's College London, UK
Ann Hackmann
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, UK
Nick Grey
Affiliation:
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and MaudsleyNHS Trust and King's College London, UK
Sheena Liness
Affiliation:
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and MaudsleyNHS Trust and King's College London, UK
Jennifer Wild
Affiliation:
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and MaudsleyNHS Trust and King's College London, UK
John Manley
Affiliation:
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and MaudsleyNHS Trust and King's College London, UK
Louise Waddington
Affiliation:
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and MaudsleyNHS Trust and King's College London, UK
Freda McManus
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, UK
*
Reprint requests to Anke Ehlers, Department of Psychology PO77, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background: Cognitive Behaviour Therapy (CBT) of anxiety disorders is usually delivered in weekly or biweekly sessions. There is evidence that intensive CBT can be effective in phobias and obsessive compulsive disorder. Studies of intensive CBT for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are lacking. Method: A feasibility study tested the acceptability and efficacy of an intensive version of Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (CT-PTSD) in 14 patients drawn from consecutive referrals. Patients received up to 18 hours of therapy over a period of 5 to 7 working days, followed by 1 session a week later and up to 3 follow-up sessions. Results: Intensive CT-PTSD was well tolerated and 85.7 % of patients no longer had PTSD at the end of treatment. Patients treated with intensive CT-PTSD achieved similar overall outcomes as a comparable group of patients treated with weekly CT-PTSD in an earlier study, but the intensive treatment improved PTSD symptoms over a shorter period of time and led to greater reductions in depression. Conclusions: The results suggest that intensive CT-PTSD is a feasible and promising alternative to weekly treatment that warrants further evaluation in randomized trials.

Type
Accelerated Publication
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2010. The online version of this article is published within Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommerial-ShareAlike licence <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/>. The written permission of British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies must be obtained for commercial re-use.

Introduction

Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) treatments are effective in a range of anxiety disorders (Hofmann and Smits, Reference Hofmann and Smits2008; Öst, Reference Öst2008) and are usually delivered in weekly or biweekly sessions over the course of several months. This format of treatment delivery may not always be ideal, for example, when patients live far away from the therapy setting or if they are under pressure to get better quickly because of problems at work or in relationships with significant others. Some patients find it difficult to engage in lengthy psychological treatment (Bisson et al., Reference Bisson, Ehlers, Matthews, Pilling, Richards and Turner2007). This raises the questions of (a) whether faster improvement can be achieved if CBT is delivered in an intensive format, and (b) how well such intensive treatments are tolerated.

Intensive CBT approaches have been evaluated for a range of phobias and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Öst (Reference Öst1989) showed that a single long session of CBT is highly effective in specific phobias. Abramowitz, Foa and Franklin (Reference Abramowitz, Foa and Franklin2003) reported that 30 hours of CBT for OCD was equally effective whether delivered over 3 or 8 weeks. Oldfield, Salkovskis and Taylor (in press) further found that 12–18 hours of CBT for OCD was equally effective if it was delivered over 5 days or over 12 to 18 weeks. Similarly, in a paediatric study comparing intensive (daily sessions for 3 weeks) and once-weekly family-based CBT for OCD, Storch et al. (Reference Storch, Geffken, Merlo, Mann, Duke, Munson, Adkins, Grabill, Murphy and Gooman2007) reported that the intensive treatment was as effective as weekly treatment. Clark (Reference Clark and Salkovskis1996) described a successful example of treating a patient with panic disorder with an intensive 2-day version of cognitive therapy for panic disorder. Deacon and Abramowitz (Reference Deacon and Abramowitz2006) delivered 9 hours of CBT for panic disorder over 2 consecutive days, reporting that 6 of their 10 patients were panic free after treatment and at follow-up. In an effectiveness study of intensive exposure treatment for agoraphobia, Hahlweg, Fiegenbaum, Frank, Schröder and Witzleben (Reference Hahlweg, Fiegenbaum, Frank, Schröder and Witzleben2001) found effect sizes in symptom improvement similar to those reported in efficacy trials. Thus, intensive versions of CBT may be as effective as the traditional weekly or biweekly sessions.

There are as yet no studies of intensive CBT in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Trauma-focused CBT programmes have been shown to be effective in PTSD (Bisson et al., Reference Bisson, Ehlers, Matthews, Pilling, Richards and Turner2007; Bradley, Greene, Russ, Dutra and Westen, Reference Bradley, Greene, Russ, Dutra and Westen2005) and are currently recommended as first line treatments for this condition (American Psychiatric Association, 2004; Australian Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health, 2007; Foa, Keane, Friedman and Cohen, Reference Foa, Keane, Friedman and Cohen2005; National Institute of Clinical Excellence, 2005; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Cloitre, Nemeroff, Nutt, Seedat, Shalev, Wittchen and Zohar2009; Veterans Health Administration and Department of Defense, 2004). Examples of trauma-focused CBT programmes include Foa's Prolonged Exposure (Foa and Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998; Foa et al., Reference Foa, Hembree, Cahill, Raunch, Riggs, Feeny and Yadin2005) and Resick's Cognitive Processing Therapy (Resick and Schnicke, Reference Resick and Schnicke1992, Reference Resick and Schnicke1993) and Ehlers and Clark's Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000; Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus, Fennell, Herbert and Mayou2003, Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell, Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005).

All trauma-focused CBT protocols require the patient to confront their trauma memories and trauma reminders, but methods of confrontation and its duration vary. It is unclear how well patients with PTSD would tolerate these procedures in an intensive treatment format. Some authors have raised the general concern that treatments that include systematic exposure to trauma memories may not be well tolerated, as confronting trauma memories can be very distressing (e.g. Kilpatrick and Best, Reference Kilpatrick and Best1984). In line with the concerns, some studies have observed high drop-out rates of between 20 and 35% with trauma-focused PTSD treatments that contain a significant degree of exposure to trauma memories (e.g. Resick, Nishith, Weaver, Astin and Feuer, Reference Resick, Nishith, Weaver, Astin and Feuer2002; Foa et al., Reference Foa, Hembree, Cahill, Raunch, Riggs, Feeny and Yadin2005; Schnurr et al., Reference Schnurr, Friedman, Engel, Foa, Shea, Chow, Resick, Thurston, Orsillo, Haug, Turner and Bernardy2007), although the average drop-out rate may not be higher than for other PTSD treatments (Hembree et al., Reference Hembree, Foa, Dorfan, Street, Kowalski and Tu2003). Note, however, that patients may have dropped out of treatment for reasons other than poor tolerance of the procedures (e.g. rapid improvement). There have also been concerns about a risk of symptom exacerbation with exposure to trauma memories (e.g. Tarrier et al., Reference Tarrier, Pilgrim, Sommerfield, Fragher, Reynolds, Graham and Barrowclough1999). However, symptom exacerbations have been found to occur only in a small minority of patients and were short-lived (Foa, Zoellner, Feeny, Hembree and Alvarez-Conrad, Reference Foa, Zoellner, Feeny, Hembree and Alvarez-Conrad2002; Hackmann, Ehlers, Speckens and Clark, Reference Hackmann, Ehlers, Speckens and Clark2004).

Nevertheless, the concerns about the acceptability of trauma-focused CBT and the potential risk of symptom exacerbation may be amplified when using an intensive treatment format and need to be investigated. On the other hand, an intensive treatment format may offer the advantage of completing the work on trauma memories, once started, over a short period of time, rather than in small doses over several weeks. This may help with making the treatment acceptable to patients.

The present study was designed to explore the feasibility and acceptability of an intensive version of Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (CT-PTSD), a highly effective version of trauma-focused CBT (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus, Fennell, Herbert and Mayou2003, Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Yule, Perrin, Tranah, Dalgleish and Clark2007). The standard once-weekly version of CT-PTSD has been shown to be very acceptable to patients. No drop-outs were observed in three randomized controlled trials (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus, Fennell, Herbert and Mayou2003, Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Yule, Perrin, Tranah, Dalgleish and Clark2007). Furthermore, CT-PTSD has been successfully disseminated to routine clinical settings (Duffy, Gillespie and Clark, Reference Duffy, Gillespie and Clark2007; Gillespie, Duffy, Hackmann and Clark, Reference Gillespie, Duffy, Hackmann and Clark2002).

This feasibility study investigated acceptability and outcomes of intensive CT-PTSD in a case series of patients with chronic PTSD. Outcomes for patients treated with intensive CT-PTSD were compared with outcomes of comparable patients treated with weekly CT-PTSD in a previous study (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005). We expected that an intensive version of CT-PTSD would be effective and that it would lead to recovery over a shorter period of time than weekly CT-PTSD.

Method

Participants

Fourteen patients were recruited from consecutive referrals from General Practitioners and Community Mental Health Teams to the Centre for Anxiety Disorders and Trauma, Maudsley Hospital, UK (n = 11), or the Anxiety Disorders Research Group at the Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford (n = 3). Patients had to meet the following inclusion criteria for the feasibility study: 18–65 years old; meeting diagnostic criteria for chronic PTSD as determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams, Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1995) with a minimum duration of 3 months (range 3–126 months, see Table 1); the current episode of PTSD was linked to one or two discrete traumatic events in adulthood; and PTSD was the main problem. Exclusion criteria were: no memory for the trauma; history of psychosis; current alcohol or drug dependence; borderline personality disorder; acute suicide risk; assessment and treatment could not be conducted without the aid of an interpreter. Of 16 eligible patients who were offered participation in the study, 2 declined. Of the 14 participants, 2 had comorbid panic disorder with agoraphobia, and 3 had comorbid major depression. Table 1 shows trauma and demographic characteristics of the sample. Table 1 also shows that the sample was similar to the group who received weekly CT-PTSD in Ehlers et al.'s (Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005) randomized controlled trial.

Table 1. Sample description: N (%) or means (standard deviations)

aThis includes patients on sick leave because of their PTSD symptoms.

Outcome measures

Clinician-rated PTSD symptoms. Independent assessors (trained psychologists) gave the Clinician-Administered PTSD scale (CAPS-SX, Blake et al., Reference Blake, Weathers, Nagy, Kaloupek, Gusman, Charney and Keane1995). The CAPS assesses the frequency and severity of each of the symptoms specified in DSM-IV. To determine interrater-reliability, a random sample of 37 CAPS interviews (conducted by 14 different interviewers) was rated by a second clinician (14 different raters). The interviews came from the present and a related study (Ehlers et al., in preparation). Results indicated very good reliability for the PTSD diagnosis, kappa = .95, and total severity score, r = .98. Patients were considered to meet DSM-IV criteria for PTSD on the CAPS-SX if they reported the minimum number of symptoms in each symptom cluster, with a score of at least 1 (on both the frequency and intensity scales) and the global severity rating was 2 or greater (“definite distress or functional impairment”).

Severity of PTSD symptoms. Patients completed the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS, Foa, Cashman, Jaycox and Perry, Reference Foa, Cashman, Jaycox and Perry1997). The PDS asks patients to rate how often they were bothered by each of the PTSD symptoms specified in DSM-IV ranging from 0 “never” to 3 “3 to 5 times per week/almost always”. The PDS yields a sum score measuring the overall severity of PTSD symptoms. Foa et al. (Reference Foa, Cashman, Jaycox and Perry1997) demonstrated that the self-report questionnaire has good reliability and concurrent validity with other PTSD measures.

Disability. Patients completed the Sheehan Disability Scale (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Patients rated the interference caused by the PTSD symptoms in their (a) work, (b) social life/leisure activities, and (c) family life/home responsibilities on 3 Likert scales from 0 “not at all” to 10 “very severe”. The disability score was the mean of these ratings.

Depression and anxiety. Symptoms of anxiety and depression were assessed with the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI, Beck and Steer, Reference Beck and Steer1993a) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI, Beck and Steer, Reference Beck and Steer1993b), standard 21-item self-report measures of high reliability and validity.

Treatment programme

CT-PTSD is based on Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model of PTSD. This model suggests that people with PTSD perceive a serious current threat that has two sources: excessively negative appraisals of the trauma and/or its sequelae, and characteristics of trauma memories that lead to reexperiencing symptoms. The problem is maintained by cognitive strategies and behaviours (such as thought suppression, rumination, safety-seeking behaviours) that are intended to reduce the sense of current threat, but maintain the problem by preventing change in the appraisals or trauma memory, and/or by increasing symptoms. CT-PTSD targets the three factors specified in the model. For each patient, an individualized version of model is developed. The maintaining factors are addressed with the procedures described below. The relative weight given to different treatment procedures differs from patient to patient, depending on the case formulation. Table 2 shows an outline of how the intensive treatment might progress. Two case examples are described by Grey, McManus, Hackmann, Clark and Ehlers (Reference Grey, McManus, Hackmann, Clark, Ehlers and Grey2009).

Table 2. Example of course of treatment in intensive cognitive therapy for PTSD

Goal 1: Modify excessively negative appraisals of the trauma and its sequelae. As in other CT programs, excessively negative appraisals of trauma sequelae, such as the initial PTSD symptoms (e.g. Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant, Reference Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant1998) and other people's responses after the event (e.g. Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers, Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001), are modified by the provision of information, Socratic questioning, and behavioural experiments. As many patients with PTSD describe a sense of permanent change since the trauma (e.g. Ehlers, Maercker and Boos, Reference Ehlers, Maercker and Boos2000), “reclaiming your life” assignments are discussed in each session and usually done as homework. Patients are encouraged to “reclaim” their former lives by reinstating significant activities or social contacts they have given up since the trauma.

Changing negative appraisals of the trauma poses a special challenge as much of the patient's evidence for the problematic appraisals stems from what they remember about the trauma. Thus, work on appraisals of the trauma needs to be closely integrated with work on the trauma memory. Disjointed recall of the trauma in PTSD (a) makes it difficult to assess the problematic meanings by just talking about the trauma, and (b) has the effect that insights from cognitive restructuring may not be sufficient to produce a large shift in affect. Ehlers and Clark (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) developed a special procedure to shift problematic meanings of the trauma, termed “Updating Trauma Memories”. This involves:

  1. (1) Identifying the idiosyncratic appraisals of the trauma: To access the problematic idiosyncratic meanings of the trauma, the moments during the trauma that create the greatest distress and sense of “nowness” during recall (hot spots) are identified through imaginal reliving (Foa and Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998) or narrative writing (Blanchard et al., Reference Blanchard, Hickling, Devineni, Veazey, Galovski, Mundy and Buckley2003; Resick and Schnicke, Reference Resick and Schnicke1992), and discussion of intrusive memories (see Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Hackmann, Steil, Clohessy, Wenninger and Winter2002). The personal meaning of these moments is explored in careful questioning.

  2. (2) Identification of updating information: The next step is to identify information that provides evidence against the idiosyncratic appraisals linked to each hot spot (“updating information”). This may be information from the course of the event that has not been linked to the meaning of the hot spot, or a new conclusion the patient has reached in cognitive restructuring. Examples of the former include information that the outcome was better than expected (e.g. patient did not die, is not paralyzed); information that explained the patient's or other people's behaviour (e.g. the patient complied with the perpetrator's instructions because he had a knife); the realization that an impression or perception during the trauma was not true (e.g. the perpetrator had a toy gun rather than a real gun); or explanations from experts of what happened (e.g. explanations about medical procedures in the course of the trauma). Examples of the latter are conclusions from the cognitive restructuring of excessively negative appraisals, such as “I am a bad person”, “It was my fault”, “My actions were disgraceful” or “I attract disaster”, using cognitive therapy techniques such as Socratic questioning, systematic discussion of evidence for and against the appraisals, behavioural experiments, pie charts, or surveys.

  3. (3) Active incorporation of the updating information into the hot spots. Once updating information that the patient finds compelling has been identified, it is actively incorporated into the relevant hot spot. Patients are asked to bring the hot spot to their mind (either through reliving or reading the narrative) and to then remind themselves (prompted by the therapist) of the updating information either (a) verbally (e.g. “I know now that . . . .”), (b) by imagery (e.g. visualizing how one's wounds have healed; visualizing perpetrator in prison; looking at recent photo of the family), (c) by performing movements or actions that are incompatible with the original meaning of this moment (e.g. moving about, jumping up and down for hot spots that involve prediction that the patient will die or be paralyzed) or (d) incompatible sensations (e.g. touching a healed arm). To summarize the updating process, a written narrative is created that includes and highlights the new meanings (e.g. “I know now that it was not my fault”).

Goal 2: Reduce reexperiencing by elaboration of the trauma memories and discrimination of triggers. Four main techniques are used to elaborate the trauma memory and reduce reexperiencing: imaginal reliving of the event (Foa and Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998), writing out a detailed narrative of the event (Blanchard et al., Reference Blanchard, Hickling, Devineni, Veazey, Galovski, Mundy and Buckley2003; Resick and Schnicke, Reference Resick and Schnicke1992), revisiting the site, and discrimination of triggers. Each procedure has advantages, and the relevant weight given to them depends on the patient's level of engagement with the trauma memory and the length of the event. “Imaginal reliving”, in which the patient visualizes the event while simultaneously describing what is happening and what he or she is feeling and thinking, is particularly good at facilitating engagement with the memory and retrieval of all aspects of the memory (including emotions and sensory components). Writing a narrative is particularly useful when aspects of what happened or the order of events are unclear. Reconstructing the event with diagrams and models and a visit to the site can be of further assistance in such instances. For patients with very long traumas and those who tend to dissociate when talking about the trauma, writing may also be easier to manage than imaginal reliving. Revisiting the site of the traumatic event is particularly helpful in facilitating the realization that the event is in the past. When visiting the site, therapist and patient therefore discuss the way the scene is different from the day of the trauma (Then versus Now). Revisiting the site is also used to complement discussion and obtain new information that helps explain why or how an event occurred.

Building on the observation that trauma memories are disjointed and often lack crucial context information, Ehlers and colleagues (Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000; Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael, Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael2004) outlined that memory elaboration needs to link the hot spots of the trauma with new information that updates their meanings. To establish this new link, CT-PTSD uses the Updating Trauma Memories procedure described above.

Discrimination of triggers of reexperiencing symptoms usually involves two stages. First, patient and therapist carefully analyze where and when intrusions occur to identify triggers. This involves some detective work as patients are usually not aware of many of the sensory triggers (e.g. particular colours, sounds, smells, tastes, touch). Systematic observation (by the patient and the therapist) is usually necessary before all triggers are identified. Once triggers have been identified, the next aim is to break the link between the triggers and the trauma memory. This involves several steps in therapy. First, the patient learns to distinguish between “Then” versus “Now” i.e. the patient learns to focus on how the present triggers and their context (Now) are different from the trauma (Then). Second, intrusions are intentionally triggered in therapy so that the patient can learn to apply the Then versus Now discrimination. This is done by bringing triggers into the therapy session. For example, traffic accident survivors may listen to sounds that remind them of the trauma, such as sounds of brakes screeching, collisions, glass breaking or sirens. People who were attacked with a knife may look at a range of metal objects. Survivors of bombings or fires may look at and smell smoke produced by a smoke machine. People who saw a lot of blood during the trauma may look at red fluids. The Then versus Now discrimination can be facilitated by carrying out actions that were not possible during the trauma (e.g. movements that were not possible in the trauma, touching objects or looking at photos that remind them of their present life). Third, patients apply these strategies in their natural environment. When reexperiencing occurs, they remind themselves that they are responding to a memory, not current reality. They focus their attention on how the present situation is different from the trauma, and may carry out actions that were not possible during the trauma.

If reexperiencing symptoms persist after successful updating of the hot spots and discrimination of triggers, imagery transformation techniques can be useful. The patient transforms the image into a new image that signifies that the trauma is over. The transformed images can provide convincing evidence that the intrusions are a product of the patient's mind rather than representing current reality. Image transformation is also particularly helpful with intrusions that represent images of things that did not really happen during the trauma (usually anticipated bad consequences of the trauma).

Goal 3: Drop dysfunctional behaviours and cognitive strategies. The first step in addressing behaviours and cognitive strategies that maintain PTSD is usually to discuss the problematic consequences of the strategy. Sometimes these can be demonstrated directly by a behavioural experiment. For example, asking the patient to try hard not to think about a certain image (e.g. black-and-white cat sitting on therapist's shoulder) demonstrates that thought suppression is likely to increase intrusions. In other instances, a discussion of advantages and disadvantages is helpful, for example when addressing rumination. The next step involves dropping or reversing the problematic strategy, usually in a behavioural experiment.

Procedure

Before treatment, patients completed the self-report scales and the CAPS. In the intensive treatment phase, patients were offered up to 18 hours of therapy, over a period of 5 to 7 working days. Treatment days usually comprised a morning and an afternoon session lasting 90 min to 2 hours, with a break for lunch. For training and piloting purposes, some of the treatments (n = 7) were conducted by two therapists. Therapists received daily supervision during this phase. Patients attended a further therapy session one week after the last intensive day and completed the self-report questionnaires and were interviewed with the CAPS. They received up to 3 booster sessions during the following 3 months and completed the self-report measures and the CAPS at 3 months and 9 months.

Data analysis

Treatment effect sizes for changes in symptom scores between the pre-treatment assessment and the 3-month assessment (end of treatment) were calculated using Cohen's d statistic (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988), following the formula used in van Etten and Taylor's (Reference van Etten and Taylor1998) meta-analysis of PTSD treatments: d = M initialM post/ SD pooled, where SD pooled = initial + SD 2post)/2.

Analyses of covariance were used to compare the outcome of the intensive CT-PTSD group with the weekly CT-PTSD comparison group at 3 weeks, 3 months and 9 months, using pre-treatment scores as the covariate.

Symptom exacerbation was defined using the cut-offs for reliable exacerbation determined by Foa et al. (Reference Foa, Zoellner, Feeny, Hembree and Alvarez-Conrad2002), i.e. increases in symptoms greater than 6.15 on the PDS, 8.37 on the BAI, and 4.53 on the BDI.

Results

Acceptability of intensive CT-PTSD

All 14 patients completed the intensive treatment. None of the patients showed symptom exacerbation on any measure, neither at 3 weeks nor at 3 months.

Duration of treatment

Patients received a mean of 9.4 (SD = 2.0) morning or afternoon sessions of about 90 minutes during the intensive phase up to the 3-week assessment, and a further mean of 2.6 (SD = 1.4) sessions up to the 3-month assessment. A mean of 107 minutes was spent in the treatment sessions on imaginal reliving the trauma or narrative writing (including updating memories).

Treatment outcome

Ten patients (71.4%) no longer met criteria for PTSD at 3 weeks, 12 (85.7%) at 3 months and 13 (92.9%) at 9 months. Table 3 shows the results for assessor-rated PTSD severity, self-reported PTSD symptoms, disability, depression and anxiety. On all measures, patients treated with intensive CT-PTSD showed very large improvements.

Table 3. Treatment outcome measures for intensive Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (CT-PTSD) and comparison group receiving weekly CT-PTSD from Ehlers et al. (Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005), means (standard deviations)

CAPS = Clinician Administered PTSD Scale; PDS = Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale; Sheehan = Sheehan Scale of Disabililty; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; BAI = Beck Anxiety Inventory; n.s. = nonsignificant; N/A = not assessed; ES = Effect size.

As expected, participants showed greater improvement in PTSD symptoms than the weekly CT-PTSD comparison group at 3 weeks, and comparable outcome at 3 months and 9 months. The results are illustrated in Figure 1. Similar results were obtained for anxiety (BAI) and disability. For depressive symptoms, the intensive group showed greater improvements than the weekly treatment group at all assessment points.

Figure 1. Comparison of outcome for case series of intensive Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (CT-PTSD) (INTENSIVE) with a comparison group receiving weekly CT-PTSD from Ehlers et al.'s (Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005) randomized controlled trial (WEEKLY). PTSD symptom severity was measured with the Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS).

Whether treatment was conducted by 1 or 2 therapists did not affect outcome on any measure (all ps > .64 for 3 months, p > .45 for 9 months) (see Bevan, Oldfield and Salkovskis, Reference Bevan, Oldfield and Salkovskis2010).

Discussion

This feasibility study showed that intensive CT-PTSD was acceptable to patients and effective. No patient dropped out and treatment outcome was very similar to that observed for weekly CT-PTSD by Ehlers et al. (Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005) in a comparable sample of patients with chronic PTSD. The number of sessions needed and time spent on imaginal reliving or narrative writing was also very similar to the weekly treatment. Possible advantages of the intensive treatment were that it led to improvement over a shorter period of time than weekly CT, and that it led to greater reductions in depressive symptoms that persisted through the follow-up phase. No disadvantages were identified in this sample. The results are promising and suggest that intensive CT-PTSD is a viable and acceptable treatment that warrants further evaluations in randomized controlled trials.

There may be several advantages of intensive treatment over weekly treatment in PTSD. Difficult social and economic circumstances often contribute to the patient's distress, and further distressing life events are not uncommon. Condensing the main course of treatment into a few days reduces the chances of daily difficulties and life events interfering with treatment and taking up valuable therapy time. Furthermore, problems with concentration and memory are common in PTSD and the intensive format may help to keep the therapeutic material fresh in patients' (and therapists') mind until the next session so that no time is lost “catching up”. Meeting the therapist each day may also be particularly helpful in overcoming avoidance and depression by building therapeutic momentum. Last, but not least, intensive CT-PTSD appears to help patients get better over a shorter period of time. This may be especially beneficial if the PTSD has led to secondary problems such as being unable to work, problems in important relationships, or avoidance of important situations such as receiving medical treatment. It may also be relevant for certain therapeutic settings such as residential units.

Although no disadvantages of the intensive treatment were identified in the present study, weekly treatment may also have its advantages. Some patients with PTSD have lost all positive elements of their previous lives, such as friends or a job. For these patients, weekly treatment may allow them more opportunity to “reclaim their lives” with ongoing support from the therapist. Furthermore, if the patient has very generalized phobic avoidance, weekly sessions may also have advantages as it allows more time for homework between sessions so that a wider range of situations can be tackled while receiving the therapist's support and encouragement. Inconsistent attendance may cause greater problems in intensive treatment than in weekly treatment as a greater proportion of therapy time is lost; for example, patients with significant alcohol or substance use problems may miss whole days of treatment. If patients drop out of intensive treatment, there is very limited time to re-engage them in therapy. Finally, patients and therapists need to clear much of a working week from other commitments and this may not always be feasible.

This study had several limitations. Most importantly, there was no random allocation and the comparisons with the previously treated patient cohort have to be interpreted with caution. Randomized controlled trials are needed to establish the efficacy of Intensive CT-PTSD. Second, the patients included in this study had developed PTSD in response to relatively short discrete traumatic events in adulthood. It is unclear whether the results would generalize to survivors of childhood trauma, or survivors of multiple and very prolonged traumas. Some research suggests that survivors of multiple early traumatic events such as childhood sexual abuse may benefit from training in emotion regulation before starting exposure-based treatment of PTSD (Cloitre et al., Reference Cloitre, Koenen, Cohen and Han2002).

Acknowledgements

The study was funded by the Wellcome Trust (grant 069777 to AE and DMC). We would like thank Kelly Archer, Anne Beaton, Anna Bevan and Ruth Morgan for their help with interviews, data collection, entry and analysis, and Margaret Dakin for administrative support.

References

Abramowitz, J. S., Foa, E. B. and Franklin, M. E. (2003). Exposure and ritual prevention for obsessive-compulsive disorder: effectiveness of intensive versus twice-weekly treatment sessions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 394398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Handbook of Psychiatric Measures. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2004). Treatment of Patients with Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. http://www.psychiatryonline.com/pracGuide/pracGuideTopic_11.aspxGoogle Scholar
Australian Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health (2007). Australian Guidelines for the Treatment of Adults with Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. http://www.acpmh.unimelb.edu.au.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T. and Steer, R. A. (1993a). Beck Anxiety Inventory Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T. and Steer, R. A. (1993b). Beck Depression Inventory Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bevan, A., Oldfield, V. B. and Salkovskis, P. M. (2010). A qualitative study of the acceptability of an intensive format for the delivery of cognitive-behavioural therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, DOI: 10.1348/014466509×447055CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisson, J. I., Ehlers, A., Matthews, R., Pilling, S., Richards, D. and Turner, S. (2007). Psychological treatments for chronic post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 97104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman, F. D., Charney, D. S. and Keane, T. M. (1995). The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 7590.Google ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, E. B., Hickling, E. J., Devineni, T., Veazey, C. H., Galovski, T. E., Mundy, E. and Buckley, T. C. (2003). A controlled evaluation of cognitive behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress in motor vehicle accident survivors. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 421, 7996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, R., Greene, J., Russ, E., Dutra, L. and Westen, D. (2005). A multidimensional meta-analysis of psychotherapy for PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 214217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. M. (1996) Panic disorder: from theory to therapy. InSalkovskis, P. M. (Ed.), Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy (pp. 318344). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cloitre, M., Koenen, K., Cohen, L. R. and Han, H. (2002). Skills training in affective and interpersonal regulation followed by exposure: a phase-based treatment for PTSD related to childhood abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 10671074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Deacon, B. and Abramowitz, J. (2006). A pilot study of two-day cognitive behaviour therapy for panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 807817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, M, Gillespie, K. and Clark, D. M. (2007). Post-traumatic stress disorder in the context of terrorism and other civil conflict in Northern Ireland: randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal, doi:10.1136/bmj.39021.846852.BE (published 11 May).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M. and Ehlers, A. (2001). A prospective study of the role of cognitive factors in persistent posttraumatic stress disorder after physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 10631084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A. and Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F. and Fennell, M. (2005). Cognitive therapy for PTSD: development and evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 413431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., Fennell, M. and Grey, N. (in preparation). Cognitive Therapy for PTSD: a therapist's guide. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., Fennell, M., Herbert, C. and Mayou, R. (2003). A randomized controlled trial of cognitive therapy, self-help booklet, and repeated early assessment as early interventions for PTSD. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 10241032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A. and Michael, T. (2004). Intrusive reexperiencing in posttraumatic stress disorder: phenomenology, theory, and therapy. Memory, 12, 403415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., Steil, R., Clohessy, S., Wenninger, K. and Winter, H. (2002). The nature of intrusive memories after trauma: the warning signal hypothesis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 10211028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Maercker, A. and Boos, A. (2000). PTSD following political imprisonment: the role of mental defeat, alienation, and permanent change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 4555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A. and Bryant, B. (1998). Psychological predictors of chronic PTSD after motor vehicle accidents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 508519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M. and Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders – Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York: Biometrics Research Department of the New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B., Cashman, L., Jaycox, L. and Perry, K. (1997). The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychological Assessment, 9, 445451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E. B., Hembree, E. A., Cahill, S. P., Raunch, S. A. M., Riggs, D. S., Feeny, N. C. and Yadin, E. (2005). Randomized trial of prolonged exposure for post-traumatic stress disorder with and without cognitive restructuring: outcome at academic and community clinics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 953964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B., Keane, T. M., Friedman, M. J. and Cohen, J. A. (2005). Effective Treatments for PTSD: practice guidelines from the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B. and Rothbaum, B. O. (1998). Treating the Trauma of Rape. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for PTSD. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B., Zoellner, L. A., Feeny, N. C., Hembree, E. A. and Alvarez-Conrad, J. (2002). Does imaginal exposure exacerbate PTSD symptoms? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 10221028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillespie, K., Duffy, M., Hackmann, A. and Clark, D. M. (2002). Community based cognitive therapy in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder following the Omagh bomb. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 345357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grey, N., McManus, F., Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M. and Ehlers, A. (2009). Intensive cognitive therapy for PTSD: case studies. InGrey, N. (Ed.), A Casebook of Cognitive Therapy for Traumatic Stress Reactions (pp. 111130). Hove, UK: Brunner-Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., Speckens, A. and Clark, D. M. (2004). Characteristics and content of intrusive memories in PTSD and their changes with treatment. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 231240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahlweg, K., Fiegenbaum, W., Frank, M., Schröder, B. and Witzleben, I. (2001). Short- and long-term effectiveness of an empirically supported treatment for agoraphobia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 375382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hembree, E. A., Foa, E. B., Dorfan, N. M., Street, G. P., Kowalski, J. and Tu, X. (2003). Do patients drop out prematurely from exposure therapy for PTSD? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16, 555562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, S. G. and Smits, J. A. J. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 621632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilpatrick, D. G. and Best, C. L. (1984). Some cautionary remarks on treating sexual assault victims with implosion. Behavior Therapy, 15, 421423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Clinical Excellence (2005). Clinical Guideline 26: posttraumatic stress disorder: the management of PTSD in adults and children in primary and secondary care. London: National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health. http://guidance.nice.org/CG26.Google Scholar
Oldfield, V., Salkovskis, P. and Taylor, T. (in press). Time-intensive cognitive-behaviour therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a case series and matched comparison group. British Journal of Clinical Psychology.Google Scholar
Öst, L. G. (1989). One-session treatment for specific phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öst, L. G. (2008). Cognitive behavior therapy for anxiety disorders: 40 years of progress. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 62, 510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resick, P. A. and Schnicke, M. K. (1992). Cognitive processing therapy for sexual assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 748756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resick, P. and Schnicke, M. (1993). Cognitive Processing Therapy for Rape Victims: a treatment manual. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Resick, P. A., Nishith, P., Weaver, T. L., Astin, M. C. and Feuer, C. A. (2002). A comparison of cognitive-processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 867879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnurr, P. P., Friedman, M. J., Engel, C. C., Foa, E. B., Shea, M. T., Chow, B. K. R. P. A., Resick, P. A., Thurston, V., Orsillo, S. M., Haug, R., Turner, C. and Bernardy, N. (2007). Cognitive behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 28, 820830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P., Yule, W., Perrin, S., Tranah, T., Dalgleish, T. and Clark, D. M. (2007). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for PTSD in children and adolescents: a preliminary randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 10511061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, D. J., Cloitre, M., Nemeroff, C. B., Nutt, D. J., Seedat, S., Shalev, A. Y., Wittchen, H. U. and Zohar, J. (2009). Cape Town consensus on posttraumatic stress disorder. CNS Spectrums, 14 (Suppl 1), 5258.Google ScholarPubMed
Storch, E. A., Geffken, G. R., Merlo, L. J., Mann, G., Duke, D., Munson, M., Adkins, J., Grabill, K. M., Murphy, T. K. and Gooman, W. K. (2007). Family-based cognitive-behavioural therapy for pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder: comparison of intensive and weekly approaches. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 469478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarrier, N., Pilgrim, H., Sommerfield, C., Fragher, B., Reynolds, M., Graham, E. and Barrowclough, C. (1999). A randomized trial of cognitive therapy and imaginal exposure in the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 1318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Etten, M. L. and Taylor, S. (1998). Comparative efficacy of treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 5, 126144.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veterans Health Administration and Department of Defense (2004). VA/DoD Clinical Practice Guideline for the Management of Post-Traumatic Stress. Version 1.0. Washington, DC: Veterans Health Administration, Department of Defense. http://www.guideline.gov/summary/summary.aspx?ss=15anddoc_id=5187Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample description: N (%) or means (standard deviations)

Figure 1

Table 2. Example of course of treatment in intensive cognitive therapy for PTSD

Figure 2

Table 3. Treatment outcome measures for intensive Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (CT-PTSD) and comparison group receiving weekly CT-PTSD from Ehlers et al. (2005), means (standard deviations)

Figure 3

Figure 1. Comparison of outcome for case series of intensive Cognitive Therapy for PTSD (CT-PTSD) (INTENSIVE) with a comparison group receiving weekly CT-PTSD from Ehlers et al.'s (2005) randomized controlled trial (WEEKLY). PTSD symptom severity was measured with the Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS).

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.