Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T23:38:49.695Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The evolution of motivational interviewing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2023

William R. Miller*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA
*
Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This review traces the development of motivational interviewing (MI) from its happenstance beginnings and the first description published in this journal in 1983, to its continuing evolution as a method that is now in widespread practice in many professions, nations and languages. The efficacy of MI has been documented in hundreds of controlled clinical trials, and extensive process research sheds light on why and how it works. Developing proficiency in MI is facilitated by feedback and coaching based on observed practice after initial training. The author reflects on parallels between MI core processes and the characteristics found in 70 years of psychotherapy research to distinguish more effective therapists. This suggests that MI offers an evidence-based therapeutic style for delivering other treatments more effectively. The most common use of MI now is indeed in combination with other treatment methods such as cognitive behaviour therapies.

Type
Main
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction: happenstance beginnings

An inherent part of history is that as events are occurring, we do not know where they will lead. Seemingly chance encounters can have substantial enduring effects on the course of a life, a field, or a community (Bandura, Reference Bandura1982). Motivational interviewing began through such a convergence of happenstance events (Moyers, Reference Moyers2004).

Eugene, Oregon

In the 1970s when this journal was still called Behavioural Psychotherapy, I received my pre-doctoral training in the clinical psychology program at the University of Oregon, which was unabashedly behavioural in orientation. Even cognitive therapy was viewed with scepticism. Before learning and practising behaviour therapy we were required to complete a year-long course on how to talk with clients. As it happened that year, none of the behavioural faculty chose to teach the course, so from the counselling psychology faculty they engaged Susan Gilmore who had been mentored by Leona Tyler and thus was an academic grandchild of Carl Rogers. She taught us fundamentals of therapeutic relationship from a client-centred perspective, with particular focus on the skill of accurate empathy (Gilmore, Reference Gilmore1973).

Milwaukee, Wisconsin

Immediately following this course, my introduction to addiction treatment came in 1973 as a summer psychology intern at the Veterans’ Affairs Medical Center in Milwaukee. The internship director invited me to tour the treatment programs and choose where I would like to learn. On the in-patient alcohol unit the psychologist director, Robert Hall, asked me what I knew about alcoholism. ‘Nothing’, I confessed. But what had I learned in my graduate courses? Actually, I didn’t recall the topic ever coming up. ‘Well’, he advised, ‘this is one of the most common diagnoses you will encounter in practice. Come and learn’. I spent the summer there and knowing nothing, I used my rudimentary listening skills to learn from the patients who taught me about alcoholism. The experience stirred me to head upstream in search of ways to intervene earlier before alcohol causes the depth of suffering that I witnessed there. It shaped my dissertation (Miller, Reference Miller1978), and addiction treatment became a passionate focus of my career.

Albuquerque, New Mexico

Joining the clinical faculty at the University of New Mexico (UNM) in 1976, I began conducting clinical trials of behaviour therapies for alcohol use disorders. Having no extramural funding, I trained our own students to be the counsellors, teaching them both behaviour therapy and empathic listening. In one of these trials three supervisors independently rated nine therapists on both their adherence to behavioural procedures and their skill in accurate empathy using a scale developed by Carl Rogers’ research group (Truax and Carkhuff, Reference Truax and Carkhuff1967). To my surprise, therapist empathy while delivering behaviour therapy accounted for two-thirds of the variance in clients’ drinking outcomes (r=.82 at 6 months), an effect that persisted for two years after treatment (Miller and Baca, Reference Miller and Baca1983; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Taylor and West1980). In contrast, the various behavioural treatment procedures we were comparing did not differ from each other in efficacy. There seemed to be something important about the therapeutic relationship.

Hjellestad, Norway

These data followed me on a sabbatical leave in 1982 at the Hjellestad Clinic near Bergen, Norway, where I was invited to lecture on the behavioural treatment of alcohol problems. The clinic’s medical director asked whether I might also meet regularly with their psychotherapists, which I gladly did. I again offered training in both client-centred and behavioural methods, and they wanted to observe my own clinical style. They role-played clients whom they were finding challenging and I did my best to respond, at which point they did something that my American students rarely did: they interrupted me in the middle of demonstrating. ‘What are you thinking right now?’, ‘Why did you ask that particular question?’, ‘Of all the things the client said, how did you decide to reflect that?’. Their thoughtful questions evoked from me some decision rules that I seemed to be using but of which I had not been consciously aware. Together we were following in the footsteps of Carl Rogers whose client-centred approach arose not from a pre-existing theory but from the careful observation and analysis of clinical practice (Kirschenbaum, Reference Kirschenbaum2009; Miller and Moyers, Reference Miller and Moyers2017).

From those discussions I wrote an informal discussion paper describing an approach called motivational interviewing (MI) and I mailed it to several respected colleagues for comment, one of whom was Ray Hodgson in Wales. To my surprise, he replied that he wanted to publish it as an article in Behavioural Psychotherapy, which he edited at the time. I protested that I had no data, but he persisted and it became the first published description of MI (Miller, Reference Miller1983). Unbeknownst to me, one of the people whom Ray chose to review the manuscript was a young colleague named Steve Rollnick.

The drinker’s check-up

Returning to New Mexico and newly interested in motivation for change (Miller, Reference Miller1985) I began considering how this approach might be turned into a treatment method. To reach problem drinkers early, before they developed serious consequences, we decided to advertise a free ‘drinker’s check-up’ for anyone wondering whether their alcohol use might be harming them (Miller and Sovereign, Reference Miller, Sovereign, Løberg, Miller, Nathan and Marlatt1989). We developed a thorough check-up using measures sensitive to early adverse effects of alcohol use, providing individual feedback of these assessment results using an MI style. Many drinkers responded, and relative to a random waiting list control group, their alcohol use decreased significantly following the check-up (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Sovereign and Krege1988; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Benefield and Tonigan1993). This MI-based check-up model has since been adapted to address marijuana use (Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Roffman, Fearer, Williams and Burke2007; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Stephens, Towe, Banes and Roffman2015), marital distress (Morrill et al., Reference Morrill, Eubanks-Fleming, Harp, Sollenberger, Darling and Cordova2011), and children’s behaviour problems in families (Dishion et al., Reference Dishion, Connell, Weaver, Shaw, Gardner and Wilson2008; Dishion et al., Reference Dishion, Brennan, Shaw, McEachern, Wilson and Jo2014; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Dishion, Supplee, Gardner and Arnds2006). A computer-based adaptation of the check-up also proved to be effective (Hester et al., Reference Hester, Squires and Delaney2005; Walters et al., Reference Walters, Vader and Harris2007b).

Sydney, Australia

For a second sabbatical leave in 1989–90 I was invited to the National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre at the University of New South Wales. I found myself officed next to an affable South African from Wales named Steve Rollnick who was there to do a PhD thesis on brief MI with heavy drinkers in a hospital setting. ‘Are you the Miller who wrote that article on motivational interviewing?’ he asked. He had been teaching MI in the United Kingdom where it was already becoming a popular method in addiction treatment. ‘I don’t even know if I’m doing it right’, he said. ‘You need to write more about it’. Through a year of collaboration we developed the first edition of the MI text on ‘preparing people to change addictive behavior’ (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick1991). Steve brought fresh perspective and depth to the psychotherapeutic method, adding the concept of resolving ambivalence and using his practical experience from teaching MI. It has been our experience ever since that through conversation we arrive at clearer understanding and expression than either of us would have found alone. He soon began applying MI more broadly in health care, where ambivalence about behaviour change is a common obstacle (Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Heather and Bell1992; Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Mason and Butler1999).

Transtheoretical stages of change

In the early 1980s while MI was emerging, a transtheoretical model of change was also being developed by Prochaska and DiClemente (Reference Prochaska and DiClemente1984). MI had synergy with their model by offering an early evidence-based treatment method for working with clients who were not yet ready for change; i.e. in the pre-contemplation, contemplation and preparation stages (Prochaska and DiClemente, Reference Prochaska, DiClemente, Hersen, Eisler and Miller1992; Prochaska et al., Reference Prochaska, Velicer, Rossi, Goldstein, Marcus, Rakowski and Rossi1994). Resonance between MI and the stages of change was explored in writings from both research groups (DiClemente et al., Reference DiClemente, Corno, Graydon, Wiprovnick and Knoblach2017; DiClemente and Velasquez, Reference DiClemente, Velasquez, Miller and Rollnick2002; Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick1991) and MI found its way into Project MATCH.

Project MATCH

It has long been known that seemingly different psychotherapies typically yield similar outcomes when compared head-to-head in clinical trials (Imel et al., Reference Imel, Wampold, Miller and Fleming2008; Rosenzweig, Reference Rosenzweig1936; Wampold and Imel, Reference Wampold and Imel2015a). It is possible, however, that particular therapies may be most beneficial for different kinds of people, a client–treatment interaction that can be masked when averaging outcomes of therapies. This idea of matching treatments to clients was the central purpose of Project MATCH, the largest randomized clinical trial ever conducted with psychotherapies for alcohol use disorders (Babor and Del Boca, Reference Babor and Del Boca2003). In funding the trial, the U.S. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) did not specify the treatments to be studied. Instead, nine clinical sites including UNM were selected to participate based on applications describing how they might conduct the trial. Then in 1990 the nine principal investigators, a coordinating centre, and NIAAA staff began negotiating the eventual design of the multisite clinical trial (Project MATCH Research Group, 1993).

We first decided on the therapies to be tested. We determined that the design could accommodate three different treatment methods, and we wanted them to be quite different in theoretical orientation. Given the prior work of the investigators it was a foregone conclusion that one of the treatments would be cognitive behavioural (Kadden et al., Reference Kadden, Carroll, Donovan, Cooney, Monti, Abrams and Hester1992). Because a 12-step approach was prevalent in U.S. community treatment programs, we developed 12-step facilitation therapy as a second method to be compared (Nowinski and Baker, Reference Nowinski and Baker1998). What should be the third? We considered a variety of evidence-based treatments (Institute of Medicine, 1990; Miller and Hester, Reference Miller, Hester, Miller and Heather1986) and chose MI as being quite different conceptually from the other two. The research team was uneasy, however, that the usual 1–2 session duration of MI could be insufficient, given that the other two treatments would consist of 12 sessions. Could MI be made longer? We settled on a 4-session format of the drinker’s check-up and called it motivational enhancement therapy (MET; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Zweben, DiClemente and Rychtarik1992). As usual, the three treatments proved to be equally effective (Project MATCH Research Group, 1997).

The research group also developed detailed a priori predictions about which clients would fare better in each treatment, resulting in 18 matching hypotheses to be tested (Longabaugh and Wirtz, Reference Longabaugh and Wirtz2001). Most of these predictions were not confirmed; in fact seven yielded interactions in a direction opposite from what had been predicted. Bottom line: we are not very good at predicting which psychotherapy will be best for whom. One prediction that was confirmed is that angrier clients were more likely to benefit from MET (Waldron et al., Reference Waldron, Miller, Tonigan, Longabaugh and Wirtz2001). Subsequent analyses also found that MET was more effective for females (but not males) with lower initial motivation (Witkiewitz et al., Reference Witkiewitz, Hartzler and Donovan2010). In any event, Project MATCH did increase awareness of MI as a bona fide way of treating alcohol use disorders.

The spirit of MI

In our early MI training workshops Steve Rollnick and I focused on treatment techniques, giving particular emphasis to those for responding to resistance, a common interest of trainees. As we watched people practising what we had taught them, however, we were dismayed. It was as if they were trying to use the techniques on clients, trying to hoodwink them into changing. The words were there, but the music was wrong. The fault was ours, not theirs; clearly we had omitted something important in our training.

In this journal we described the missing element as the underlying spirit with which MI is practised (Rollnick and Miller, Reference Rollnick and Miller1995). Carl Rogers had much earlier described important attitudes underlying a client-centred approach: empathic understanding, unconditional positive regard, and genuineness (Rogers, Reference Rogers1957; Rogers, Reference Rogers and Koch1959). Such attitudes are expressed in clinician behaviour, of course; indeed they must be communicated in order to benefit clients. Both internal (attitude) and external (behaviour) components matter in therapeutic relationships (Miller and Moyers, Reference Miller and Moyers2021).

Our understanding of these attitudinal components of MI has evolved over time. In our second edition we briefly described three elements of the spirit of MI as collaboration, evocation and autonomy (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2002). Our third edition (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2013) contained a full chapter on the underlying spirit, describing partnership, acceptance and evocation, and adding compassion – a conscious commitment to the well-being and best interests of the client. We added this fourth component because some of the same techniques involved in MI can be used in self-serving ways, prompting us also to offer some guidelines on the ethical use of MI (Miller, Reference Miller1994).

Fostering change

The third edition of Motivational Interviewing included two important developments that are reflected in its subtitle: Helping People Change. Although our early expectation had been that MI would primarily be used in the contemplation (ambivalence) stage, practitioners found that they were not ‘letting go of’ MI once readiness for change emerged. Whereas our first two editions had focused on preparing people for change, it became clear that MI can be a more general way of working with clients (Rogers, Reference Rogers1980b) and useful throughout a helping process across a broad range of concerns.

A second development was to broaden the concept of change beyond behaviour. Although it is simplest to describe MI in relation to increasing or decreasing a particular behaviour like exercise or smoking, the same method can also be applied in promoting complex change that is not about specific behaviour except in the broadest sense of the term. An example that we used is forgiveness, a clearly important clinical issue (Worthington, Reference Worthington2003; Worthington, Reference Worthington2005). A choice to forgive may not result in overt behaviour change; the forgiven person may have been dead for decades, and the change is more cognitive and affective in focus (Wagner and Ingersoll, Reference Wagner and Ingersoll2008). We were also considering how MI can be applied in primary and secondary prevention outside the realm of treatment (Dishion et al., Reference Dishion, Brennan, Shaw, McEachern, Wilson and Jo2014; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Toscova, Miller and Sanchez2000).

Principles to processes

In the first two editions we described some general principles of MI, whereas in the third edition we jettisoned these in favour of four component tasks or processes of MI: engaging, focusing, evoking and planning. These may sound linear and sequential: first you engage, next you identify shared goals, then you evoke the client’s motivation to pursue those goals, and finally you develop a plan to reach them. In practice the processes are more iterative and flexible. The ‘how to’ discussion of planning can intermix with the ‘whether’ of evoking, and both can emerge quite early in consultation. Goals may change, and sometimes one needs to re-engage. The empathic clinical skills of engaging undergird all four processes, and evoking is still used to develop plans for change.

The language of change

Paul Amrhein’s psycholinguistic contributions to MI emerged between our second and third editions, informing how we understood and described change talk. Paul’s perspective was that our concept of change talk (as well as sustain talk) was too broad, mixing together a variety of speech acts that to a psycholinguist are quite different. There are verbs signalling desire for change (e.g. I want, wish, would like). Other words describe perceived ability or self-efficacy to change (e.g. I can, could, am able). Both desire and ability speech differ from giving reasons for change that have an if-then structure (e.g. If I do, then …; If I don’t, then…). Furthermore, there are words that bespeak a need or urgency for change without necessarily stating reasons, desire, or ability (e.g. I have to, need to, must). In English these four – Desire, Ability, Reasons and Need – formed the convenient acronym DARN.

Paul distinguished these preparatory (DARN) forms of speech from commitment language of implied promise (e.g. I will, I promise, I guarantee; Amrhein et al., Reference Amrhein, Miller, Yahne, Palmer and Fulcher2003). There are also near linguistic neighbours that are not actually promissory, but nevertheless signal an inclination toward action (e.g. I am willing, will consider, probably will). These are insufficient when taking an oath (e.g. Will you tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth?) or entering into a contract, but they still sound more promising than DARN speech and we termed these activation language. Yet another form of change talk emerged from coding MI sessions: taking steps (Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Martin, Christopher, Houck, Tonigan and Amrhein2007). Such statements indicate that the person has already done something that favours change; e.g. I filled the prescription, disposed of all the alcohol, got the book you recommended. Together Commitment, Activation and Taking Steps form the English acronym CATS and are what we call mobilizing change talk.

The very same forms of speech can augur against change as well, and we termed these sustain talk. Such statements bespeak desire, ability, reasons or need for and even a commitment to the status quo. In practice, change talk and sustain talk often come intermixed in the same sentence or paragraph (Miller, Reference Miller2022), and the ratio of change talk to sustain talk is an operational definition of ambivalence or decisional balance (Prochaska et al., Reference Prochaska, Velicer, Rossi, Goldstein, Marcus, Rakowski and Rossi1994). When directly compared, sustain talk tends to be a better predictor of outcome than change talk alone, and the ratio of change talk to sustain talk is related to the probability of subsequent change (Magill et al., Reference Magill, Apodaca, Borsari, Gaume, Hoadley, Gordon and Moyers2018). Sustain talk may be a particularly important predictor with certain populations, such as clients mandated or coerced into treatment. MI-consistent and -inconsistent counsellor responses do influence the balance of client change talk and sustain talk in correlational (Fischer and Moyers, Reference Fischer and Moyers2014; Magill et al., Reference Magill, Apodaca, Borsari, Gaume, Hoadley, Gordon and Moyers2018), sequential (Drage et al., Reference Drage, Masterson, Tober, Farragher and Bewick2019; Moyers and Martin, Reference Moyers and Martin2006; Walthers et al., Reference Walthers, Janssen, Mastroleo, Hoadley, Barnett, Colby and Magill2019), and experimental studies (Glynn and Moyers, Reference Glynn and Moyers2010; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Benefield and Tonigan1993).

These findings caused us to question the popular clinical technique of constructing a decisional balance by exploring the client’s pros and cons of change. If one equally evokes and reinforces client change talk and sustain talk, the expected outcome would be ambivalence and no change. Experimental trials show that doing a decisional balance intervention with people who are ambivalent tends to decrease their commitment to change (Miller and Rose, Reference Miller and Rose2015). Although MI is sometimes confused with decisional balance, MI involves preferentially evoking and reinforcing client change talk.

Resistance

Another significant development in our third edition was deconstruction of the concept of resistance. The word itself seems to blame the client for not changing via intentional obstruction or pathological processes. Theresa Moyers correctly pointed out to us that most of what we had been calling ‘resistance’ was simply sustain talk – just one side of a person’s ambivalence. Sustain talk is interpersonal behaviour that can be influenced by counsellor responses and is not pathological in itself (Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Houck, Glynn, Hallgren and Manual2017). This raised an interesting question: If we were to subtract sustain talk from what therapists mean by ‘resistance’, what is left? We called the remainder discord. Whereas sustain talk is about the change being considered, discord is about the therapeutic relationship itself and often contains the word ‘you’ (e.g. You’re not listening to me; You can’t tell me what to do).

Both of these components – sustain talk and discord – predict poor outcome if they persist or predominate. Both are interpersonal behaviour and highly responsive to counsellor style. ‘Resistance’ can be dialled up or down by the counsellor within a session, as demonstrated in A-B-A-B experimental studies (Glynn and Moyers, Reference Glynn and Moyers2010; Patterson and Forgatch, Reference Patterson and Forgatch1985).

Research on motivational interviewing

Clinical trial outcomes

As of 2022, more than 1900 controlled clinical trials involving MI have been described across a broad range of issues, and there are more than 200 published meta-analyses and systematic reviews of MI research. Although MI is now commonly found on lists of ‘evidence-based treatments’, a question like ‘Does it work?’ is far too simplistic. In terms of human change and growth, a better question would be ‘How much of what kind and quality of MI delivered by whom to whom in what context will improve what outcomes how much and for how long?’. Between two-thirds and three-quarters of clinical trials have reported significant benefits of MI, indicating that something about it does seem to be working.

At the same time, outcomes of MI are highly variable. The efficacy of MI differs across studies, by sites within multisite trials, and by counsellors within sites. Average effect sizes are in the small to medium range, again with substantial variability. Effects also vary across target problems and outcome measures. Such heterogeneity is not unique to MI – similar outcome variability is often found in psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy research – but it is certainly characteristic of MI studies.

There are also some patterns of findings. When smaller ‘doses’ of MI (for example, fewer sessions) are compared with other more extended interventions, outcomes are often comparable (e.g. Project MATCH Research Group, 1997; UKATT Research Team, 2005). This renders MI a reasonable baseline treatment with which to compare other psychotherapy or pharmacotherapy. However, when MI is added to other evidence-based interventions, outcomes are often improved or longer-lasting (Hettema et al., Reference Hettema, Steele and Miller2005; Westra et al., Reference Westra, Constantino and Antony2016). Efficacy can also vary over time. The earliest trials of MI for smoking cessation showed little promise; later studies, however, yielded sufficiently positive results to regard MI as an evidence-based component of treatment for tobacco smoking (Heckman et al., Reference Heckman, Egleston and Hofmann2010; Hettema and Hendricks, Reference Hettema and Hendricks2010). Although earlier studies tested MI alone, most clinical trials now use MI as one component of treatments being tested. Particularly common have been combinations of MI with cognitive behavioural therapies (Barrett et al., Reference Barrett, Begg, O’Halloran and Kingsley2018; Marker and Norton, Reference Marker and Norton2018; Naar and Safren, Reference Naar and Safren2017; Thurstone et al., Reference Thurstone, Hull, Timmerman and Emrick2017; Westra et al., Reference Westra, Constantino and Antony2016).

A further question is for whom MI does or doesn’t help. The only known contraindication to date is based on reports that MI can delay or diminish improvement for people who are already high in readiness or confidence for change (e.g. Kuerbis et al., Reference Kuerbis, Houser, Levak, Shao and Morgenstern2018; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Romano, Byrow, Gregory, McLellan, Brockveld, Baillie, Gaston and Rapee2019; Rohsenow et al., Reference Rohsenow, Monti, Martin, Colby, Myers, Gulliver, Brown, Mueller, Gordon and Abrams2004). The mismatch here may be using the wrong MI process; evoking is unlikely to benefit people who are already motivated for change. The planning process and proceeding directly to action seem more appropriate for such clients. MI typically benefits adults and older teens, but efficacy is unknown with younger children, for whom MI with their parents or caregivers can be effective (e.g. Arrow et al., Reference Arrow, Raheb and Miller2013; Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Li, Ho, Chan, Lam and Chung2022; Weinstein, Reference Weinstein2007).

Process research

MI process measures usually predict client outcomes. Various reliable measures are available to rate the fidelity/quality of MI from recordings or transcripts of sessions (Lane et al., Reference Lane, Huws-Thomas, Hood, Rollnick, Edwards and Robling2005; Madson and Campbell, Reference Madson and Campbell2006; Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Martin, Catley, Harris and Ahluwalia2003; Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Martin, Manuel, Hendrickson and Miller2005). Providers’ skillfulness in delivering MI accounts in part for variability in findings, with higher-quality MI yielding more positive outcomes (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2014).

Miller and Rose (Reference Miller and Rose2009) distinguished between relational and technical components of MI. The relational aspects derive primarily from client-centred counselling (Truax and Carkhuff, Reference Truax and Carkhuff1967) with particular emphasis on the skill of accurate empathy (Bohart et al., Reference Bohart, Elliott, Greenberg, Watson and Norcross2002; Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Bohart, Watson and Murphy2018). Research on the technical element of MI has focused primarily on evoking change talk, the client’s own expressed motivations for change (e.g. Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Moyers, Sussman, Smith, Rohrbach, Sun and Spruit-Metz2014; Bertholet et al., Reference Bertholet, Faouzi, Gmel, Gaume and Daeppen2010; Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Martin, Christopher, Houck, Tonigan and Amrhein2007). As mentioned earlier, client sustain talk (voiced arguments against change) appears to be a stronger (inverse) predictor of client outcomes than change talk alone, with the ratio of change talk to sustain talk capturing variance from both (Magill et al., Reference Magill, Gaume, Apodaca, Walthers, Mastroleo, Borsari and Longabaugh2014; Magill et al., Reference Magill, Apodaca, Borsari, Gaume, Hoadley, Gordon and Moyers2018; Magill et al., Reference Magill, Bernstein, Hoadley, Borsari, Apodaca, Gaume and Tonigan2019). Therapist skill in MI influences the ratio of change talk to sustain talk which mediates client outcomes (Glynn and Moyers, Reference Glynn and Moyers2010; Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Houck, Glynn, Hallgren and Manual2017; Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Martin, Houck, Christopher and Tonigan2009). In a randomized clinical trial comparing client-centred counselling with and without technical components of MI, the relational elements alone did not improve outcomes relative to a control condition whereas adding the technical components did (Sellman et al., Reference Sellman, Sullivan, Dore, Adamson and MacEwan2001). In a similar experimental trial, Morgenstern and colleagues (Reference Morgenstern, Kuerbis, Amrhein, Hail, Lynch and McKay2012) found that MI including the technical components yielded change more rapidly than did the relational elements alone, although by the end of treatment the outcomes were comparable in this and a subsequent trial (Morgenstern et al., Reference Morgenstern, Kuerbis, Houser, Levak, Amrhein, Shao and McKay2017).

Human coding of MI sessions is a time-intensive and expensive process, particularly with sequential analyses that require reliability at the level of individual responses. A recent development is automated computer coding based on voice recognition and machine learning technology to detect particular responses. An obvious advantage of this approach is that thousands of interviews can be coded within the same amount of time required for human coding of a few samples. Automated coding converges reasonably well with human expert coding at a session level for responses like questions, reflections, and giving information (Klonek et al., Reference Klonek, Quera and Kauffeld2015). Agreement is more difficult at the level of individual utterances but will likely improve with technology (Atkins et al., Reference Atkins, Steyvers, Imel and Smyth2014; Tanana et al., Reference Tanana, Hallgren, Imel, Atkins and Srikumar2016). Automated coding can be used to provide more economical feedback to interviewers on MI practice (Imel et al., Reference Imel, Pace, Soma, Tanana, Hirsch, Gibson, Georgiou, Narayanan and Atkins2019).

Teaching motivational interviewing

Training research

Enough studies have evaluated the effectiveness of MI training methods to warrant reviews and meta-analyses (Madson et al., Reference Madson, Loignon and Lane2009; Schwalbe et al., Reference Schwalbe, Oh and Zweben2014). Clinicians who are professionally trained in MI show significant improvement in practice behaviour (Barwick et al., Reference Barwick, Bennett, Johnson, McGowan and Moore2012; Schwalbe et al., Reference Schwalbe, Oh and Zweben2014). Workshop training alone without follow-up typically yields short-lived gains in MI skill that are too small to have much impact on client outcomes (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Staiger, Simpson, Best and Lubman2016; Miller and Mount, Reference Miller and Mount2001). Adding follow-up feedback and coaching based on observed practice can significantly improve the acquisition and retention of MI skills (Croffoot et al., Reference Croffoot, Bray, Black and Koerber2010; Fu et al., Reference Fu, Roth, Battaglia, Nelson, Farmer, Do and Zillich2015; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Yahne, Moyers, Martinez and Pirritano2004).

Training of trainers

Within 2 years of the first edition of Motivational Interviewing we were receiving enough requests for training that Steve Rollnick and I decided to begin offering an annual workshop to prepare MI trainers. For the initial 1993 workshop in New Mexico, we accepted 40 participants, several of whom would become prominent MI researchers and trainers. After 4 years, some who had completed this workshop asked whether they might gather informally to exchange ideas about how to teach MI, and the first such meeting convened in parallel with our 1997 workshop in Malta. Annual meetings began and led to the eventual formation of the MI Network of Trainers (MINT; www.motivationalinterviewing.org) that has continued offering workshops and prepared more than 3000 trainers in dozens of nations and languages. The COVID-19 pandemic required MINT to offer virtual training for trainer events in 2020 and 2021, and in the process discover the feasibility of this training format. Although there are pros and cons to virtual training, it does facilitate international participation without requiring travel.

Therapist effects

It is clear that client outcomes vary with the therapist providing MI (Project MATCH Research Group, 1998). This is not unique to MI, of course. Outcomes normally differ among therapists delivering treatment at the same agency, or even those following the same manual-guided therapy in a clinical trial (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Ogles, Patterson, Lambert and Vermeersch2009; Crits-Christoph et al., Reference Crits-Christoph, Baranackie, Kurcias and Beck1991; Erekson et al., Reference Erekson, Clayson, Park and Tass2020; Wampold and Bolt, Reference Wampold and Bolt2006; Wampold and Imel, Reference Wampold, Imel, Wampold and Imel2015b). Component skills of MI such as accurate empathy themselves predict improved outcomes in research on psychotherapy and counseling (Bohart et al., Reference Bohart, Elliott, Greenberg, Watson and Norcross2002; Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Bohart, Watson and Murphy2018; Truax and Carkhuff, Reference Truax and Carkhuff1967). Nor are such effects limited to psychotherapy; student outcomes vary by their teachers (Chetty et al., Reference Chetty, Friedman and Rockoff2014), surgical outcomes by physicians (Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, Efron, Canner, Dultz, Xu, Jones and Sakran2017), and athletes’ outcomes by coaches (Jowett, Reference Jowett2017).

In 2019 I set out with my colleague Theresa Moyers to review 70 years of psychotherapy research to discover characteristics of therapists whose clients have better (and worse) treatment outcomes. What is it that more effective therapists actually do regardless of their theoretical orientation? This is one of the most studied questions in psychotherapy research, dating to the very beginnings of clinical science in psychology (Kirschenbaum, Reference Kirschenbaum2009; Miller and Moyers, Reference Miller and Moyers2017; Truax and Carkhuff, Reference Truax and Carkhuff1967). We identified eight characteristics of more effective psychotherapists (Miller and Moyers, Reference Miller and Moyers2021). The strongest effects are found for accurate empathy – the effort and ability to understand what clients are telling you and to reflect that understanding back to them. Three other therapist characteristics highlighted by Carl Rogers (Reference Rogers and Koch1959, Reference Rogers1980b) are also linked to better treatment outcomes: expressing positive regard, therapist genuineness or authenticity in the helping relationship, and unconditional acceptance of people as they are. Better client outcomes have also been linked to therapists having a clear focus or goals, conveying and evoking hope, evoking clients’ own motivation and resources, and offering information and advice.

Looking back on this 2-year project, I was struck that at least seven of these eight characteristics of effective therapists have been central components of MI. Table 1 traces these parallels. Only genuineness has been given little consideration thus far in MI, and we mean to correct this in our fourth edition. This causes me to wonder whether what we have actually been studying, developing and teaching for four decades is the dynamics of therapeutic relationship. By far the usual use of MI is not instead of but in combination with other evidence-based treatment methods. MI has always been about relationship and is not allied with a comprehensive theory of behaviour or psychopathology (Atkinson and Woods, Reference Atkinson and Woods2017; Egizio et al., Reference Egizio, Smith, Wahab, Bennett, Payne and Hall2019). It can be and has been used in tandem with a broad range of other services including cognitive behaviour therapy (Naar and Safren, Reference Naar and Safren2017), diabetes education (Steinberg and Miller, Reference Steinberg and Miller2015), sports coaching (Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Fader, Breckon and Moyers2019), classroom teaching (Reinke et al., Reference Reinke, Herman and Sprick2011; Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Kaplan and Rutschman2016), social work (Forrester et al., Reference Forrester, Wilkins and Whittaker2021; Hohman, Reference Hohman2021), pastoral care (Miller and Jackson, Reference Miller and Jackson2010), preventive dentistry (Carlisle, Reference Carlisle2014), and working with offenders (Bradford and Nandi, Reference Bradford and Nandi2012; Stinson and Clark, Reference Stinson and Clark2017). To the practical know-how of such professions (e.g. in behavioural treatment manuals), MI can add evidence-based guidance for how to be in relationship with those whom you are serving. MI may also offer observable and learnable practices to try when implementing an aspirational humanistic approach such as servant leadership, where practical how-to guidelines have been sparse (Organ, Reference Organ2021).

Table 1. Parallels of motivational interviewing with characteristics of more effective psychotherapists

Continuing evolution

When someone asks me what I think will be happening with MI in 10 years I am reluctant to say. My reticence is related to how much evolution has occurred in the decade prior to each edition of our Motivational Interviewing text. As we completed each edition, we could not have accurately anticipated what we would be saying in the next. It has never been a matter of making minor tweaks to the prior edition. Each time we have almost completely rewritten the book, and that is happening again as we prepare a fourth edition anticipated for release in 2023.

Nevertheless, some future directions do seem to be emerging. Whereas MI began by focusing on addictive behaviour, it has now been applied and tested across a wide range of behavioural changes. Furthermore, we now understand MI as useful beyond specific behaviours, in helping people change and grow. The concept of ambivalence suggests pros and cons of a particular change, whereas human growth is more about a multiplicity of choices and the development of values. Applications are growing in education (Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Kaplan and Rutschman2016), life and health coaching (Clifford and Curtis, Reference Clifford and Curtis2016; Lawson and Wolever, Reference Lawson and Wolever2009), sports (Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Fader, Breckon and Moyers2019), organizational leadership (Marshall and Nielsen, Reference Marshall and Nielsen2020; Organ, Reference Organ2021; Wilcox et al., Reference Wilcox, Kersh and Jenkins2017), probation and parole (Stinson and Clark, Reference Stinson and Clark2017; Walters et al., Reference Walters, Clark, Gingerich and Meltzer2007a). MI also appears to cross cultures well, now being taught and practised in many languages and nations on six continents (Self et al., Reference Self, Borsari, Ladd, Nicolas, Gibson, Jackson and Manuelin press).

It is also clear that MI can be provided through media well beyond face-to-face conversations. It can be delivered remotely via telephone and other audio and virtual connections (e.g. Boccio et al., Reference Boccio, Sanna, Adams, Goler, Brown, Neugebauer and Schmittdiel2017; Cranney et al., Reference Cranney, O’Hara, Gale, Rissel, Bauman and Phongsavan2019; Gates et al., Reference Gates, Norberg, Copeland and Digiusto2012). Interactive journalling is a psychoeducational format that can be MI-informed (Miller, Reference Miller2014). More recently, MI-based text messaging is being evaluated (Mason et al., Reference Mason, Mennis, Way, Zaharakis, Campbell, Benotsch and King2016; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Spieker, Kripalani, Rothman, Roumie, Coco, Fabbri, Levy, Collins and McNaughton2021).

Across four decades of research, Carl Rogers and his colleagues developed person-centred counselling methods with wide applicability (Kirschenbaum, Reference Kirschenbaum2009). At first, his insights were presented as a rival therapeutic approach to compete with psychodynamic and behavioural treatment methods. His final book, however, described a person-centred approach as ‘a way of being’ with broad applicability in education, peacemaking, politics and social change (Rogers, Reference Rogers1980b). He was concerned about vital relational aspects such as empathy being misunderstood and reduced to techniques like reflective listening (Rogers, Reference Rogers and Rogers1980a).

Our own journey with MI has been similar. We early discovered the hazards of teaching techniques that could be used on or done to people, and soon we were emphasizing the mindset and heartset with which it is practised for and with people. Our understanding of evoking has expanded to empowerment – helping people to realize and use what they already have, their own wisdom and strengths. As discussed earlier, MI appears to be applicable far beyond the world of counselling and psychotherapy. We hope that MI will help to further humanize services for those who receive and those who provide them (Douaihy and Driscoll, Reference Douaihy and Driscoll2018; Gordon and Edwards, Reference Gordon and Edwards1997; Rakel, Reference Rakel2018; Roberts, Reference Roberts2001).

From such a soaring view, MI might be misunderstood as an abstract aspirational philosophy. It never has been. Like the work of Carl Rogers, it has from the very beginning been rooted in the close observation of practice and the testing of specific hypotheses that can be replicated by others. Good practice of MI is specifiable, and at least in psychotherapy research, well-trained observers can reliably agree on the quality of MI being delivered within a particular sample. Over a thousand clinical trials demonstrate its beneficial impact.

Acquiring MI skillfulness does involve practising observable behaviours that predict positive outcomes, and one could likewise misunderstand MI as nothing more than these specific responses. I believe, though, that there is a deeper level of learning that comes with the practice of a person-centred way of helping such as MI. It is as though practising the skills teaches you the relational spirit. Reflective listening brings a deeper understanding of others’ inner worlds. Affirmations become sincere appreciation of people’s strengths. Practising acceptance fosters a calm presence that is at peace with one’s own and others’ shortcomings. Such changes are less readily observable than specific practice behaviours, and perhaps we can be at peace with that as well while MI continues to evolve in unanticipated ways.

Some reflections for clinical science

What scientific lessons might be learned from the evolution of MI? One is surely the value of directly observing and learning from clinical practice. Indeed, science begins with careful observation. The major scientific contributions of Carl Rogers to psychotherapy did not begin from a theory but from paying close attention to clinical practice, moving through abductive reasoning toward provisional hypotheses (Kirschenbaum, Reference Kirschenbaum2009; Miller and Moyers, Reference Miller and Moyers2017). MI similarly emerged from observing and asking testable questions about clinical experience. This is the empirical context of discovery that informs the twin context of justification in science (Reichenbach, Reference Reichenbach1938), serving as a counterpoint to the hypothetico-deductive approach and binary significance testing that are often more highly prized in psychology.

It can be valuable, then, to bring a beginner’s mind to the observation of clinical practice, seeking fresh perspectives and hypotheses that may be missed when viewing data through a priori lenses. Be open and curious rather than defensive when your predictions are not confirmed. Unexpected findings have been and will continue to be important in understanding MI.

Don’t stray too far from direct observation of practice. Developing reliable systems for observing therapy facilitated learning both in MI and in the client-centred approach from which it evolved (Truax and Carkhuff, Reference Truax and Carkhuff1967). My first impression was that coding MI interviews would be deadly dull, but in fact it is a way to see therapeutic processes unfolding in real time. Process coding of interviews seems to be a boon in developing skillfulness in MI. Deliberate practice with direct observation and shaping of skills can be used in training and supervision (Rousmaniere et al., Reference Rousmaniere, Goodyear, Miller and Wampold2017; Westra et al., Reference Westra, Norouzian, Poulin, Coyne, Constantino, Hara, Olson and Antony2021).

Run new ideas by colleagues who differ in cultural background and professional training. MI first emerged from the questions and observations of Norwegian practitioners. Carl Rogers suggested circulating novel insights in a ‘discussion paper’ to invite comments, which is how the first published description of MI happened (Miller, Reference Miller1983). MI has been enriched by the insights of colleagues with expertise in anthropology (Carr, Reference Carrin press), psycholinguistics (Amrhein, Reference Amrhein2004), and evolutionary psychology (de Almeida Neto, Reference de Almeida Neto2017).

Introspection has a long history as an investigational method in psychology, particularly in the development of hypotheses and theory (Danziger, Reference Danziger1980; Weger et al., Reference Weger, Wagemann and Tewes2019). A ‘think-aloud’ method asks people to articulate their thoughts while engaged in a simulated situation, or while reviewing recorded sessions (Davison et al., Reference Davison, Vogel and Coffman1997). That is precisely what my Norwegian colleagues asked me to do while demonstrating counselling, and it yielded new testable hypotheses that led to the development of MI. I recommend thinking aloud as a tool in the context of discovery when studying psychotherapy.

Finally, I can commend William of Occam’s parsimonious principle that entities should not be multiplied beyond necessity. There are hundreds of brand-name psychotherapies that are allegedly different and advantageous. Yet when compared directly their average outcomes are usually similar, with large differences depending on who provided the treatment. We are also fairly clueless in predicting who will benefit most from allegedly different psychotherapies. In writing our fourth edition of Motivational Interviewing we are seeking simplicity on the far side of complexity. Rather than continuing to proliferate jargon and brand names, perhaps we can pursue common and complementary principles of behaviour change.

Data availability statement

No data are presented in this article.

Acknowledgements

Author contributions

William R. Miller: Conceptualization (lead), Writing – original draft (lead), Writing – review & editing (lead).

Financial support

The author’s research reported in this review was supported by grants from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (K05-AA00133, T15-SP07540, T32-AA07460, U10-AA08435, U10-AA11716), the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01-DA09864, R01-DA13081, U10-DA015833), the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (049533), and the U.S. Department of Education, Fund for the Improvement of Post-Secondary Education (G008730491).

Conflicts of interest

The author receives royalties from Guilford Press, Psychwire.com, and also from The Change Companies where he serves as a senior consultant.

Ethical standards

The author has abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS. Ethical review and approval of research were conducted by the Human Research Review committees of the University of New Mexico.

References

Amrhein, P. C. (2004). How does motivational interviewing work? What client talk reveals. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 18, 323336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amrhein, P. C., Miller, W. R., Yahne, C. E., Palmer, M., & Fulcher, L. (2003). Client commitment language during motivational interviewing predicts drug use outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 862878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, T., Ogles, B. M., Patterson, C. L., Lambert, M. J., & Vermeersch, D. A. (2009). Therapist effects: facilitative interpersonal skills as a predictor of therapist success. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 755768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arrow, P., Raheb, J., & Miller, M. (2013). Brief oral health promotion intervention among parents of young children to reduce early childhood dental decay. BMC Public Health, 13, 245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkins, D. C., Steyvers, M., Imel, Z. E., & Smyth, P. (2014). Scaling up the evaluation of psychotherapy: evaluating motivational interviewing fidelity via statistical text classification. Implementation Science, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, C., & Woods, K. (2017). Establishing theoretical stability and treatment integrity for motivational interviewing. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy, 45, 337350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babor, T. F., & Del Boca, F. K. (eds). (2003). Treatment Matching in Alcoholism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1982). The psychology of chance encounters and life paths. American Psychologist, 37, 747755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, E., Moyers, T. B., Sussman, S., Smith, C., Rohrbach, L. A., Sun, P., & Spruit-Metz, D. (2014). From counselor skill to decreased marijuana use: does change talk matter? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 46, 498505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, S., Begg, S., O’Halloran, P., & Kingsley, M. (2018). Integrated motivational interviewing and cognitive behaviour therapy can increase physical activity and improve health of adult ambulatory care patients in a regional hospital: the Healthy4U randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barwick, M. A., Bennett, L. M., Johnson, S. N., McGowan, J., & Moore, J. E. (2012). Training health and mental health professionals in motivational interviewing: a systematic review. Children and Youth Services Review, 34, 17861795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertholet, N., Faouzi, M., Gmel, G., Gaume, J., & Daeppen, J. B. (2010). Change talk sequence during brief motivational intervention, towards or away from drinking. Addiction, 105, 21062112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boccio, M., Sanna, R. S., Adams, S. R., Goler, N. C., Brown, S. D., Neugebauer, S., … & Schmittdiel, J. A. (2017). Telephone-based coaching: a comparison of tobacco cessation programs in an integrated health care system. American Journal of Health Promotion, 31, 136142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohart, A. C., Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., & Watson, J. C. (2002). Empathy. In Norcross, J. C. (ed), Psychotherapy Relationships That Work (pp. 89108). New York, USA: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bradford, B., & Nandi, A. (2012). Motivational Interviewing in Corrections: A Comprehensive Guide to Implementing MI in Corrections. National Institute of Corrections, U.S. Department of Justice.Google Scholar
Carlisle, L. (2014). Motivational Interviewing in Dentistry: Helping People Become Healthier. Carbondale, CO: Adjuvant New Media.Google Scholar
Carr, E. S. (in press). Working the Difference: Science, Spirit and the Spread of Motivational Interviewing. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chetty, R., Friedman, J. N., & Rockoff, J. E. (2014). Measuring the impacts of teachers II: teacher value-added and student outcomes in adulthood. American Economic Review, 104, 26332679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, A. T., Li, W. H. C., Ho, L. L. K., Chan, G. C.-F., Lam, H. S., & Chung, J. O. K. (2022). Efficacy of mobile instant messaging-delivered brief motivational interviewing for parents to promote physical activity in pediatric cancer survivors: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Network Open, 5, e2214600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clifford, D., & Curtis, L. (2016). Motivational Interviewing in Nutrition and Fitness. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cranney, L., O’Hara, B., Gale, J., Rissel, C., Bauman, A., & Phongsavan, P. (2019). Telephone based coaching for adults at risk of diabetes: impact of Australia’s Get Healthy Service. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 9, 11781185.Google ScholarPubMed
Crits-Christoph, P., Baranackie, K., Kurcias, J. S., & Beck, A. T. (1991). Meta-analysis of therapist effects in psychotherapy outcome studies. Psychotherapy Research, 1, 8191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croffoot, C., Bray, K. K., Black, M. A., & Koerber, A. (2010). Evaluating the effects of coaching to improve motivational interviewing skills of dental hygiene students. Journal of Dental Hygiene, 84, 5764.Google ScholarPubMed
Danziger, K. (1980). The history of introspection reconsidered. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 16, 241262.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davison, G. C., Vogel, R. S., & Coffman, S. G. (1997). Think-aloud approaches to cognitive assessment and the articulated thoughts in simulated situations paradigm. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 950958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Almeida Neto, A. C. (2017). Understanding motivational interviewing: an evolutionary perspective. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 3, 379389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiClemente, C. C., Corno, C. M., Graydon, M. M., Wiprovnick, A. E., & Knoblach, D. J. (2017). Motivational interviewing, enhancement, and brief interventions over the last decade: a review of reviews of efficacy and effectiveness. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31, 862887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiClemente, C. C., & Velasquez, M. M. (2002). Motivational interviewing and the stages of change. In Miller, W. R. & Rollnick, S. (eds), Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for Change. (2nd edn, pp. 201216). New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Dishion, T. J., Brennan, L. M., Shaw, D. S., McEachern, A. D., Wilson, M. N., & Jo, B. (2014). Prevention of problem behavior through annual family check-ups in early childhood: Intervention effects from home to early elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 42, 343354.Google ScholarPubMed
Dishion, T. J., Connell, A., Weaver, C., Shaw, D., Gardner, F., & Wilson, M. (2008). The family check-up with high-risk indigent families: preventing problem behavior by increasing parents’ positive behavior support in early childhood. Child Development, 79, 13951414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douaihy, A., & Driscoll, H. P. (2018). Humanizing Addiction Practice: Blending Science and Personal Transformation. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drage, L., Masterson, C., Tober, G., Farragher, T., & Bewick, B. (2019). The impact of therapists’ responses to resistance to change: a sequential analysis of therapist–client interactions in motivational interviewing. Alcohol & Alcoholism, 54, 173176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egizio, L. L., Smith, D. C., Wahab, S., & Bennett, K. (2019). Motivational interviewing’s theory of practice for social work: promises and pitfalls. In Payne, M. & Hall, E. R. (eds), Handbook for Social Work Theory (pp. 236246). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Elliott, R., Bohart, A. C., Watson, J. C., & Murphy, D. (2018). Therapist empathy and client outcome: an updated meta-analysis. Psychotherapy, 55, 399410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erekson, D. M., Clayson, R., Park, S. Y., & Tass, S. (2020). Therapist effects on early change in psychotherapy in a naturalistic setting. Psychotherapy Research, 30, 6878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, D. J., & Moyers, T. B. (2014). Is there an association between empathic speech and change talk in motivational interviewing sessions? Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 32, 318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrester, D., Wilkins, D., & Whittaker, C. (2021). Motivational Interviewing for Working with Children and Families: A Practical Guide for Early Intervention and Child Protection. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publications.Google Scholar
Fu, S. S., Roth, C., Battaglia, C. T., Nelson, D. B., Farmer, M. M., Do, T., … & Zillich, A. J. (2015). Training primary care clinicians in motivational interviewing: a comparison of two models. Patient Education and Counseling, 98, 6168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gates, P. J., Norberg, M. M., Copeland, J., & Digiusto, E. (2012). Randomized controlled trial of a novel cannabis use intervention delivered by telephone. Addiction, 107, 21492158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilmore, S. K. (1973). The Counselor-in-Training. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Glynn, L. H., & Moyers, T. B. (2010). Chasing change talk: the clinician’s role in evoking client language about change. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 39, 6570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, T., & Edwards, W. S. (1997). Making the Patient your Partner: Communication Skills for Doctors and Other Caregivers. New York, USA: Auburn House Paperback.Google Scholar
Hall, K., Staiger, P. K., Simpson, A., Best, D., & Lubman, D. I. (2016). After 30 years of dissemination, have we achieved sustained practice change in motivational interviewing? Addiction, 111, 11441150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckman, C. J., Egleston, B. L., & Hofmann, M. T. (2010). Efficacy of motivational interviewing for smoking cessation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Tobacco Control, 19, 410416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hester, R. K., Squires, D. D., & Delaney, H. D. (2005). The Drinker’s Check-Up: 12-month outcomes of a controlled clinical trial of a stand-alone software program for problem drinkers. Journal of Substance Abuse, 28, 159169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hettema, J., Steele, J., & Miller, W. R. (2005). Motivational interviewing. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 91111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hettema, J. E., & Hendricks, P. S. (2010). Motivational interviewing for smoking cessation: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 868884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohman, M. (2021). Motivational Interviewing in Social Work Practice (2nd edn). New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Imel, Z. E., Pace, B. T., Soma, C. S., Tanana, M., Hirsch, T., Gibson, J., Georgiou, P., Narayanan, S., & Atkins, D. C. (2019). Design feasibility of an automated, machine-learning based feedback system for motivational interviewing. Psychotherapy, 56, 318328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imel, Z. E., Wampold, B. E., Miller, S. D., & Fleming, R. R. (2008). Distinctions without a difference: direct comparisons of psychotherapies for alcohol use disorders. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22, 533543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Institute of Medicine (1990). Broadening the Base of Treatment for Alcohol Problems. Washington, D.C., USA: National Academies Press (U.S.) Google Scholar
Jowett, S. (2017). Coaching effectiveness: the coach–athlete relationship at its heart. Current Opinion in Psychology, 16, 154158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kadden, R., Carroll, K., Donovan, D., Cooney, N., Monti, P., Abrams, D., … & Hester, R. (1992). Cognitive-Behavioral Coping Skills Therapy Manual (Vol. 3). Rockville, MD, USA: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.Google Scholar
Kirschenbaum, H. (2009). The Life and Work of Carl Rogers. Alexandria, VA, USA: American Counseling Association.Google Scholar
Klonek, F. E., Quera, V., & Kauffeld, S. (2015). Coding interactions in motivational interviewing with computer-software: what are the advantages for process researchers? Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 284292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuerbis, A., Houser, J., Levak, S., Shao, S., & Morgenstern, J. (2018). Exploration of treatment matching of problem drinker characteristics to motivational interviewing and non-directive client-centered psychotherapy. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 86, 916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, C., Huws-Thomas, M., Hood, K., Rollnick, S., Edwards, K., & Robling, M. (2005). Measuring adaptations of motivational interviewing: the development and validation of the behavior change counseling index (BECCI). Patient Education and Counseling, 56, 166173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawson, K., & Wolever, R. (2009). Health Coaching for Behavior Change: Motivational Interviewing Methods and Practice. Manasquan, NJ, USA: Healthcare Intelligence Network.Google Scholar
Longabaugh, R., & Wirtz, P. W. (eds) (2001). Project MATCH Hypotheses: Results and Causal Chain Analyses (Project MATCH Monograph Series, Vol. 8). Bethesda, MD, USA: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.Google Scholar
Madson, M. B., & Campbell, T. C. (2006). Measures of fidelity in motivational enhancement: a systematic review. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 31, 6773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madson, M. B., Loignon, A. C., & Lane, C. (2009). Training in motivational interviewing: a systematic review. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 36, 101109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magill, M., Apodaca, T. R., Borsari, B., Gaume, J., Hoadley, A., Gordon, R. E. F., … & Moyers, T. (2018). A meta-analysis of motivational interviewing process: technical, relational, and conditional process models of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86, 140157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magill, M., Bernstein, M. H., Hoadley, A., Borsari, B., Apodaca, T. R., Gaume, J., & Tonigan, J. S. (2019). Do what you say and say what you are going to do: a preliminary meta-analysis of client change and sustain talk subtypes in motivational interviewing. Psychotherapy Research, 29, 860869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magill, M., Gaume, J., Apodaca, T. R., Walthers, J., Mastroleo, N. R., Borsari, B., & Longabaugh, R. (2014). The technical hypothesis of motivational interviewing: a meta-analysis of MI’s key causal model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82, 973983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marker, I., & Norton, P. J. (2018). The efficacy of incorporating motivational interviewing to cognitive behavior therapy for anxiety disorders: a review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 62, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, C., & Nielsen, A. S. (2020). Motivational Interviewing for Leaders in the Helping Professions: Facilitating Change in Organizations. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mason, M., Mennis, J., Way, T., Zaharakis, N., Campbell, L. F., Benotsch, E. G., … & King, L. (2016). Text message delivered peer network counseling for adolescent smokers: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Primary Prevention, 37, 403420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehta, A., Efron, D. T., Canner, J. K., Dultz, L., Xu, T., Jones, C., … & Sakran, J. V. (2017). Effect of surgeon and hospital volume on emergency general surgery outcomes. Journal of the American College of Surgeons, 225, 666675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R. (1978). Behavioral treatment of problem drinkers: a comparative outcome study of three controlled drinking therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 7486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R. (1983). Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 11, 147172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R. (1985). Motivation for treatment: a review with special emphasis on alcoholism. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 84107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R. (1994). Motivational interviewing: III. On the ethics of motivational intervention. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22, 111123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R. (2014). Interactive journaling as a clinical tool. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 36, 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R. (2022). On Second Thought: How Ambivalence Shapes Your Life. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Baca, L. M. (1983). Two-year follow-up of bibliotherapy and therapist-directed controlled drinking training for problem drinkers. Behavior Therapy, 14, 441448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., Benefield, R. G., & Tonigan, J. S. (1993). Enhancing motivation for change in problem drinking: a controlled comparison of two therapist styles. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 455461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., & Hester, R. K. (1986). The effectiveness of alcoholism treatment: what research reveals. In Miller, W. R. & Heather, N. (eds), Treating Addictive Behaviors: Processes of Change (pp. 121174). New York, USA: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Jackson, K. A. (2010). Practical Psychology for Pastors (2nd edn). Eugene, OR, USA: Wipf & Stock.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Mount, K. A. (2001). A small study of training in motivational interviewing: does one workshop change clinician and client behavior? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 457471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Moyers, T. B. (2017). Motivational interviewing and the clinical science of Carl Rogers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85, 757766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., & Moyers, T. B. (2021). Effective Psychotherapists: Clinical Skills that Improve Client Outcomes. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People to Change Addictive Behavior. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for Change (2nd edn). New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2013). Motivational Interviewing: Helping People Change (3rd edn). New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2014). The effectiveness and ineffectiveness of complex behavioral interventions: impact of treatment fidelity. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 37, 234241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., & Rose, G. S. (2009). Toward a theory of motivational interviewing. American Psychologist, 64, 527537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rose, G. S. (2015). Motivational interviewing and decisional balance: contrasting responses to client ambivalence. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 43, 129141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., & Sovereign, R. G. (1989). The check-up: a model for early intervention in addictive behaviors. In Løberg, T., Miller, W. R., Nathan, P. E., & Marlatt, G. A. (eds), Addictive Behaviors: Prevention and Early Intervention (pp. 219231). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., Sovereign, R. G., & Krege, B. (1988). Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers: II. The Drinker’s Check-Up as a preventive intervention. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 16, 251268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., Taylor, C. A., & West, J. (1980). Focused versus broad-spectrum behavior therapy for problem drinkers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 590601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., Toscova, R. T., Miller, J. H., & Sanchez, V. (2000). A theory-based motivational approach for reducing alcohol/drug problems in college. Health Education & Behavior, 27, 744759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., Yahne, C. E., Moyers, T. B., Martinez, J., & Pirritano, M. (2004). A randomized trial of methods to help clinicians learn motivational interviewing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 10501062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., Zweben, A., DiClemente, C., & Rychtarik, R. (1992). Motivational Enhancement Therapy Manual: A Clinical Research Guide for Therapist Treating Individuals with Alcohol Abuse and Dependence (Vol. 2). Bethesda, MD, USA: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, J., Kuerbis, A., Amrhein, P., Hail, L., Lynch, K., & McKay, J. R. (2012). Motivational interviewing: a pilot test of active ingredients and mechanisms of change. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26, 859869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgenstern, J., Kuerbis, A., Houser, J., Levak, S., Amrhein, P., Shao, S., & McKay, J. R. (2017). Dismantling motivational interviewing: effects on initiation of behavior change among problem drinkers seeking treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31, 751762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrill, M. I., Eubanks-Fleming, C. J., Harp, A. G., Sollenberger, J. W., Darling, E. V., & Cordova, J. V. (2011). The marriage check-up: increasing access to marital health care. Family Process, 50, 471485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyers, T. B. (2004). History and happenstance: how motivational interviewing got its start. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 18, 291298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyers, T. B., Houck, J. M., Glynn, L. H., Hallgren, K. A., & Manual, J. K. (2017). A randomized controlled trial to influence client language in substance use disorder treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 172, 4350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., & Martin, T. (2006). Therapist influence on client language during motivational interviewing sessions. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 30, 245252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., Martin, T., Catley, D., Harris, K. J., & Ahluwalia, J. S. (2003). Assessing the integrity of motivational interventions: reliability of the Motivational Interviewing Skills Code. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 31, 177184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyers, T. B., Martin, T., Christopher, P. J., Houck, J. M., Tonigan, J. S., & Amrhein, P. C. (2007). Client language as a mediator of motivational interviewing efficacy: where is the evidence? Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 31 (suppl), 40S47S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., Martin, T., Houck, J. M., Christopher, P. J., & Tonigan, J. S. (2009). From in-session behaviors to drinking outcomes: a causal chain for motivational interviewing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 11131124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., Martin, T., Manuel, J. K., Hendrickson, S. M. L., & Miller, W. R. (2005). Assessing competence in the use of motivational interviewing. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 28, 1926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naar, S., & Safren, S. A. (2017). Motivational Interviewing and CBT: Combining Strategies for Maximum Effectiveness. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, L. A., Spieker, A. J., Kripalani, S., Rothman, R. L., Roumie, C. L., Coco, J., Fabbri, D., Levy, P., Collins, S. P., & McNaughton, C. D. (2021). User preferences for and engagement with text messages to support antihypertensive medication adherence: findings from a pilot study evaluating an emergency department-based behavioral intervention. Patient Education and Counseling, 105(6), 16061613. CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowinski, J., & Baker, S. (1998). The Twelve-Step Facilitation Handbook: A Systematic Approach to Early Recovery from Alcoholism and Addiction. San Francisco, CA, USA: Jossey Bass.Google Scholar
Organ, J. N. (2021). Motivational interviewing: a tool for servant leadership. International Journal of Servant Leadership, 15, 209234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, G. R., & Forgatch, M. S. (1985). Therapist behavior as a determinant for client noncompliance: a paradox for the behavior modifier. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 846851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, L., Romano, M., Byrow, Y., Gregory, B., McLellan, L. F., Brockveld, K., Baillie, A., Gaston, J., & Rapee, R. M. (2019). Motivational interviewing prior to cognitive behavioural treatment for social anxiety disorder: a randomised controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 256, 7078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1984). The Transtheoretical Approach: Crossing Traditional Boundaries of Therapy. Homewood, IL, USA: Dow/Jones Irwin.Google Scholar
Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1992). Stages of change in the modification of problem behaviors. In Hersen, M., Eisler, R. M., & Miller, P. M. (eds), Progress in Behavior Modification (pp. 184212). Sycamore, IL, USA: Sycamore Publishing.Google Scholar
Prochaska, J. O., Velicer, W. F., Rossi, J. S., Goldstein, M. G., Marcus, B. H., Rakowski, W., … & Rossi, S. R. (1994). Stages of change and decisional balance for 12 problem behaviors. Health Psychology, 13, 3946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Project MATCH Research Group (1993). Project MATCH: rationale and methods for a multisite clinical trial matching patients to alcoholism treatment. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 17, 11301145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Project MATCH Research Group (1997). Matching alcoholism treatments to client heterogeneity: Project MATCH posttreatment drinking outcomes. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 58, 729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Project MATCH Research Group (1998). Therapist effects in three treatments for alcohol problems. Psychotherapy Research, 8, 455474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rakel, D. (2018). The Compassionate Connection: The Healing Power of Empathy and Mindful Listening. New York, USA: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Reichenbach, H. (1938). Experience and Prediction. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Reinke, W. M., Herman, K. C., & Sprick, R. (2011). Motivational Interviewing for Effective Classroom Management: The Classroom Check-Up. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, M. (2001). Horse Sense for People. Toronto, Canada: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. In Koch, S. (ed), Psychology: The Study of a Science. Vol. 3. Formulations of the Person and the Social Contexts (pp. 184256). New York, USA: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1980a). Empathic: an unappreciated way of being. In Rogers, C. R. (ed), A Way of Being (pp. 137163). New York, USA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1980b). A Way of Being. Boston, USA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Rohsenow, D. J., Monti, P. M., Martin, R. A., Colby, S. M., Myers, M. G., Gulliver, S. B., Brown, R. A., Mueller, T. I., Gordon, A., & Abrams, D. B. (2004). Motivational enhancement and coping skills training for cocaine abusers: effects on substance use outcomes. Addiction, 99, 862874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rollnick, S., Fader, J. S., Breckon, J., & Moyers, T. B. (2019). Coaching Athletes to be their Best: Motivational Interviewing in Sports. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rollnick, S., Heather, N., & Bell, A. (1992). Negotiating behaviour change in medical settings: the development of brief motivational interviewing. Journal of Mental Health, 1, 2537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rollnick, S., Kaplan, S. G., & Rutschman, R. (2016). Motivational Interviewing in Schools: Conversations to Improve Behavior and Learning. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rollnick, S., Mason, P., & Butler, C. (1999). Health Behavior Change: A Guide for Practitioners. New York, USA: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Rollnick, S., & Miller, W. R. (1995). What is motivational interviewing? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 325334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. (1936). Some implicit common factors in diverse methods of psychotherapy. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 6, 412415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousmaniere, T., Goodyear, R. K., Miller, S. D., & Wampold, B. E. (2017). The Cycle of Excellence: Using Deliberate Practice to Improve Supervision and Training. John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwalbe, C. S., Oh, H. Y., & Zweben, A. (2014). Sustaining motivational interviewing: a meta-analysis of training studies. Addiction, 109, 12871294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Self, K. J., Borsari, B., Ladd, B. O., Nicolas, G., Gibson, C. J., Jackson, K., & Manuel, J. K. (in press). Cultural adaptations of motivational interviewing: a systematic review. Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Sellman, J. D., Sullivan, P. F., Dore, G. M., Adamson, S. J., & MacEwan, I. (2001). A randomized controlled trial of motivational enhancement therapy (MET) for mild to moderate alcohol dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62, 389396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Supplee, L. H., Gardner, F., & Arnds, K. (2006). Randomized trial of a family-centered approach to the prevention of early conduct problems: 2-year effects of the family check-up in early childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, M. P., & Miller, W. R. (2015). Motivational Interviewing in Diabetes Care. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stephens, R. S., Roffman, R. A., Fearer, S. A., Williams, C., & Burke, R. S. (2007). The Marijuana Check-Up: promoting change in ambivalent marijuana users. Addiction, 102, 947957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stinson, J. D., & Clark, M. D. (2017). Motivational Interviewing with Offenders: Engagement, Rehabilitation, and Reentry. New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tanana, M., Hallgren, K. A., Imel, Z. E., Atkins, D. C., & Srikumar, V. (2016). A comparison of natural language processing methods for automated coding of motivational interviewing. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 65, 4350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thurstone, C., Hull, M., Timmerman, J., & Emrick, C. (2017). Development of a motivational interviewing/acceptance and commitment therapy model for adolescent substance use treatment. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6, 375379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truax, C. B., & Carkhuff, R. R. (1967). Toward Effective Counseling and Psychotherapy: Training and Practice. Chicago, USA: Aldine.Google Scholar
UKATT Research Team (2005). Effectiveness of treatment for alcohol problems: findings of the randomized UK alcohol treatment trial (UKATT). British Medical Journal, 331, 541544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, C. C., & Ingersoll, K. S. (2008). Beyond cognition: broadening the emotional base of motivational interviewing. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 18, 191206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldron, H. B., Miller, W. R., & Tonigan, J. S. (2001). Client anger as a predictor of differential response to treatment. In Longabaugh, R. & Wirtz, P. W. (eds), Project MATCH Hypotheses: Results and Causal Chain Analyses (vol. 8, pp. 134148). Bethesda, MD, USA: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.Google Scholar
Walker, D. D., Stephens, R. S., Towe, S., Banes, K., & Roffman, R. (2015). Maintenance check-ups following treatment for cannabis dependence. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 56, 1115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, S. T., Clark, M. D., Gingerich, R., & Meltzer, M. (2007a). Motivating Offenders to Change: A Guide for Probation and Parole. Washington, DC, USA: National Institute of Corrections.Google Scholar
Walters, S. T., Vader, A. M., & Harris, T. R. (2007b). A controlled trial of web-based feedback for heavy drinking college students. Prevention Science, 8, 8388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walthers, J., Janssen, T., Mastroleo, N. R., Hoadley, A., Barnett, N. P., Colby, S. M., & Magill, M. (2019). A sequential analysis of clinician skills and client change statements in a brief motivational intervention for young adult heavy drinking. Behavior Therapy, 50, 732742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, B. E., & Bolt, D. M. (2006). Therapist effects: clever ways to make them (and everything else) disappear. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 184187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. (2015a). The Great Psychotherapy Debate: The Evidence for What Makes Psychotherapy Work (2nd edn). New York, USA: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. (2015b). Therapist effects: an ignored but critical factor. In Wampold, B. E. & Imel, Z. E. (eds), The Great Psychotherapy Debate: The Evidence for What Makes Psychotherapy Work (2nd edn, pp. 158177): Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weger, U. W., Wagemann, J., & Tewes, C. (2019). The challenges and opportunities of introspection in psychology: theory and method. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, P. (2007). Effect of motivational interviewing on rates of early childhood caries: a randomized trial. Pediatric Dentistry, 29, 1622.Google Scholar
Westra, H. A., Constantino, M. J., & Antony, M. M. (2016). Integrating motivational interviewing with cognitive-behavioral therapy for severe generalized anxiety disorder: an allegiance-controlled randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 84, 768782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westra, H. A., Norouzian, N., Poulin, L., Coyne, A., Constantino, M. J., Hara, K., Olson, D., & Antony, M. M. (2021). Testing a deliberate practice workshop for developing appropriate responsivity to resistance markers. Psychotherapy, 58, 175185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, J., Kersh, B. C., & Jenkins, E. A. (2017). Motivational Interviewing for Leadership: MI-LEAD: Gray Beach Publishing.Google Scholar
Witkiewitz, K., Hartzler, B., & Donovan, D. (2010). Matching motivation enhancement treatment to client motivation: re-examining the Project MATCH motivation matching hypothesis. Addiction, 105, 14031413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Worthington, E. L. Jr (2003). Forgiving and Reconciling: Bridges to Wholeness and Hope. Downers Grove, IL, USA: InterVarsity Press.Google Scholar
Worthington, E. L. Jr (ed) (2005). Handbook of Forgiveness. New York, USA: Routledge.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Parallels of motivational interviewing with characteristics of more effective psychotherapists

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.