Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T19:11:36.402Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attentional Bias for Appetitive Cues: Effects of Fasting in Normal Subjects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2009

Edith H. Lavy
Affiliation:
University of Limburg, the Netherlands
Marcel A. van den Hout
Affiliation:
University of Limburg, the Netherlands

Abstract

A long series of earlier studies demonstrated that fearful subjects selectively allocate attention to sources of perceived threat. Such attentional bias may not be limited to negative cues. It is hypothesized that attentional bias serves to enhance early identification of action-relevant cues and that such bias also occurs when subjects are confronted with positive valenced cues that evoke an urge to act immediately. In order to determine whether or not the attentional bias effect is limited to unpleasant stimuli, we studied the effects of 24 hours of fasting, focusing our attention on a possible attentional bias for (positively evaluated) food stimuli. The experiment controlled for emotionality effects. It is shown that fasting results in a positive evaluation of food stimuli and an increased desire for these stimuli. The attentional bias effects found were slight and more likely attributable to the increased urge to act than to general emotionality effects. The power of the experimental manipulation and the computerized Stroop task are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bentall, R. and Thompson, M. (1990). Emotional Stroop performance and the manic defence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 29, 235237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Channon, S. and Hayward, A. (1990). The effect of short-term fasting on processing of food cues in normal subjects. International Journal of Eating Disorders 9, 447452.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Channon, S., Hemsley, D. and De Silva, P. (1988). Selective processing of food words in anorexia nervosa. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 27, 259260.Google Scholar
Cook, J., Jones, N. and Johnston, D. (1989). The effects of imminent minor surgery on the cognitive processing of health and interpersonal threat. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 28, 281282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, K. and Furnham, A. (1989). The colour naming of emotional words. British Journal of Psychology 80, 383389.Google Scholar
Ehlers, A., Margraf, J., Davies, S. and Roth, W. (1988). Selective processing of threat cues in subjects with panic attacks. Cognition and Emotion 2, 201219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E.B. and McNally, R.J. (1986). Sensitivity to feared stimuli in obsessive-compulsives: a dichotic listening analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research 10, 477485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N.H. (1986). The Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Giles, M. and Cairns, E. (1989). Colour naming of violence-related words in Northern Ireland. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 28, 8788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. and McCann, C. (1984). Construct accessibility and depression: an examination of cognitive and affective factors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47, 427439.Google Scholar
Kaspi, S. and McNally, R. (1991). Selective processing of idiographic emotional information in PTSD.Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy,New York.Google Scholar
Klein, G.S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with colour-naming. American Journal of Psychology 77, 576588.Google Scholar
Landau, R. (1980). The role of semantic schemata in phobic word interpretation. Cognitive Therapy and Research 4, 427434.Google Scholar
Lavy, E., Van Den Hout, M. and Arntz, A. (1993). Attentional bias and facilitated escape: a pictorial test. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, in press.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. and Hagan, R. (1992). Individual differences in the selective processing of threatening information, and emotional responses to a stressful life event. Behavioural Research and Therapy 30, 151161.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. and Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40, 653670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C. and Mathews, A. (1991). Biased cognitive operations in anxiety: accessibility of information or assignment of processing priorities? Behaviour Research and Therapy 29, 599610.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. and Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95, 1520.Google Scholar
Martin, M., Williams, R. and Clark, D. (1991). Does anxiety lead to selective processing of threat-related information? Behaviour Research and Therapy 29, 147160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, A. (1989). Cognitive aspects of the etiology and phenomenology of anxiety disorders. In Emmelkamp, P., Everaerd, W., Kraaimaat, F. and Berwyn, P.A. Van Son M. (Eds). Fresh Perspectives on Anxiety Disorders. North America: Swets.Google Scholar
Mathews, A. and Eysenck, M. (1987). Clinical anxiety and cognition. In Eysenck, H. and Martin, I. (Eds). Theoretical Foundations of Behavior Therapy. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Mathews, A. and MacLeod, C. (1986). Discrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95, 131138.Google Scholar
McKenna, F. (1986). Effects of unattended emotional stimuli on color-naming performance. Current Psychological Research and Reviews 5, 39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNally, R.J., Riemann, B.C. and Kim, E. (1990). Selective processing of threat cues in panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 407412.Google Scholar
Mogg, M. and Marden, B. (1990). Processing of emotional information in anxious subjects. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 29, 227229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mogg, K., Mathews, A., Eysenck, M. and May, J. (1991). Biased cognitive operations in anxiety: artefact, processing priorities or attentional search? Behaviour Research and Therapy 29, 459467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mogg, K., Mathews, A., May, J., Grove, M., Eysenck, M. and Weinman, J. (1991). Assessment of cognitive bias in anxiety and depression using a colour perception task. Cognition and Emotion 5, 221238.Google Scholar
Mogg, K., Mathews, A. and Weinman, J. (1987). Memory bias in clinical anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 96, 9498.Google Scholar
Mogg, K., Mathews, A. and Weinman, J. (1989). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: a replication. Behaviour Research and Therapy 27, 317323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moray, N. (1959). Attention and dichotic listening: effect of cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 11, 5660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polivy, J., Herman, C.P. and Warsch, S. (1978). Internal and external components of emotionality in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 87, 197204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schotte, D., McNally, R. and Turner, M. (1990). A dichotic listening analysis of body weight concern in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders 9, 109113.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643662.Google Scholar
Van Strien, T., Frijters, J.E.R., Bergers, G.P.A. and Defares, P.B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restraint, emotional and external eating behaviour. International Journal of Eating Disorders 2, 295315.Google Scholar
Watts, F. and Dalgleish, T. (1991). Memory for phobia-related words in spider phobics. Cognition and Emotion 5, 313329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, F., Trezise, L. and Sharrock, R. (1986). Processing of phobic stimuli. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 25, 253259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. and Broadbent, K. (1986). Distraction by emotional stimuli: use of a Stroop task with suicide attempters. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 25, 101110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J., Watts, F., MacLeod, C. and Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.